Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T12:23:14.529Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Physiology and psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

The nineteenth century was a time of great change in Europe and America. Agricultural reforms ensured a steady food supply, and improvements in public hygiene decreased fatalities from contagious diseases such as cholera. The population of Europe increased from about 140 to 420 million people between 1750 and 1900, with many congregated in the new urban centers. The dramatic expansion of industry led to a general increase in wealth, although the insecurities of the capitalist state (with periods of boom followed by periods of economic downturn) led some to question a system in which most of the wealth was owned by a privileged few and to look to alternative political systems such as socialism and communism. New developments in transportation and communication saw the spread of modern road networks, railways, canals, ocean lines, and telegraph and postal systems (Jansz, 2004).

The nineteenth century witnessed the growth and increasing political strength of the middle class, whose long struggle to attain voting rights eventually bore fruit, although throughout most of the nineteenth century real political control remained in the hands of the conservative aristocracy. In the reactionary period following the Napoleonic wars in Europe, which ended with Napoleon's defeat at the battle of Waterloo in 1815, naturalistic approaches to psychology were repressed through censorship and the secret police. Nobody who promoted such views could hold a professorship in Europe and America in the early half of the century, and in the years immediately following 1815, advocacy of such views was punishable by imprisonment in some parts of Europe (Reed, 1997).

As noted earlier, Joseph Priestley (1733–1804) was hounded out of Britain for his promotion of Hartley's associationist psychology as materialist psychology. Erasmus Darwin (1731–1802), the grandfather of Charles Darwin (1809–1882), who developed an early naturalistic evolutionary theory in Zoonomia (1794–1796), found his work suppressed. One of Darwin's followers, the British surgeon William Lawrence (1783–1867), published his theory that insanity is a neurophysiological disorder in Lectures on Physiology, Zoology and the Natural History of Man (1819). The medical establishment forced him to withdraw his book, and he lost his lectureship at the Royal College of Surgeons (Reed, 1997).

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 139 - 184
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avenarius, R. (1888–1890). Kritik der reinen Erfahrung [Critique of pure experience]. (Vols. 1–2.) Leipzig: Fues (R. Reisland).Google Scholar
Bailey, S. (1842). Review of Berkeley's Theory of Vision: Designed to Show the Unsoundness of that Celebrated Speculation. London: Ridgeway.Google Scholar
Bailey, S. (1843). Letter to a Philosopher in Reply to some Recent Attempts to Vindicate Berkeley's Theory of Vision. London: Ridgeway.Google Scholar
Bailey, S. (1855–1863). Letters on the Philosophy of the Human Mind. (Vols. 1–3.) London: Longmans, Brown, Green, Longmans.Google Scholar
Bain, A. (1855). The Senses and the Intellect. London: Parker.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bain, A. (1859). The Emotions and the Will. London: Parker.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bain, A. (1861). On the Study of Character, Including an Estimate of Phrenology. London: Parker.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bain, A. (1868). Mental Science. New York, NY: Appleton.Google Scholar
Bakan, D. (1966). The influence of phrenology on American psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 2, 200–220.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bantz, D. A. (1980). The structure of discovery: evaluation of structural accounts of chemical bonding. In Nickles, T. (Ed.), Scientific Discovery: Case Studies. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Bartholow, R. (1874). Experimental investigations into the functions of the human brain. American Journal of the Medical Sciences, 67, 305–313.Google Scholar
Berkeley, G. (1975). An essay towards a new theory of vision. In Ayers, M. R. (Ed.), Philosophical Works. Totowa, NJ: Rowman and Littlefield. (Original work published 1709.)Google Scholar
