Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:47:03.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Culture, Self, and Agency

An Ecosocial View

from Section 2 - The Situated Brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Laurence J. Kirmayer
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Shinobu Kitayama
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Robert Lemelson
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
The Foundation for Psychocultural Research
Get access

Summary

Agency refers to the human capacity to choose, initiate, and control actions to influence events in the world. The experience of agency is fundamental to our sense of self but may be altered in certain neurological conditions and forms of psychopathology. In this chapter, we review recent work in cognitive neuroscience that shows how agency depends on sensorimotor loops between the body and the environment as well as higher-order processes of attribution and interpretation. The sensorimotor loops that contribute to the sense of agency and ownership of the body and its actions can be manipulated in laboratory experiments to give rise to startling illusions like being out of one’s body, having a rubber hand, or controlling random events. Religious experiences like spirit possession and dissociative symptoms like conversion disorder may depend on attributing action to agencies other than the self. Even everyday actions depend on interpretive processes that draw from cultural affordances and ontologies as well as social and political structures. Embodied experience grounds to the sense of agency, but culturally mediated attributions can alter bodily experience. Far from being simply a consequence of individual cognition or ability, therefore, agency is rooted in ongoing engagement with the social–cultural world.

Type
Chapter
Information
Culture, Mind, and Brain
Emerging Concepts, Models, and Applications
, pp. 244 - 272
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azuma, H. (1984). Secondary control as a heterogeneous category. American Psychologist, 39(9): 970–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.970Google Scholar
Bart, V. K. E., Sharavdorj, E., Bazarvaani, K., Munkhbat, T., Wenke, D., & Rieger, M. (2019). It was me: The use of sense of agency cues differs between culturesFrontiers in Psychology10, 650. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00650Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Free will in scientific psychologyPerspectives on Psychological Science3(1), 1419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00057.xGoogle Scholar
Beyer, F., Sidarus, N., Bonicalzi, S., & Haggard, P. (2017). Beyond self-serving bias: Diffusion of responsibility reduces sense of agency and outcome monitoringSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience12(1), 138–45. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw160Google Scholar
Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 556–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3292Google Scholar
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669), 756. https://doi.org/10.1038/35784Google Scholar
Brass, M., Lynn, M. T., Demanet, J., & Rigoni, D. (2013). Imaging volition: What the brain can tell us about the willExperimental Brain Research229(3), 301–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3472-xGoogle Scholar
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (2013). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Buhrmann, T., & Di Paolo, E. (2017). The sense of agency – A phenomenological consequence of enacting sensorimotor schemesPhenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences16(2), 207–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-015-9446–7Google Scholar
Chambon, V., Sidarus, N., & Haggard, P. (2014). From action intentions to action effects: How does the sense of agency come about? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience8, 320. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00320Google Scholar
Chirkov, V. I., Ryan, R. M., & Sheldon, K. M. (Eds.). (2011). Human autonomy in cross-cultural context: Perspectives on the psychology of agency, freedom, and people’s well-being. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9667-8Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Di Costa, S., Beck, B., & Haggard, P. (2019). I just lost it! Fear and anger reduce the sense of agency: A study using intentional bindingExperimental Brain Research, 237(5), 1205–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5461-6Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
David, N. (2012). New frontiers in the neuroscience of the sense of agencyFrontiers in Human Neuroscience6, 161. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00161Google Scholar
David, N., Newen, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008). The “sense of agency” and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanismsConsciousness and Cognition17(2), 523–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.004Google Scholar
Deeley, Q., Oakley, D. A., Walsh, E., Bell, V., Mehta, M. A., & Halligan, P. W. (2014). Modelling psychiatric and cultural possession phenomena with suggestion and fMRICortex53, 107–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.004Google Scholar
Dogge, M., Schaap, M., Custers, R., Wegner, D. M., & Aarts, H. (2012). When moving without volition: Implied self-causation enhances binding strength between involuntary actions and effectsConsciousness and Cognition21(1), 501–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.10.014Google Scholar
