Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T09:32:52.931Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Cultural Transmission: A View from Chimpanzees and Human Infants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ute Schönpflug
Affiliation:
Freie Universität Berlin
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Primates are highly social beings. They begin their lives clinging to their mother and nursing, and they spend their next few months, or even years, still in proximity to her. Adult primates live in close-knit social groups, for the most part, in which members individually recognize one another and form various types of long-term social relationships (Tomasello & Call, 1994, 1997). As primates, human beings follow this same pattern, of course, but they also have unique forms of sociality that may be characterized as “ultrasocial” or, in more common parlance, “cultural” (Tomasello, Krüger, & Ratner, 1993). The forms of sociality that are mostly clearly unique to human beings emerge in their ontogeny at approximately 9 months of age – what I have called the 9-month social-cognitive revolution (Tomasello, 1995). This is the age at which infants typically begin to engage in the kinds of joint-attentional interactions in which they master the use of cultural artifacts, including tools and language, and become fully active participants in all types of cultural rituals, scripts, and games. In this chapter, my goals are to (1) characterize the primate and human forms of sociality and cultural transmission, and (2) characterize in more detail the ontogeny of human cultural propensities.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cultural Transmission
Psychological, Developmental, Social, and Methodological Aspects
, pp. 33 - 47
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 79–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interactions. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 211–245). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human culture. Contemporary Anthropology, 39, 591–604.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1996). Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 77–93.Google Scholar
Brown, P. (2000). The conversational context for language acquisition: A Tzeltal (Mayan) case study. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 133–154). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Sixteen-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(255), 1–174.Google Scholar
Galef, B. (1992). The question of animal culture. Human Nature, 3, 157–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E., & Rader, N. (1979). Attention: The perceiver as performer. In Hale, G. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), Attention and cognitive development (pp. 6–36). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 45, 450–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kawai, M. (1965). Newly acquired precultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates, 6, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krüger, A., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Cultural learning and learning culture. In Olson, D. (Ed.), Handbook of education and human development: New models of teaching, learning, and schooling (pp. 169–187). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kummer, H., & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behavior in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 308, 203–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1988). Infant imitation after a one-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24, 470–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & Dunham, P. (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (1990). Language as a cause-effect communication system. Philosophical Psychology, 3, 55–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. & Galef, B. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1994). Social cognition of monkeys and apes. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 37, 273–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J., Frost, T., Carpenter, M., & Nagell, K. (1997). The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: A comparison across groups and generations. Evolution of Communication, 1, 223–253.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Krüger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Krüger, A. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 1688–1705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Strosberg, R., & Akhtar, N. (1996). Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 23, 157–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visalberghi, E., & Fragaszy, D. M. (1990). Food-washing behaviour in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, and crab-eating macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Animal Behaviour, 40, 829–836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (Cole, M., Ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., & Custance, D. (1996). Studies of imitation in chimpanzees and children. In Heyes, C. & Galef, B. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 78–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C., Wrangham, R., & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In Slater, P. J. B., Rosenblatt, J. S., Beer, C., & Milinsky, M. (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (pp. 239–283). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×