Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T03:34:12.870Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part V - The Acquisition of Spanish

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2018

Kimberly L. Geeslin
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Aguado-Orea, J. and Pine, J. M. (2002). There is no Evidence for a “No Overt Subject” Stage in Early Child Spanish: A Note on Grinstead (2000). Journal of Child Language, 29, 865874.Google Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., Cantrell, L. M., Smith, L. B., and Alva Canto, E. A. (2014). Early Comprehension of the Spanish Plural. Journal of Child Language, 41, 13561372.Google Scholar
Baauw, S., Escobar, M. A., and Philip, W. (1997). A Delay of Principle B Effect in Spanish Speaking Children: The Role of Lexical Feature Acquisition. In Sorace, A., Heycock, C., and Shillcock, R. (eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ’97 Conference on Language Acquisition. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, pp. 1621.Google Scholar
Bel, A. (2001). Teoria lingüística i adquisició del llenguatge. Anàlisi comparada dels trets morfològics en català i en castellà. Barcelona: Institut d’Estudis Catalans.Google Scholar
Bel, A. (2003). The Syntax of Subjects in the Acquisition of Spanish and Catalan. Probus, 15, 150177.Google Scholar
Belletti, A. (ed.) (2004). Structures and Beyond – The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, Vol. 3. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Castilla, A., Pérez-Leroux, A.-T., and Eriks-Brophy, A. (2008). Omissions and Substitutions in Early Spanish Clitics. In Gavarró, A. and Freitas, M. J. (eds.), Language Acquisition and Development. Proceedings of GALA 2007. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Press, pp. 112122.Google Scholar
Chien, Y. C. and Wexler, K. (1992). Children’s Knowledge of Locality Conditions in Binding as Evidence for the Modularity of Syntax and Pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1, 225295.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G. (1998). Reference to Kinds across Languages. Natural Language Semantics, 6, 339405.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G., Guasti, M. T., and Gualmini, A. (1999). Nouns and Articles in Child Grammar and the Syntax/Semantics Map. Paper presented at Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition, Potsdam.Google Scholar
Choe, J. and Deen, K. (2015). Children’s Difficulty with Raising: A Performance Account. Language Acquisition, 23 (2), 112141.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Aveledo, F., and Roca, I. (2002). The Development of Regular and Irregular Verb Inflection in Spanish Child Language. Journal of Child Language, 29, 591622.Google Scholar
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., and Tomasello, M. (2008). Young German Children’s Early Syntactic Competence: A Preferential Looking Study. Developmental Science, 11 (4), 575582.Google Scholar
Elliot, M. and Pirvulescu, M. (2016). Very Early Object Clitic Omission: How Early is too Early? Paper presented at the Romance Turn 8, Bellaterra. Available from http://filcat.uab.cat/theromanceturn/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/Elliott_et_alii.pdf (last access January 6, 2018).Google Scholar
Escobar, L. and Gavarró, A. (2000). The Acquisition of Clitics and Strong Pronouns in Catalan. In Gerlach, B. and Grijzenhout, J. (eds.), Clitics in Phonology, Morphology and Syntax. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 161180.Google Scholar
Franck, J., Millotte, S., Posada, A., and Rizzi, L. (2011). Abstract Knowledge of Word Order by 19 Months: An Eye-Tracking Study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34 (2), 323336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., Belletti, A., and Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized Relatives: Types of Intervention in the Acquisition of A-Bar Dependencies. Lingua, 119, 331344.Google Scholar
García del Real, I. (2015). The Acquisition of Tense and Aspect in Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). UPV/EHU.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A. and Cabré-Sans, Y. (2009). Subjects, Verb Classes and Word Order in Child Catalan. In Grinstead, J. (ed.), Hispanic Child Languages. Typical and Impaired Development. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 175194.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A., Torrens, V., and Wexler, K. (2010). Object Clitic Omission: Two Language Types. Language Acquisition, 17 (4), 192219.Google Scholar
Grinstead, J. (2004). Subjects and Interface Delay in Child Spanish and Catalan. Language, 80 (1), 4072.Google Scholar
Grinstead, J., Lintz, P., Vega-Mendoza, M., de la Mora, J., Cantú-Sánchez, M., and Flores- Avalos, B. (2014). Evidence of Optional Infinitive Verbs in the Spontaneous Speech of Spanish-Speaking Children with SLI. Lingua, 140, 5266.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (19931994). Verb Syntax in Italian Child Grammar: Finite and Non-Finite Verbs. Language Acquisition, 3, 140.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (1996). Acquisition of Italian Interrogatives. In Clahsen, H. (ed.), Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 241269.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language Acquisition. The Growth of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. and Gavarró, A. (2003). Catalan as a Test for Hypotheses Concerning Article Omission. In Beachley, B., Brown, A., and Colin, F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 288298.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Gavarró, A., de Lange, J., and Caprin, C. (2008). Article Omission across Child Languages. Language Acquisition, 15 (2), 89119.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez Mangado, M. J. (2006). Acquiring Long-Distance wh-Questions in L1 Spanish. In Torrens, V. and Escobar, L. (eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax in Romance Languages. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 251287.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (2001). The Semantics of Spanish Plural Existential Determiners. Probus, 13, 113154.Google Scholar
