Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T16:48:04.377Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - A Complex-Adaptive-Systems Approach to the Evolution of Language and the Brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2017

Salikoko S. Mufwene
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Christophe Coupé
Affiliation:
Université Lumière Lyon II
François Pellegrino
Affiliation:
Université Lumière Lyon II
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Complexity in Language
Developmental and Evolutionary Perspectives
, pp. 67 - 100
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alias-i, . 2008. LingPipe 4.1.0. http://alias-i.com/lingpipe.Google Scholar
Baars, Bernard J. & Gage, Nicole M.. 2007. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. 2005. Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends Cogn Sci 9, 309–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2010. Grounded Cognition: Past, Present, and Future. Topics in Cognitive Science 2, 716–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batali, J. 1998. Computational Simulations of the Emergence of Grammar. Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, ed. by Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Knight, C., 405–26. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth & Goodman, Judith C.. 1997. On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Language & Cognitive Processes 12, 507–84.Google Scholar
Beckner, Clay, Blythe, Richard, Bybee, Joan, Christiansen, Morten H., Croft, William, Ellis, Nick C., Holland, John, Jinyun Ke, L. C., Larsen-Freeman, Diane & Schoenemann, Tom. 2009. Language Is a Complex Adaptive System: Position Paper. Language Learning 59, 126.Google Scholar
Bergman, Thore J., Beehner, Jacinta C., Cheney, Dorothy L. & Seyfarth, Robert M.. 2003. Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302, 1234–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berwick, Robert C., Friederici, Angela D., Chomsky, Noam & Bolhuis, Johan J.. 2013. Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, 8998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brodmann, K. 1909. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth Verlag.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1998. A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution. Evolution of Communication 2, 249–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changizi, M. A. & Shimojo, S.. 2005. Parcellation and area-area connectivity as a function of neocortex size. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 66, 8898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M.. 1997. Reconciliatory grunts by dominant female baboons influence victims’ behaviour. Animal Behaviour 54, 409–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, Dorothy L. & Seyfarth, Robert M.. 2005. Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. The Linguistic Review 22, 135–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1972. Language and Mind New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program Cambridge, M A: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Christiansen, Morten H. & Chater, Nick. 2008. Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, 489509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, E., Reichard, U. H. & Zuberbuhler, K.. 2006. The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS ONE 1.e73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, Antonio R. & Damasio, Hanna. 1992. Brain and language. Scientific American 267, 8995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Damasio, A., Tranel, D., Boles-Ponto, L., Watkins, G. L. & Hichwa, R. D.. 1993. Visual recall with eyes closed and covered activates early visual cortices. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 19, 1603.Google Scholar
de Waal, Frans. 1982. Chimpanzee Politics Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, Frans B. M. 2008. Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy. Annual Review of Psychology 59, 279300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, Terrence William. 1997. The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Deacon, Terrence W. 2012. Beyond the Symbolic Species. The Symbolic Species Evolved, ed. by Schilhab, T., Stjernfelt, F. & Deacon, T., 938. Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaner, R. O., Isler, K., Burkart, J. & van Schaik, C.. 2007. Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 70, 115–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Nicholas & Levinson, Stephen C.. 2009. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, 429–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foraker, Stephani, Regier, Terry, Khetarpal, Naveen, Perfors, Amy & Tenenbaum, Joshua. 2009. Indirect Evidence and the Poverty of the Stimulus: The Case of Anaphoric One. Cognitive Science 33, 287300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, R. A. & Gardner, B. T.. 1984. A vocabulary test for chimpanzee (Pan Troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 98, 381404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. & van Cantfort, T. E.. 1989. Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Garstang, Michael 2010. Elephant infrasounds: long-range communication. Handbook of Mammalian Vocalization; An Integrative Neuroscience Approach, ed. by Brudzynski, S. M., 5767. London: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. 2002. Evolution of human intelligence: the roles of brain size and mental construction. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 59, 1020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gil-da-Costa, R., Martin, A., Lopes, M. A., Munoz, M., Fritz, J. B. & Braun, A. R.. 2006. Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the macaque. Nature Neuroscience 9, 1064–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. 1990. The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1, 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila & Gleitman, Henry. 1997. What is a language made out of? Lingua: International Review of General Linguistics 100, 2955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila R., Cassidy, Kimberly, Nappa, Rebecca, Papafragou, Anna & Trueswell, John C.. 2005. Hard words. Language Learning and Development 1, 2364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2004. But do we need universal grammar? Comment on Lidz et al. (2003). Cognition 94, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, Timothy H. & Zimmerman, William F.. 2000. Biology, Evolution, and Human Nature New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Gong, T. 2009. Computational simulation in evolutionary linguistics: a study on language emergence Taipei: Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