Edge, H., & Suryani, L. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of volition. Florida Philosophical Review, 2 (2), 5672.Google Scholar
Fallin, M., Whooley, O., & Barker, K. K. (2018). Criminalizing the brain: Neurocriminology and the production of strategic ignorance. BioSocieties, 1–25. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41292-018-0135-yGoogle Scholar
Fausey, C. M., Long, B. L., Inamori, A., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Constructing agency: The role of language. Frontiers of Psychology, 1, 162. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00162Google Scholar
Feldman, G. (2017). Making sense of agency: Belief in free will as a unique and important constructSocial and Personality Psychology Compass11(1), e12293. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12293Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2014). Action, agency and responsibilityNeuropsychologia55, 137–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.007Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198794325.001.0001Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-controlPersonality and Social Psychology Review11(4), 303–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307303030Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1979). Central problems in social theory: Actions, structure and contradiction in social analysis. Palgrave. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-16161-4_3Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Franco, M., & Lanier, J. (2017). Model of illusions and virtual reality. Frontiers in Psychology8, 1125. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01125Google Scholar
Greely, H. T., & Farahany, N. A. (2019). Neuroscience and the criminal justice systemAnnual Review of Criminology2, 451–71. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-criminol-011518-024433Google Scholar
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377–96. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000093Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brainNature Reviews Neuroscience18(4), 196207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.14Google Scholar
Haggard, P., & Eitam, B. (Eds.). (2015). The sense of agency. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190267278.001.0001Google Scholar
Hilgard, E. R. (1986). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action (Expanded ed.). Wiley.Google Scholar
Ismail, M. A. F., & Shimada, S. (2016). ‘Robot’ hand illusion under delayed visual feedback: Relationship between the senses of ownership and agencyPLoS ONE11(7), e0159619. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159619Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion – Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulationPsychological Science21(11), 1686–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610384745Google Scholar
Johansson, P., Hall, L., Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision taskScience310(5745), 116–19. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1111709Google Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/2528.001.0001Google Scholar
Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: A dissociation of ownership and agencyFrontiers in Human Neuroscience6, 40. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J. (1990). Resistance, reactance, and reluctance to change: A cognitive attributional approach to strategic interventionsJournal of Cognitive Psychotherapy4(2), 83104.Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., & Gómez-Carrillo, A. (2019). Agency, embodiment and enactment in psychosomatic theory and practiceMedical Humanities, 45(2), 169–82. https://doi.org/10.1136/medhum-2018-011618Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2017). Embodiment and enactment in cultural psychiatry. In Durt, C., Fuchs, T., & Tewes, C. (Eds.), Embodiment, enaction, and culture: Investigating the constitution of the shared world (pp. 397422). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262035552.003.0021Google Scholar
Kokkinara, E., Kilteni, K., Blom, K. J., & Slater, M. (2016). First person perspective of seated participants over a walking virtual body leads to illusory agency over the walkingScientific Reports6, 28879. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep28879Google Scholar
Kpanake, L. (2018). Cultural concepts of the person and mental health in Africa. Transcultural Psychiatry55(2), 198218. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461517749435Google Scholar
Kushnir, T. (2018). The developmental and cultural psychology of free willPhilosophy Compass13(11), e12529. https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12529Google Scholar
Lee, B. O. (2016). Transformation in dang-ki healing: The embodied self and perceived legitimacyCulture, Medicine, and Psychiatry40(3), 422–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-016-9497-4Google Scholar
Lee, B.-O., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2019). Dang-ki healing: An embodied relational healing practice in Singapore. Transcultural Psychiatry. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1363461519858448Google Scholar
Lee, B. O., Kirmayer, L. J., & Groleau, D. (2010). Therapeutic processes and perceived helpfulness of dang-ki (Chinese shamanism) from the symbolic healing perspectiveCulture, Medicine, and Psychiatry34(1), 56105.Google Scholar
Lenggengager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, O. (2007). Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science, 317(5841), 1096–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1143439Google Scholar
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): The unconscious initiation of freely voluntary act. Brain, 106(3), 623–42. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623Google Scholar
