Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T08:43:39.250Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Inhibition, Rumination, and Mood Regulation in Depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2010

Jutta Joormann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bldg. 420 Jordan Hall, Room 160, Stanford University Stanford, CA 94305
Randall W. Engle
Affiliation:
Georgia Institute of Technology
Grzegorz Sedek
Affiliation:
Warsaw School of Social Psychology and Polish Academy of Sciences
Ulrich von Hecker
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Daniel N. McIntosh
Affiliation:
University of Denver
Get access

Summary

The mind seizes hold of something and simply cannot let it go.

– Winston Churchill

Rumination, as described above by Winston Churchill, is a central, debilitating feature of depressive episodes and of other emotional disorders. Rumination means bringing an idea back to mind over and over again. Originally, the word referred to the way cows and certain other animals eat, storing partially digested food in a stomach called a rumen, bringing that food up later to chew over more thoroughly. Even in the original Latin, however, it took on a vivid figurative meaning, describing the practice of bringing an idea back to mind for further work-over. Many of us mull over important matters in this way, digesting them a little at a time. But in depression, rumination becomes less a deliberate practice of thinking but more an involuntary state of mind, featuring negative, self-deprecating statements and pessimistic ideas about the world and the future. To say to a depressed ruminator, “Just stop thinking about it” – whatever the “it” might be – is not very helpful.

Apart from being a debilitating symptom, rumination has also been linked to a heightened vulnerability for recurring depressive episodes and to prolonged depressive episodes. Therefore, a closer analysis of underlying causes and short-term and long-term consequences of this “symptom” of depression might facilitate our understanding of depressive disorders, ultimately leading to refined treatments and effective prevention.