Gong, Tao. 2011. Simulating the coevolution of compositionality and word order regularity. Interaction Studies 12, 63106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gong, Tao, Shuai, Lan & Zhang, Menghan. 2014. Modelling language evolution: Examples and predictions. Phys Life Rev 11, 280302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goroll, Nils. 1999. Master's thesis, (The Deep Blue) Nile: Neuronal Influences on Language Evolution. Edinburgh, England: University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia M. & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue. 1991. Imitation, grammatical development, and the invention of protogrammar by an ape. Biological and Behavioral Determinants of Language Development, ed. by Krasnegor, N., Rumbaugh, D. M., Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Studdert-Kennedy, M., 235–58. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gross, C. G. 2002. Genealogy of the “Grandmother Cell.” The Neuroscientist 8, 512–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haiman, John (ed.) 1985. Iconicity in Syntax (Typological Studies in Language). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haug, H. 1987. Brain sizes, surfaces, and neuronal sizes of the cortex cerebri: A stereological investigation of man and his variability and a comparison with some mammals (primates, whales, marsupials, insectivores, and one elephant). American Journal of Anatomy 180, 126–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herculano-Houzel, S. 2012. The remarkable, yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its associated cost. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (Supplement 1), 1066110668.Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. 1979. Acquisition, generalization, and discrimination reversal of a natural concept. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 5, 116–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Holland, John H. 1998. Emergence: From chaos to order. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, John H. 2006. Studying complex adaptive systems. Journal of Systems Science and Complexity 19, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, John. 1995. From complexity to perplexity. Scientific American, 104–09.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, Arthur J. 2010. The evolution of the eye from algae and jellyfish to humans: How vision adapts to environment. Lewiston, NY: Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jerison, H. J. 1973. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jones, G. & Holderied, M. W.. 2007. Bat echolocation calls: adaptation and convergent evolution. Proc Biol Sci 274, 905–12.Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, M. N. & Mewhort, D. J.. 2007. Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psychological Review 114, 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. 1980. Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Alpert, N. M., Thompson, W. L., Maljkovic, V., Weise, S. B., Chabris, C. F., Hamilton, S. E., Rauch, S. L. & Buonanno, F. S.. 1993. Visual mental imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5, 263–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G. & Thompson, W. L.. 2003. Mental imagery: against the nihilistic hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci 7, 109–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krubitzer, Leah. 1995. The organization of neocortex in mammals: are species differences really so different? Trends in Neurosciences 18, 408–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, George & Johnson, Mark. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langton, Chris G. 1990. Computation at the edge of chaos: Phase transitions and emergent computation. Physica D 42, 1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lidz, Jeffrey, Gleitman, Henry & Gleitman, Lila. 2003a. Understanding how input matters: Verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar. Cognition 87, 151–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lidz, Jeffrey & Gleitman, Lila R. 2004a. Yes, we still need Universal Grammar. Cognition 94, 8593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lidz, Jeffrey & Gleitman, Lila R.. 2004b. Argument structure and the child's contribution to language learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 157–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lidz, Jeffrey, Waxman, Sandra & Freedman, Jennifer. 2003b. What infants know about syntax but couldn't have learned: experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months. Cognition 89, 295303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyn, Heidi & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue. 2000. Observational word learning in two bonobos (Pan paniscus): ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Language & Communication 20, 255–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGurk, H. & MacDonald, J.. 1976. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 746–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, A. R. 2000. Towards a network theory of cognition. Neural Networks 13, 861–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulecky, D. C. 2001. The emergence of complexity: science coming of age or science growing old? Computers and Chemistry 25, 341–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, H. Lyn White. 1990. The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives, ed. by Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R., 511–39. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, George A. & Gildea, Patricia M.. 1991. How children learn words. The Emergence of Language: Development and Evolution, ed. by Wang, W. S.-Y., 150–58. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Miller, P. E. & Murphy, C. J.. 1995. Vision in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 207, 1623–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, Melanie, Hraber, Peter T. & Crutchfield, James P.. 1993. Revisiting the Edge of Chaos: Evolving Cellular Automata to Perform Computations. Complex Systems 7, 89130.Google Scholar
Neitz, J., Geist, T. & Jacobs, G. H.. 1989. Color vision in the dog. Visual Neuroscience 3, 119–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northcutt, R. G. & Kaas, J. H.. 1995. The emergence and evolution of mammalian neocortex. Trends in Neurosciences 18, 373–9.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Grady, William. 1987. Principles of Grammar & Learning Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. 1971. Imagery and verbal processes New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Parr, Lisa A. 2011. The evolution of face processing in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366, 1764–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, C. S. 1867. On a new list of categories. Proceedings of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 7, 287–98.Google Scholar