Bianchi, L. (1895). The functions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 18, 497–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviorism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1957). A History of Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Broca, P. P. (1960). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulate language, followed by an observation of aphemia. In Some Papers on the Cerebral Cortex. (von Bonin, G., Trans.) Springfield, IL: Thomas. (Original work published 1861.)Google Scholar
Carpenter, W. B. (1855). Principles of Human Physiology. (edn.) London: Churchill.Google Scholar
Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of Mental Physiology with their Applications to the Training and Discipline of the Mind and the Study of its Morbid Conditions. (edn.) London: King.Google Scholar
Clarke, E. and Jacyna, L. S. (1987). Nineteenth-Century Origins of Neuroscientific Concepts. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Clifford, W. K. (1874). Body and mind. The Fortnightly Review, 16, 714–736. Reprinted in Stephen, L. and Pollock, F., (Eds.), Lectures and Essays of the Late W. K. Clifford. (Vol II.) London: Macmillan, 1901.Google Scholar
Comte, A. (1830–1842). Cours de philosophie positive. (Vols 1–6.) Paris: Rouen.Google Scholar
Comte, A. (1851–1854). La systeme de politique positive. (Vols 1–4.) Paris: Carilian–Goeury.Google Scholar
Coombe, G. (1828). Essay on the Constitution of Man and its Relation to External Objects. Edinburgh: John Anderson.Google Scholar
Cranefield, P. F. (1957). The organic physics of 1847 and the biophysics of today. Journal of the History of Medicine, 12, 407–423.Google ScholarPubMed
Danziger, K. (1982). Mid-nineteenth-century British psycho-physiology: a neglected chapter in the history of psychology. In Woodward, W. and Ach, S. (Eds.), The Problematic Science: Psychology in Nineteenth Century Thought. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Darwin, E. (1794–1796). Zoonomia: Or, the Laws of Organic Life. (2 vols.) London: J. Johnson.Google Scholar
Dobson, V. and Bruce, D. (1972). The German university and the development of experimental psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 204–207.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dods, J. B. (1850). Electrical Psychology. New York, NY: Fowler and Wells.Google Scholar
du Bois-Reymond, E. (1852). Animal Electricity. (Jones, H. B., Ed.) London: Churchill. (Original work published 1848–1849.)Google Scholar
du Bois-Reymond, E. (1927). Zwei grosse Naturforscher des 19 Jahrhunderts: Ein Briefwechsel zwischen Emil du Bois-Reymond and Karl Ludwig [Two major scientists of the nineteenth century: Emil du Bois-Reymond and Karl Ludwig]. Leipzig: Barth. (Original work published 1842.)Google Scholar
Fechner, G. T. (1966). Elements of Psychophysics. (Adler, H. E., Trans.) New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. (Original work published 1860.)Google Scholar
Ferrier, D. (1873). Experimental researches in cerebral physiology and pathology. West Riding Lunatic Asylum Medical Reports, 3, 30–96.Google Scholar
Ferrier, D. (1886). The Functions of the Brain. (. edn.) London: Smith, Elder. (Original work published 1876.)Google Scholar
Flourens, M-J. P. (1842). Researches expérimentales sur les propriétés et les fonctions du systeme nerveux dans les animaux vertébrés [Experimental research on the properties and functions of the nervous system in vertebrates]. (edn.) Paris: Ballière. (Original work published 1824.)Google Scholar
Flourens, M-J. P. (1843). Examen de la phrénologie [An examination of phrenology]. Paris: Paulin.Google Scholar
Flourens, M-J. P. (1863). De la phrénologie [On phrenology]. Paris: Paulin.Google Scholar
Fritsch, G. and Hitzig, E. (1960). On the electrical excitability of the cerebrum. In von Bonin, G. (Trans.), Some Papers on the Cerebral Cortex. Springfield, IL: Thomas. (Original work published 1870.)Google Scholar
Gall, F. J. and Spurzheim, J. C. (1810–1819). The Anatomy and Physiology of the Nervous System in General and of the Brain in Particular, with Observations on the Possibility of Discovering the Number of Intellectual and Moral Dispositions of Men and Animals through the Configurations of their Heads. (Vols. 1–4.) Boston, MA: Marsh, Capen and Lyon.Google Scholar
Gall, F. J. and Spurzheim, J. C. (1835). On the Functions of the Brain and Each of its Parts. (Lewis, W. Jr., Trans.) (Vols. 1–6.) Boston, MA: Marsh, Capen and Lyon. (Original work published 1822–1825.)Google Scholar
Haller, A.. (1803). Elementa Physiologiae [Elements of Physiology]. (Vols. 1–8.) Troy, NY: Penniman. (Original work published 1757–1765.)Google Scholar
Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech. (Vols. 1–2.) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hearnshaw, L. J. (1964). A Brief History of British Psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H.. (1855). Ueber das Sehen des Menschen [About Seeing in Humans]. Leipzig: Voss.Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H.. (1856–1866). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik [Treatise on Physiological Optics]. Hamburg: Voss.Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H.. (1862). Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen [On the Sensation of Tone]. Germany: Vieweg.Google Scholar
Hodgson, S. (1865). Time and Space, a Metaphysical Essay. London: Longman, Green, Longman, Roberts and Green.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1874). On the hypothesis that animals are automata, and its history. The Fortnightly Review, 22, 555–580.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. H. (1931). Selected Writings of Hughlings Jackson. (Taylor, J., Ed.) (Vols. 1–2.) London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
James, W. (1879). Are we automata?Mind, 4, 1–22.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. (Vols. 1–2.) New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Jansz, J. (2004). Psychology and society: an overview. In Jansz, J. and Drunen, P. van (Eds.), A Social History of Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, W. (1819). Lectures on Physiology, Zoology and the Natural History of Man. London: J Callow.Google Scholar
Laycock, T. (1845). On the reflex functions of the brain. British and Foreign Medical Review, 19, 298–311.Google Scholar
Lederer, S. E. (1995). Subjected to Science: Human Experimentation in America Before the Second World War. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Leek, S. (1970). Phrenology. New York, NY: Collier Books.Google Scholar
Lenoir, T. (1994). Helmholtz and the materialities of communication. Osiris, 9, 185–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowry, R. (1982). The Evolution of Psychological Theory. (edn.) Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Mach, E. (1959). The Analysis of Sensations. New York, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1886.)Google Scholar
Magendie, F. (1843). An Elementary Treatise on Human Physiology. (Revere, J., Trans.) (edn.) New York, NY: Harper. (Original work published 1838.)Google Scholar
Magendie, F. (1944). Experiments on the functions of the roots of the spinal nerves. In Olmsted, J. M. D., Francois Magendie – Pioneer in Experimental Method in Medicine in XIX Century France. New York, NY: Schuman. (Original work published 1822.)Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1908). Introduction to Social Psychology. New York, NY: John Luce.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, B. P. (1992). The rise and fall of British emergentism. In Beckermann, A., Flohr, H., and Kim, J. (Eds.), Emergence or Reduction? Prospects for Nonreductive Physicalism. New York, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Mill, J. (1817). A History of British India. (Vols 1–3.) London: Baldwin, Cradock, and Joy.Google Scholar
Mill, J. (1829). Analysis of the Phenomena of the Human Mind. (Vols. 1–2.) London: Baldwin and Craddock.Google Scholar
Mill, J. (1869). Analysis of the Phenomena of the Human Mind. (Mill, J. S., Ed.) (Vols. 1–2.) London: Longmans, Green, Reader, and Dyer.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1859). On Liberty. London: John W. Parker.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1863). Utilitarianism. London: Parker, Son, and Bourn.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1865). Dissertations and Discussions. (Vol. 2.) Boston, MA: Spencer.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1867). Bain's psychology. In Dissertations and Discussions. (Vol. 2.) London: Longmans. (Original work published 1859.)Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1869). The Subjection of Women. London: Longmans, Green, Reader, and Dyer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1961). Auguste Comte and Positivism. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. (Original work published 1866.)Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1973–1974). A System of Logic, Ratiocinative and Inductive; Being a Connected View of the Principles of Evidence, and the Methods of Scientific Investigation. (Robson, J. M., Ed.) Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published 1843.)Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1979). An Examination of Sir William Hamilton's Philosophy and of the Principal Philosophical Questions Discussed in his Writings. (Robson, J. M., Ed.) Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published 1865.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, J. (1838–1842). Handbook of Human Physiology. (Baly, W., Trans.) (Vols. 1–2.) London: Taylor and Walton. (Original work published 1833–1840.)Google Scholar