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., & Weekes, J. R. (1990). Hypnotic involuntariness: A social cognitive analysis. Psychological Review, 97(2), 169–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.169Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitutionPerspectives on Psychological Science5(4), 420–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375557Google Scholar
Maurer, C. W., LaFaver, K., Ameli, R., Epstein, S. A., Hallett, M., & Horovitz, S. G. (2016). Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: A resting-state fMRI studyNeurology87(6), 564–70. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002940Google Scholar
Miller, G. (2016, March 1). The brain gets its day in court. The Atlantic. www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/03/neurolaw-brain-scans-court/471615Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt, Reinhart, Winston. https://doi.org/10.1037/10039-000Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Koven, M., & Lin, S. (2011). Language socialization and narrative. In Durant, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), The handbook of language socialization (pp. 190208). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901.ch8Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., Berman, M. G., Casey, B. J., Gotlib, I. H., Jonides, J., Kross, E., Teslovich, T., Wilson, N. L., Zayas, V., & Shoda, Y. (2010). ‘Willpower’ over the life span: Decomposing self-regulationSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience6(2), 252–6. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq081Google Scholar
Moore, J. W. (2016). What is the sense of agency and why does it matter? Frontiers in Psychology7, 1272. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01272Google Scholar
Moretto, G., Walsh, E., & Haggard, P. (2011). Experience of agency and sense of responsibilityConsciousness and Cognition20(4), 1847–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.014Google Scholar
Nahab, F. B., Kundu, P., Maurer, C., Shen, Q., & Hallett, M. (2017). Impaired sense of agency in functional movement disorders: An fMRI studyPLoS ONE12(4), e0172502. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172502Google Scholar
Oerter, R., Oerter, R., Agostiani, H., Kim, H. O., & Wibowo, S. (1996). The concept of human nature in East Asia: Etic and emic characteristicsCulture & Psychology2(1), 951. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X9621002Google Scholar
Papies, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2016). Automatic self-regulation: From habit to goal pursuit. In Vohs, K. & Baumeister, R.F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed., pp. 203–22). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., Woody, E. Z., & Connors, M. H. (2014). Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosisPsychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000010Google Scholar
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Aldine.Google Scholar
Raine, A. (2019). A neurodevelopmental perspective on male violence. Infant Mental Health 40(1), 8497. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21761Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J., Veissière, S. P., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attentionFrontiers in Psychology7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Rose, N. (1998). Inventing our selves: Psychology, power, and personhood. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Saigle, V., Dubljević, V., & Racine, E. (2018). The impact of a landmark neuroscience study on free will: A qualitative analysis of articles using Libet and colleagues’ methodsAJOB Neuroscience9(1), 2941. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2018.1425756Google Scholar
Seligman, R., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2008). Dissociative experience and cultural neuroscience: Narrative, metaphor and mechanismCulture, Medicine and Psychiatry32(1), 3164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-007-9077-8Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395Google Scholar
Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(2), 214–20. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Fneuro.01.029.2009Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agencyConsciousness and Cognition17(1), 219–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.010Google Scholar
Tilly, C. (2006). Why? What happens when people give reasons … and why. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Veissière, S. P., Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J., Friston, K. J., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2020). Thinking through other minds: A variational approach to cognition and cultureBehavioral and Brain Sciences43, e90: 175. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X19001213Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. (2003). The mind’s best trick: How we experience conscious will. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(2), 65–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613%2803%2900002-0Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Gilbert, D., & Wheatley, T. (2017). The illusion of conscious will. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/11151.001.0001Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and JapanAmerican Psychologist39(9), 955–69. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.955Google Scholar
Wikan, U. (1990). Managing turbulent hearts: A Balinese formula for living. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×