Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T17:09:51.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Psychology of judgment and choice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2014

M. G. Myriam Hunink
Affiliation:
Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam
Milton C. Weinstein
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Eve Wittenberg
Affiliation:
Harvard School of Public Health, Massachusetts
Michael F. Drummond
Affiliation:
University of York
Joseph S. Pliskin
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
John B. Wong
Affiliation:
Tufts University, Massachusetts
Paul P. Glasziou
Affiliation:
Bond University, Queensland
Get access

Summary

We are pawns in a game whose forces we largely fail to understand.

Dan Ariely

Introduction

This book has discussed a host of methods and approaches to analyzing decisions, to predicting alternate and optimal outcomes, and to applying quantitative and analytic approaches to understand and explain choices. Most of the chapters up until now have emphasized technical methods and are likely to have challenged and expanded your analytic skills. For this final chapter we will change gears to discuss the side of the brain that is commonly thought of as less analytical and more creative, the ‘right brain’ contribution to decision making. In this concluding chapter we will discuss the psychology underlying judgment and choice.

Think back for a moment to the example in Chapter 4 about genetic susceptibility for breast cancer. Here was the situation we were considering:

Genetic susceptibility for breast cancer

A 25-year-old woman has a strong family history of breast cancer, including a sister who developed the disease at age 35. Her sister has undergone genetic testing for cancer predisposition and has been found to carry a mutation in the BRCA1 breast cancer gene. The woman is concerned about her own risk of breast cancer and chooses to be tested. She is found to have the same mutation, and is told that her lifetime risk of developing breast cancer is approximately 65%. If she does nothing at all with this information, her chance of surviving to age 70 is 53% (compared with all women’s survival probability of 84%). She has a number of options open to her: (1) careful surveillance, including regular mammography and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), which would increase her chance of surviving to age 70 to 59%; (2) prophylactic mastectomy – surgical removal of both breasts – which would increase her chance of surviving to age 70 to 66%; (3) prophylactic mastectomy now plus prophylactic oophorectomy – surgical removal of the ovaries – when she turns 40, which would increase her chance of surviving to age 70 to 79%. Both surgical options increase her chance of survival beyond that of surveillance, but carry some personal costs – mastectomy can affect sexual function and body image, oophorectomy causes early-onset menopause and prevents child bearing. Does the benefit of risk reduction with surgery outweigh the personal costs of these interventions?

Type
Chapter
Information
Decision Making in Health and Medicine
Integrating Evidence and Values
, pp. 392 - 413
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Tversky, A, Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211:453–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D, Tversky, A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47(2):263–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D, Slovic, P, Tversky, A, eds. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1982.
Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2011.Google Scholar
McFadden, D. Rationality for economists?J Risk Uncertainty. 1999;19(1–3):73–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P, Fischhoff, B, Lichtenstein, S. Characterizing perceived risk. In: Kates R, Hohenemser C, Kasperson R, eds. Perilous Progress: Managing the Hazards of Technology. Boulder, CO: Westview; 1985.
Ariely, D. Predictably Irrational. New York: HarperCollins Publishers; 2008.Google Scholar
Katapodi, MC, Lee, KA, Facione, NC, Dodd, MJ. Predictors of perceived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: a meta-analytic review. Prev Med. 2004;38(4):388–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A, Kahneman, D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185:1124–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avis, NE, Smith, KW, McKinlay, JB. Accuracy of perceptions of heart attack risk: what influences perceptions and can they be changed?Am J Public Health. 1989;79(12):1608–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cull, A, Anderson, ED, Campbell, S, et al. The impact of genetic counselling about breast cancer risk on women’s risk perceptions and levels of distress. Br J Cancer. 1999;79(3–4):501–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurmankin, AD, Domchek, S, Stopfer, J, Fels, C, Armstrong, K. Patients’ resistance to risk information in genetic counseling for BRCA1/2. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(5):523–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassirer, J, Wong, J, Kopelman, R. Learning Clinical Reasoning. 2nd edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins; 2010.
Brewer, NT, Chapman, GB, Schwartz, JA, Bergus, GR. The influence of irrelevant anchors on the judgments and choices of doctors and patients. Med Decis Making. 2007;27(2):203–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earle, CC, Landrum, MB, Souza, JM, et al. Aggressiveness of cancer care near the end of life: is it a quality-of-care issue?J Clin Oncol. 2008;26(23):3860–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmboe, ES, Concato, J. Treatment decisions for localized prostate cancer: asking men what’s important. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15(10):694–701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, J, Dailey, RK, Eggly, S, Neale, AV, Schwartz, KL. Men’s perspectives on selecting their prostate cancer treatment. J Natl Med Assoc. 2011;103(6):468–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, I, Dent, S, Peters, E, Slovic, P, Starmer, C. The affect heuristic and the attractiveness of simple gambles. J Behav Decis Making. 2007;20(4):365–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, B, Pauker, S, Sox, H, Tversky, A. On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. N Engl J Med. 1982;306:1259–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D, Knetsch, J, Thaler, R. The endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. J Econ Perspect. 1991;5(1):193–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D, Knetsch, J, Thaler, R. Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase theorem. J Polit Econ. 1990;98(6):1325–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bansal-Travers, M, Hammond, D, Smith, P, Cummings, KM. The impact of cigarette pack design, descriptors, and warning labels on risk perception in the U.S. Am J Prev Med. 2011;40(6):674–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nonnemaker, J, Farrelly, M, Kamyab, K, Busey, A, Ann, N. Experimental Study of Graphic Cigarette Warning Labels. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI International; 2010.Google Scholar
Allais, M. Le comportement de l’homme rationnel devant le risque: critique des postulats et axiomes de l’école Américaine. Econometrica. 1953;21(4):503–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loomes, G, Sugden, R. Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. Econ J. 1982;92(4):805–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×