Penfield, Wilder & Rasmussen, Theodore. 1950. Cerebral Cortex of Man, a Clinical Study of Localization of Function. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R.. 2005. The faculty of language: what's special about it? Cognition 95, 201–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven & Bloom, Paul. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13, 707–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F. 2010. Language as a Social Institution: Why Phonemes and Words Do Not Live in the Brain. Ecological Psychology 22, 304–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F. & Leary, Adam P.. 2005. Against Formal Phonology. Language 81, 927–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, Ann James & Premack, David. 1972. Teaching language to an ape. Scientific American 227, 9299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regier, Terry & Gahl, Susanne. 2004. Learning the unlearnable: the role of missing evidence. Cognition 93, 147–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rilling, J. K. & Seligman, R. A.. 2002. A quantitative morphometric comparative analysis of the primate temporal lobe. Journal of Human Evolution 42, 505–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringo, James L. 1991. Neuronal interconnection as a function of brain size. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 38, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, William A. & Mazmanian, Dwight S.. 1988. Concept learning at different levels of abstraction by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 14, 247–60.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue, Murphy, Jeannine, Sevcik, Rose A., Brakke, Karen E., Williams, Shelly L. & Rumbaugh, Duane M.. 1993. Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58, 1222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue & Rumbaugh, Duane M.. 1993. The emergence of language. Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution, ed. by Gibson, K. R. & Ingold, T., 86108. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue, Rumbaugh, Duane & Fields, William M.. 2009. Empirical Kanzi: The Ape Language Controversy Revisited. Skeptic 15, 2533.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. T., Budinger, T. F., Sarich, V. M. & Wang, W. S.. 2000. Brain size does not predict general cognitive ability within families. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97, 4932–7.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas. 1999. Syntax as an emergent characteristic of the evolution of semantic complexity. Minds and Machines 9, 309–46.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas. 2005. Conceptual complexity and the brain: Understanding language origins. Language Acquisition, Change and Emergence: Essays in Evolutionary Linguistics, ed. by Wang, W. S.-Y. & Minett, J. W., 4794. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong Press.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas. 2006. Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain. Annual Review of Anthropology 35, 379406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas. 2010. The meaning of brain size: the evolution of conceptual complexity. Human Brain Evolving: Papers in Honor of Ralph Holloway, ed. by Broadfield, D., Yuan, M., Schick, K. & Toth, N.. Bloomington, IN: Stone Age Institute Press.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas. 2012. Evolution of brain and language. Progress in Brain Research, ed. by Hofman, M. A. & Falk, D., 443–59. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K. & Van Hoesen, G. W.. 2001. Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 114, 224–41.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, Robert M. & Cheney, Dorothy L.. 2000. Social Awareness in Monkeys. American Zoologist 40, 902–09.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M., Cheney, Dorothy L. & Marler, Peter. 1980. Monkey Responses to Three Different Alarm Calls: Evidence of Predator Classification and Semantic Communication. Science 210, 801–03.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepard, R. N. & Cooper, L. A.. 1982. Mental images and their transformations New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shepherd, Gordon M. 2006. Smell images and the flavour system in the human brain. Nature 444, 316–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K., Kirby, S. & Brighton, H.. 2003. Iterated learning: a framework for the emergence of language. Artificial Life 9, 371–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowdon, Charles T. 1990. Language capacities of nonhuman animals. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 33, 215–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spranger, Michael & Steels, Luc. 2012. Emergent functional grammar for space. Experiments in Cultural Language Evolution. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Steels, L. 2011. Modeling the cultural evolution of language. Phys Life Rev 8, 339–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, Heinz, Frahm, Heiko & Baron, Georg. 1981. New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in Insectivores and Primates. Folia Primatologica 35, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, Stephen J. 1997. Nonverbal communication in nonhuman primates: Implications for the emergence of culture. Nonverbal Communication: Where Nature Meets Culture, ed. by Segerstråle, U. C. & Molnár, P., 131–46. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., van Erp, T., Poutanen, V. P., Huttunen, M., Lonnqvist, J., Standertskjold-Nordenstam, C. G., Kaprio, J., Khaledy, M., Dail, R., Zoumalan, C. I. & Toga, A. W.. 2001. Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature Neuroscience 4, 1253–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. 2000. The item-based nature of children's early syntactic development. Trends Cogn Sci 4, 156–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2004. Syntax or semantics? Response to Lidz et al. Cognition 93, 139–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael & Patricia, J. Brooks. 1998. Young children's earliest transitive and intransitive constructions. Cognitive Linguistics 9, 379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G. 2010. Information integration: its relevance to brain function and consciousness. Archives Italiennes de Biologie 148, 299322.Google ScholarPubMed
Wasserman, E. A., Kiedinger, R. E. & Bhatt, R. S.. 1988. Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categories, subcategories, and pseudocategories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 14, 235–46.Google Scholar
Wynne, Clive. 2008. Aping language: A skeptical analysis of the evidence for nonhuman primate language. Skeptic 13, 1014.Google Scholar
Zuberbuhler, K. 2000. Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267, 713–18.Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×