Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T07:17:48.572Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Generative Linguistics: Syntax

from Part I - Theories and Approaches to Spanish Linguistics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2018

Kimberly L. Geeslin
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Get access

Summary

This chapter presents an overview of the basic assumptions of generative syntax, which has been a predominant theoretical framework in research on Spanish syntax for the last several decades. It examines developments in the theory from early work in Principles and Parameters (the Government-Binding model) through the current Minimalist Program, and also looks at the interplay between empirical data and theoretical desiderata. The chapter does not present a comprehensive summary of current research topics in generative research on Spanish syntax, but rather sets out to illustrate this interplay between data and theory by presenting a broad look at research on two particular issues in the field: the syntax of clitic pronouns and the syntax and discourse properties of left peripheral constructions such as topicalization. The chapter also discusses the issue of what constitutes valid data within generative syntax, stressing the importance of introspective speaker judgements while touching briefly on current experimental work as a complement to such judgements.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adger, D. (2003). Core Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ben-Yami, H. (2015). Descartes’ Philosophical Revolution: A Reassessment. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Bonet, E. (1991). Morphology After Syntax: Pronominal Clitics in Romance (Doctoral dissertation). MIT.Google Scholar
Bosque, I. and Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (2009). Fundamentos de sintaxis formal. Madrid: Akal.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1993). A Minimalist Program for Linguistic Theory. In Hale, K. and J. Keyser, S. (eds.), The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 152.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2000). Minimalist Inquiries: The Framework. In Martin, R., Michaels, D., and Uriagereka, J. (eds.), Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 89155.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by Phase. In Kenstowicz, M. (ed.), Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 152.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2004). Beyond Explanatory Adequacy. In Belletti, A. (ed.), The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, Vol. 3: Structures and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 104131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2005). Three Factors in Language Design. Linguistic Inquiry, 36, 122.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1985). The Acquisition of Romance with Special Reference to French. In Slobin, D. (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 688782.Google Scholar
Contreras, H. (1991). On the Position of Subjects. In Rothstein, S. (ed.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol. 25: Perspectives on Phrase Structure. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 6379.Google Scholar
den Dikken, M., et al. (2007). Data and Grammar: Means and Individuals. Theoretical Linguistics, 33, 335352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eguren, L. and Fernández Soriano, O. (2004). Introducción a una sintaxis minimista. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Featherstone, S. (2007). Data in Generative Grammar: The Stick and the Carrot. Theoretical Linguistics, 33, 269318.Google Scholar
Fernández Soriano, O. (2016). Clíticos. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (ed.), Enciclopedia de lingüística hispánica. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gallego, A. (2016). Patterns of Object Agreement in Romance. Paper given at 46th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages, Stony Brook University, New York.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
González López, V. (2008). Spanish Clitic Climbing (Doctoral dissertation). Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Goodall, G. (2004). On the Syntax and Processing of wh-Questions in Spanish. In Schmeiser, B. et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 101114.Google Scholar
Grüter, T. (2008). When Learners Know More than Linguists: (French) Direct Object Clitics are not Objects. Probus, 20, 211234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupton, T. and Leal Méndez, T. (2013). Experimental Methodologies: Two Case Studies Investigating the Syntax–Discourse Interface. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 6, 139164.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (2014). Interfaces and Domains of Quantification. Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar
Hoot, B. (2016). Narrow Presentational Focus in Mexican Spanish: Experimental Evidence. Probus, 28, 335365.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. (1975). French Syntax: The Transformational Cycle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1968). Linguistic Universals and Linguistic Change. In Bach, E. and Harms, R. T. (eds.), Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 171202.Google Scholar
