Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-7jkgd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T09:29:54.698Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - From the Protolanguage Spectrum to the Underlying Bases of Language

from Part I - The Evolution of Syntax

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2025

Dany Adone
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Astrid Gabel
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Get access

Summary

Arbib’s chapter places the old debate over whether the protowords of protolanguage may often be holophrases or are more akin to words of current languages within the context of Bickerton’s changing views on the emergence of languages from protolanguages. He traces Bickerton’s ideas from the Universal Grammar with a default parameters approach of the Language Bioprogram Hypothesis via the “just add Merge” account of Adam’s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans to the replacement of Universal Grammar by a notion of Universal Bases of Language in More than Nature Needs: Language, Mind, and Evolution. As a counterpoise, Arbib considers the Mirror System Hypothesis of the evolution of the language-ready brain in which, starting from protowords, words and constructions akin to those of modern languages emerged via cultural evolution with fractionation of holophrases playing a crucial (but not the only) initial role.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolution, Acquisition and Development of Syntax
Insights from Creole Languages and Beyond
, pp. 11 - 33
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aboitiz, F., and García, R. (2009). Merging of phonological and gestural circuits in early language evolution. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 20(1), 7184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2009.20.1.71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 105167. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x05000038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A. (2008). Holophrasis and the protolanguage spectrum. Interaction Studies: Social Behavior and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 9(1), 151165.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2024). Pantomime within and beyond the evolution of language. In Zywiczynski, P., Blomberg, J., and Boruta-Żywiczyńska, M., (eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction 16–57. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., and Bickerton, D. (eds.). (2010). The Emergence of Protolanguage: Holophrasis vs compositionality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., and Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393424.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (2001). The Atoms of Language: The Mind’s Hidden Rules of Grammar. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bakker, P. (2019). Intentional language change and the connection between mixed languages and genderlects. Language Dynamics and Change, 9(2), 135161. https://doi.org/10.1163/22105832-00902001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrès, V. (2017). Schema Architecture for Language-Vision InterActions: A Computational Cognitive Neuroscience Model of Language Use. Los Angeles: University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., and Chomsky, N. (2015). Why Only Us: Language and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 173221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and Human Behavior. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2008). But how did protolanguage actually start? Interaction Studies, 9(1), 169176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam’s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill & Wang.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2014). More than Nature Needs: Language, Mind, and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/doi:10.4159/9780674728523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, J. (1988). Yanyuwa: “Men speak one way, women speak another.” In Coates, J., (ed.), Language and Gender: A Reader. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, pp. 1320.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., and Lasnik, H. (1993). The Theory of Principles and Parameters. In Jacobs, J., van Stechow, A., Sternefeld, W., and Vennemann, T. (eds.), Syntax: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research, Vol. I. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 506556 (repr. in N. Chomsky (1995), The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2018). Mental travels and the cognitive basis of language. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 352369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Arbib, M., and Garrod, S. (2013). How to bootstrap a human communication system. Cognitive Science, 37(7), 13561367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, N., Lister, C. J., Ellison, T. M., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Creating a communication system from scratch: Gesture beats vocalization hands down. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 354. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4010783/pdf/fpsyg-05-00354.pdfCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, C. J. (1966). The Case for Case. In Bach, E. and Harms, R. T. (eds.), Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 188.Google Scholar
Fleming, L. (2012). Gender indexicality in the Native Americas: Contributions to the typology of social indexicality. Language in Society, 41(3), 295320. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404512000267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fouts, R. S. (1974). Capacities for language in great apes. In Tuttle, R. H. (ed.), Socioecology and Psychology of Primates. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 371390.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P., and Högberg, A. (2017). The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58(2), 188208. https://doi.org/10.1086/691178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, J. C. (1983). A computational model of language acquisition in the two-year-old. Cognition and Brain Theory, 6, 287317.Google Scholar
Hinzen, W. (2006). Mind Design and Minimal Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemmerer, D. (2005). Against innate grammatical categories. Behavioral Brain Sciences, www.bbsonline.org/Preprints/Arbib-05012002/Supplemental/Google Scholar
Kirton, J. F. (1988). Men’s and women’s dialects. Aboriginal Linguistics, 1, 111125.Google Scholar
Lee, J. Y. (2012). Linking eyes to mouth: A schema-based computational model for describing visual scenes Ph.D. dissertation, Computer Science, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language development. In Kroll, J. F. and Groot, A. M. B. D. (eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4967.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Kempe, V., Li, P., and Brooks, P. J. (eds.). (2022). Emergentist Approaches to Language. Frontiers Media SA. https://doi.org/doi:10.3389/978-2-88974-483-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Grady, W. (2022). Natural Syntax: An Emergentist Primer. University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., and Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Polich, L. (2005). The Emergence of the Deaf Community in Nicaragua: “With Sign Language You Can Learn So Much.” Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. K., & Scholz, B. C. (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review, 19, 950.Google Scholar
Rose, F. (2015). On male and female speech and more: Categorical gender indexicality in indigenous South American languages. International Journal of American Linguistics, 81(4), 495537. https://doi.org/10.1086/683158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M., and Liebal, K. (2011). The language void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 919924. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347211000558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 82(3), 171177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R., and Wixted, J. T. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34(1), 259288. https://doi.org/doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supalla, T., and Newport, E. L. (1978). How many seats in a chair? The derivation of nouns and verbs in American Sign Language. In Siple, P. (ed.), Understanding Language through Sign Language Research. New York: Academic Press, pp. 91159.Google Scholar
Stout, D., and Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367, 7587. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrace, H., Petitto, L., Sanders, R., and Bever, T. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206, 891902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2019). Cultural evolution in animals. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 50(1), 2748. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110218-025040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language & Communication, 18, 4767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, A. (2000). Holistic utterances in protolanguage: The link from primates to humans. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. (eds.), The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 285302. doi:https://10.1017/CBO9780511606441.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×