Hernández-Pina, F. (1984). Teorías psicolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K. R. and Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The Origins of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hodgson, M. (2010). Locatum Structures and the Acquisition of Telicity. Language Acquisition, 17 (3), 155182.Google Scholar
Hu, S. (2014). Intervention Effects and the Acquisition of Relativization and Topicalization in Chinese (Doctoral dissertation). Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona/Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Müller, N., Kang, O.-K., Biemer, B., and Rigaut, C. (1996). On the Acquisition of the Pronominal System in French and German. In Stringfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E., and Zukowski, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 374385.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. and Nash, L. (forthcoming). Why Accusative Clitics are Avoided in Normal and Impaired Language Development. In Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., and Wexler, K. (eds.), Essays in Syntax, Morphology and Phonology of SLI. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Katsos, N., Cummins, C., Ezeizabarrena, M.-J., Gavarró, A., Kuvač Kraljević, J., et al. (2016). Cross-linguistic patterns in the acquisition of quantifiers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 113 (33), 92449249. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601341113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lassotta, R., Omaki, A., and Franck, J. (2014). Abstract Knowledge of Non-Canonical Word Order by Age 2. Paper presented at the 39th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Abstract available from www.bu.edu/bucld/files/2015/03/BUCLD-39-Handbook.pdf (last access January 6, 2018).Google Scholar
Legendre, G., Culberston, J., Zaroukian, E., Hsin, L., Barrière, I., and Nazzi, T. (2014). Is Children’s Comprehension of Subject–Verb Agreement Universally Late? Comparative Evidence from French, English, and Spanish. Lingua, 144, 2139.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C. and Fernald, A. (2007). Young Children Learning Spanish Make Rapid Use of Grammatical Gender in Spoken Word Recognition. Psychological Science, 18 (3), 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lleó, C. and Demuth, K. (1999). Prosodic Constraints on the Emergence of Grammatical Morphemes: Crosslinguistic Evidence from Germanic and Romance Languages. In Greenhill, A., Littlefield, H., and Tano, C. (eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 407418.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S. (1994). La adquisición del lenguaje: Talón de Aquiles y poción mágica de la teoría cognitiva. Cognitiva, 6 (2), 213239.Google Scholar
Lorusso, P., Caprin, C., and Guasti, M. T. (2005). Overt Subject Distribution in Early Italian Children. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., and Ha, S. (eds.), BUCLD 29 Online Proceedings Supplement. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla. Available from http://www.bu.edu/bucld/files/2011/05/29-LorussoBUCLD2004.pdf (last access December 27, 2017).Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1995). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mateu, V. E. (2015). Object Clitic Omission in Child Spanish: Evaluating Representational and Processing Accounts. Language Acquisition, 22 (3), 240284.Google Scholar
McKee, C. (1992). A Comparison of Pronouns and Anaphors in Italian and English Acquisition. Language Acquisition, 2, 2154.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). The Acquisition of Spanish. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Noveck, I. (2001). When Children are More Logical than Adults: Experimental Investigations of Scalar Implicature. Cognition, 78 (2), 165188.Google Scholar
Omaki, A. and Lidz, J. (2015). Linking Parser Development to Acquisition of Syntactic Knowledge. Language Acquisition, 22 (2), 158192.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (1993). Empty Categories and the Acquisition of wh-Movement (Doctoral dissertation). University of Massachusetts–Amherst.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (1995). Resumptives in the Acquisition of Relative Clauses, Language Acquisition, 4, 105138.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2005). Number Problems in Children. In Guski, C. (ed.), Proceedings of the 2005 Canadian Linguistics Association Annual Conference. Published online at http://cla-acl.ca/actes-2005-proceedings/.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla, A. P., and Brunner, J. (2012). General and Specific Effects of Lexicon in Grammar: Determiner and Object Pronoun Omissions in Child Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55 (2), 313327.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A.