Observations from Practical Experiences in the Field
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 June 2014
INTRODUCTION
Although tradition and experience often provide the only guidance for criminal justice practitioners, there is a growing consensus among scholars, practitioners, and policy makers that crime prevention practices and policies should be rooted as much as possible in scientific evidence about “what works” (Cullen and Gendreau 2000; MacKenzie 2000; Sherman 1998; Sherman et al. 2006). “Evidence-based crime prevention” is part of a larger and increasingly expanding movement in social policy to use scientific research evidence to guide program development and implementation. In general terms, this movement is dedicated to the improvement of society through the utilization of the highest quality scientific evidence on what works best (see, e.g., Sherman et al. 2006).
In an evidence-based model, the source of scientific evidence is empirical research in the form of evaluations of programs, practices, and policies. Common evaluation designs include randomized experiments, quasi-experiments, and nonexperimental (sometimes called observational) research designs. Not all evaluation designs are considered equal, however. Some evaluation designs, namely randomized controlled experiments, are considered more scientifically valid than others (Campbell and Stanley 1966; Cook and Campbell 1979; Weisburd 2003). The findings of stronger evaluation designs are privileged over the findings of weaker research designs in determining “what works” in crime and justice interventions. For instance, in their report to the United States Congress on what works in preventing crime, University of Maryland researchers developed the Maryland Scientific Methods Scale to indicate to scholars, practitioners, and policymakers that studies evaluating criminological interventions may differ in terms of methodological quality of evaluation techniques (Sherman et al. 1997).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.