However, a series of important questions on the relation of rumination and depression remains unanswered.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. C., & Bjork, R. A. (1994). Mechanisms of inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. In Dagenbach, D. & Carr, T. (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 265–325). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 366–369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 443–455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 911–927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslow, R., Kocsis, J. H., & Belkin, B. (1980). Memory deficits in depression: Evidence utilizing the Wechsler Memory Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 541–542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S., Baker, J. E., & Robertson, M. M. (1993). Working memory in clinical depression: An experimental study. Psychological Medicine, 23, 87–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behaviour. In Hastorf, A. H. & Isen, A. M. (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 73–108). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Colby, C. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Memory deficits in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 611–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M., & Ross, M. (1984). Getting what you want by revising what you had. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 47, 738–748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognitions in depression: A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, mood, and anxiety disorders: An affective neuroscience approach. In Davidson, R. J. (Ed.), Anxiety, depression, and emotion (pp. 88–108). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 699–711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In Fiedler, K. & Forgas, J. P. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 25–43). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Ellis, H. C., Thomas, R. L., & Rodriguez, I. A. (1984). Emotional mood states and memory: Elaborative encoding, semantic processing, and cognitive effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 470–482.Google ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102–134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enright, S. J., & Beech, A. R. (1990). Obsessional states: Anxiety disorders or schizotypes? An information processing and personality assessment. Psychological Medicine, 20, 621–627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, W. (1971). Mechanisms in directed forgetting. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Erber, R. (1996). The self-regulation of moods. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 251–275). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Erber, R., & Erber, M. W. (1994). Beyond mood and social judgment: Mood incongruent recall and mood regulation. European Journal of Social Psychology Special Issue: Affect in social judgments and cognition, 24, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 395–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment. The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P., Johnson, R., & Ciarrochi, J. (1998). Mood management: The role of processing strategies in affect control and affect infusion. In Kofta, M., Weary, G., & Sedek, G. (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms (pp. 155–195). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1979). Consciousness, information processing and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 225–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1997). Cognitive biases and affect persistence in previously dysphoric and never-dysphoric individuals. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 517–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in university students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 19–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., & Krasnoperova, E. (1998). Biased information processing as a vulnerability factor for depression. Behavior Therapy, 29, 603–617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Neubauer, D. L., & Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 386–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., & Neubauer, D. L. (2000). Information-processing approaches to the study of cognitive biases in depression. In Johnson, S. L., Hayes, A. M., Field, T. M., Schneiderman, N., & McCabe, P. M. (Eds.), Stress, coping, and depression (pp. 117–143). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., Roberts, J. E., & Gilboa, E. (1996). Cognitive interference in depression. In I. Sarason, G., Pierce, G. R., & Sarason, B. R. (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. 347–377). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gray, J. W., Dean, R. S., Rattan, G., & Cramer, K. M. (1987). Neuropsychological aspects of primary affective depression. International Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 911–918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 163–169.Google Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hasher, L., Zacks. R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In Gopher, D. & Koriat, A. (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hedlund, S., & Rude, S. S. (1995). Evidence of latent depressive schemas in formerly depressed individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 517–525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertel, P. T. (1997). On the contributions of deficient cognitive control to memory impairments in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 569–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, P. T. (1998). The relationship between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 166–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, P. T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In Medin, D. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 39, pp. 47–71). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hertel, P. T. (2004). Memory for emotional and nonemotional events in depression: A question of habit? In Reisberg, D. & Hertel, P. (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 186–216). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, P. T., & Gerstle, M. (2003). Depressive deficits in forgetting. Psychological Science, 14, 573–578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertel, P. T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, P. T., & Milan, S. (1994). Depressive deficits in recognition: Dissociation of recollection and familiarity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 736–742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertel, P. T., & Rude, S. S. (1991). Depressive deficits in memory. Focusing attention improves subsequent recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 301–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. H., & Magaro, P. A. (1987). Effects of mood and severity on memory processes in depression and mania. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 28–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J. (2003a). The relation of rumination and inhibition: Evidence from a negative priming task. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Joormann, J. (2003b). Inhibitory dysfunctions in memory in individuals with a history of major depression. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 125–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 437–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression: Tests and an extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 221–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients: An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema's response styles theory. Psychological Medicine, 29, 1323–1333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBerge, D. (1995). Attentional processing: The brain's art of mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linville, P. (1996). Attention inhibition: Does it underlie ruminative thought? In Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts. Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 121–133). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C., Tata, P., & Mathews, A. (1987). Perception of emotionally valenced information in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 67–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarland, C., & Buehler, R. (1998). The impact of negative affect on autobiographical memory: The role of self-focused attention to moods. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 75, 1424–1440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milliken, B., & Tipper, S. P. (1998). Attention and inhibition. In Pashler, H. (Ed.), Attention (pp. 191–221). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 17–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In Dempster, F. N. & Brainerd, C. J. (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 207–261). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolan, S. A., Roberts, J. E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Neuroticism and ruminative response style as predictors of change in depressive symptomatology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 445–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504–511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1061–1072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parrott, W. G., & Sabini, J. (1990). Mood and memory under natural conditions: Evidence for mood incongruent recall. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 59, 321–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1995). Attention in cognitive neuroscience: An overview. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 615–624). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Power, M. J., Dalgleish, T., Claudio, V., Tata, P., & Kentish, J. (2000). The directed forgetting task: Application to emotionally valent material. Journal of Affective Disorders, 57, 147–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. C., Herring, F. H., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A new self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 401–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rokke, P. D., Arnell, K. M., Koch, M. D., & Andrews, J. T. (2002). Dual-task attention deficits in dysphoric mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 370–379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 165–196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L., & DeHart, T. (2000). Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: Consequences for mood-congruent memory. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 78, 737–752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, Z. V., & Vella, D. D. (1990). Self-schema in major depression: Replication and extension of a priming methodology. Special Issue: Selfhood processes and emotional disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 161–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Memory and Cognition, 19, 507–513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 365–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can't shake that feeling: Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 693–707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 68, 1092–1107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stromgren, L. S. (1977). The influence of depression on memory. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 56, 109–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory processes by ignored objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 571–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tipper, S. P., Bourque, T. A., Anderson, S. H., & Brehaut, J. C. (1989). Mechanisms of attention: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 353–378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, F. N., & Sharrock, R. (1985). Description and measurement of concentration problems in depressed patients. Psychological Medicine, 15, 317–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affective information: Evidence for negative priming in the evaluation task. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 65–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M. (1993). The mental control of depression: Psychological obstacles to emotional well-being. In Wegner, D. M. & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 238–257). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Luxton, D. D. (2003). The role of thought suppression in depressive rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 293–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M., Meier, J., & Salas, D. M. (2002). Thought suppression and memory biases during and after depressive moods. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 403–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M., & Roper, D. W. (1988). Depression and mental control: The resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 882–892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorder. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorder. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×