Koopman, H. and Sportiche, D. (1991). The Position of Subjects. Lingua, 85, 211258.Google Scholar
López, L. (2009). A Derivational Syntax for Information Structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S. (1990). La formación de la oración simple: Las omisiones sintácticas (S-V-O) en la adquisición del español. Estudios de Psicología, 11, 4172.Google Scholar
Lorenzo, G. (2016). Gramática generativa. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (ed.), Enciclopedia de lingüística hispánica. London: Routledge, pp. 138150.Google Scholar
Marantz, A. (2005). Generative Linguistics within the Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. The Linguistic Review, 22, 429445.Google Scholar
Martín, J. (2003). Against a Uniform wh-Landing Site for Spanish. In Kempchinsky, P. and Piñeros, C.-E. (eds.), Theory, Practice and Acquisition: Papers from the 6th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 156174.Google Scholar
Matushansky, O. (2006). Head Movement in Linguistic Theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 37, 69109.Google Scholar
Maynes, J. (2012). Linguistic Intuition and Calibration. Linguistics and Philosophy, 35, 443460.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. (1983). Grammatical Theory: Its Limits and Possibilities. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W. (2012). Three Factors in the Design and Acquisition of Language. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 493499.Google Scholar
Olarrea, A. (1998). On the Position of Subjects in Spanish. Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca Julio de Urquijo (Universidad del País Vasco), 32, 47108.Google Scholar
Ordóñez, F. and Olarrea, A. (2006). Microvariation in Caribbean and Non-Caribbean Spanish Interrogatives. Probus, 18, 5997.Google Scholar
Ormazabal, J. and Romero, J. (2013). Object Clitics, Agreement and Dialectal Variation. Probus, 25, 301344.Google Scholar
Otero, C. P. (1971). Evolución y revolución en romance, Vol. 1. Barcelona: Seix Barral.Google Scholar
Otero, C. P. (1972). Acceptable Grammatical Sentences in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 3, 233242.Google Scholar
Otero, C. P. (1973). Agrammaticality in Performance. Linguistic Inquiry, 4, 551562.Google Scholar
Otero, C. P. (1976). Evolución y revolución en romance, Vol. 2. Barcelona: Seix Barral.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2013). On the Nature of Island Constraints I: Language Processing and Reductionist Accounts. In Sprouse, J. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 65108.Google Scholar
Phillips, C., et al. (2011). Grammatical Illusions and Selective Fallibility in Real Time Language Comprehension. In Runner, J. (ed.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol. 37: Experiments at the Interfaces. Bingley: Emerald Publications, pp. 147180.Google Scholar
Reglero, L. (2005). Wh-in-situ Constructions: Syntax and/or Phonology? In Alderete, J. et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the 24th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 334342.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1997). The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery. In Haegeman, L. (ed.), Elements of Grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 281337.Google Scholar
Schütze, C. (1996). The Empirical Base of Linguistics: Grammaticality Judgments and Linguistic Methodology. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. and Keller, F. (2005). Gradience in Linguistic Data. Lingua, 115, 14971524.Google Scholar
Sportiche, D. (1988). A Theory of Floating Quantifiers and its Corollaries for Constituent Structure. Linguistic Inquiry, 19, 425449.Google Scholar
Sportiche, D. (1996). Clitic Constructions. In Rooryck, J. and Zaring, L. (eds.), Phrase Structure and the Lexicon. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 213275.Google Scholar
Strozer, J. (1976). Clitics in Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). University of California–Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Suñer, M. (1989). Spanish Syntax and Semantics in the Eighties: The Principles-and-Parameters Approach. Hispania, 72, 832847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suñer, M. (1992). Two Properties of Clitics in Clitic-Doubled Structures. In Huang, C.-T. J. and May, R. (eds.), Logical Structure and Linguistic Structure. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 233252.Google Scholar
Suñer, M. (2009). Formal Linguistics and the Syntax of Spanish: Past, Present and Future. In Collentine, J. et al. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 926.Google Scholar
Torrego, E. (1995). On the Nature of Clitic Doubling. In Campos, H. and Kempchinsky, P. (eds.), Evolution and Revolution in Linguistic Theory. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 399418.Google Scholar
Uriagereka, J. (1995). Aspects of the Syntax of Clitic Placement in Western Romance. Linguistic Inquiry, 26, 79124.Google Scholar
Uribe-Etxebarria, M. (2002). In Situ Questions and Masked Movement. In Pica, P. and Rooryck, J. (eds.), Linguistic Variation Yearbook. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 259303.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M.-L. (1998). Prosody, Focus and Word Order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×