Neary, F. (2001). A question of “peculiar importance”: George Croom Robertson, mind, and the changing relationship between British psychology and philosophy. In Bunn, G. C., Love, A. D., and Richards, G. D. (Eds.), Psychology in Britain: Historical Essays and Personal Reflections. Leicester: British Psychological Society.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, J. M. (1985). The Origins of Behaviorism: American Psychology, 1870–1920. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Pastore, N. (1965). Samuel Bailey's critique of Berkeley's theory of vision. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 1, 321–337.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pastore, N. (1974). Re-evaluation of Boring on Kantian influence, nineteenth century nativism, Gestalt psychology and Helmholtz. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 10, 375–390.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ray, I. (1838). Treatise on the Medical Jurisprudence of Insanity. Boston, MA: Charles C. Little and James Brown.Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1997). From Soul to Mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Robertson, G. C. (1876). Prefatory words. Mind, 1, 1–6.Google Scholar
Robertson, G. C. (1877). Critical notice of “The Functions of the Brain,” by David Ferrier. Mind, 2, 92–98.Google Scholar
Sechenov, I. (1965). Reflexes of the Brain (Gibbons, G., Ed.; Belsky, S., Trans.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1863.)Google Scholar
Sechenov, I. (1973). Who must investigate the problems of psychology and how? In I. M. Sechenov: Biographical Sketch and Essays. New York, NY: Arno Press. (Original work published 1871.)Google Scholar
Sizer, N. (1882). Forty Years in Phrenology: Embracing Recollections of History, Anecdote and Experience. New York, NY: Fowler and Wells.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, R. (1992). Inhibition: History and Meaning in the Sciences of Mind and Brain. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (2001). Practical phrenology as psychological counseling in the 19th century United States. In Green, C. D., Shore, M., and Teo, T. (Eds.), The Transformation of Psychology: Influences of 19th Century Philosophy, Technology, and Natural Science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Spalding, D. A. (1874). The relation of body to mind. Nature, 178–179.CrossRef
Spencer, H. (1855). Principles of Psychology. London: Longmans.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. (2014). “An Unfit Subject for the Gallows”: Phrenology, Insanity, and Criminal Responsibility in America, 1830–1850. Paper presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of Cheiron, Hood College, Frederick, MD, June.
Turner, R. S. (1977). Hermann von Helmholtz and the empiricist vision. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 13, 48–58.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, R. S. (1982). Helmholtz, sensory physiology, and the disciplinary development of German psychology. In Woodward, W. and Asch, S. (Eds.), The Problematic Science: Psychology in Nineteenth Century Thought. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Walsh, A. A. (1972). The American tour of Dr Spurzheim. Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences, 27, 187–205.Google ScholarPubMed
Wundt, W. (1873–1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of Physiological Psychology]. Leipzig: Englemann.Google Scholar
Young, R. M. (1990). Mind, Brain and Adaptation in the Nineteenth Century. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Physiology and psychology
  • John D. Greenwood, City University of New York
  • Book: A Conceptual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 September 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107414914.007
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Physiology and psychology
  • John D. Greenwood, City University of New York
  • Book: A Conceptual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 September 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107414914.007
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Physiology and psychology
  • John D. Greenwood, City University of New York
  • Book: A Conceptual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 September 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107414914.007
Available formats
×