-T. and Dalious, J. (1998). The Acquisition of Spanish Interrogative Inversion. Hispanic Linguistics, 10, 84114.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A.-T., Munn, A., Schmitt, C., and de Irish, M. (2004). Learning Definite Determiners: Genericity and Definiteness in English and Spanish. In Brugos, Alejna, Micciulla, Linnea, and Smith, Christine E. (eds.), BUCLD 28 Online Proceedings Supplement. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla. Available from http://www.bu.edu/bucld/files/2011/05/28-perez-leroux.pdf (last access December 27, 2017).Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A.-T., Pirvulescu, M. and Roberge, Y. (2008). Null Objects in Child Language: Syntax and the Lexicon. Lingua, 118 (3), 370398.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1989). The Acquisition of Morphemes: Some Evidence from Spanish. Journal of Pyscholinguistic Research, 18 (3), 289312.Google Scholar
Pierce, A. (1992). The Acquisition of Passives in Spanish and the Question of A-Chain Maturation. Language Acquisition, 2, 5581.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E. and Caselli, M. C. (1992). The Acquisition of Italian Morphology: Implications for Models of Language Development. Journal of Child Language, 19, 491557.Google Scholar
Pratt, A. and Grinstead, J. (2007). Optional Infinitives in Child Spanish. In Belikova, A., Meroni, L., and Umeda, M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 351362.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized Minimality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (19931994). Some Remarks on Linguistic Theory and Language Development. Language Acquisition, 3, 371393.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1997). The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery. In Haegeman, L. (ed.), Elements of Grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 281337.Google Scholar
Saceda, M. (2005). Adquisición prosódica en español peninsular septentrional: La sílaba y la palabra prosódica (MA thesis). Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Salustri, M. and Hyams, N. (2003). Is there an Analogue to the RI Stage in the Null Subject Languages? In Beachley, B., Brown, A., and Colin, F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 692703.Google Scholar
Schaeffer, J. (2000). The Acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling and Clitic Placement. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Searchinger, Gene (dir.) (1995). Acquiring the Human Language, Part 2: Playing the Language Game. New York: Equinox Films/Ways of Knowing.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. (1990). Adventures in Long-Distance Moving: The Acquisition of Complex wh-Questions (Doctoral dissertation). University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Toribio, J. (2000). Setting Parametric Limits on Dialectal Variation in Spanish. Lingua, 110, 315341.Google Scholar
Torrens, V. (1995). The Acquisition of Inflection in Spanish and Catalan. In Schütze, C., Ganger, J., and Broihier, K. (eds.), Papers on Language Processing and Acquisition. Boston, MA: MITWPL, pp. 451472.Google Scholar
Torrens, V. (2002). La adquisición del tiempo y la concordancia, Madrid: UNED.Google Scholar
Torrens, V. and Wexler, K. (2000). The Acquisition of Clitic Doubling in Spanish. In Powers, S. and Hamann, C. (eds.), The Acquisition of Scrambling and Cliticization. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 279297.Google Scholar
van Hout, A., Arche, M., Demirdache, H., García del Real, I., García Sanz, A., Gavarró, A., Gómez Marzo, L., Hommes, S., Kazanina, N., Liu, J., Lungu, O., Martin, F., and Strangmann, I. (2017). Agent Control and the Acquisition of Event Culmination in Basque, Dutch, English, Spanish, and Mandarin. In LaMendola, Maria and Scott, Jennifer (eds.), Proceedings of the 41st Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 323332.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic Subjects in the Early Speech of American and Italian Children. Cognition, 40, 2181.Google Scholar
Vargas-Tokuda, M., Gutiérrez-Rexach, J., and Grinstead, J. (2009). Context and the Scalar Implicatures of Indefinites in Child Spanish. In Grinstead, J. (ed.), Hispanic Child Languages: Typical and Impaired Development. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 93116.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional Infinitives, Head Movement and the Economy of Derivations. In Lightfoot, D. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Verb Movement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 305350.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (2011) Grammatical Computation in the Optional Infinitive Stage. In de Villiers, J. and Roeper, T. (eds.), Handbook of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition. Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 53118.Google Scholar
Wexler, K., Gavarró, A., and Torrens, V. (2004). Feature Checking and Object Clitic Omission in Child Catalan. In Bok-Bennema, R., Hollebrandse, B., Kampers-Mahne, B., and Sleeman, P. (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2002. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 253268.Google Scholar

References

Alonso Ovalle, L., Fernández-Solera, S., Frazier, L., and Clifton, C. (2002). Null vs. Overt Pronouns and the Topic-Focus Articulation in Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 14 (2), 151169.Google Scholar
Atkinson, D. (ed.). (2011). Alternative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bailey, N., Madden, C., and Krashen, S. D. (1974). Is There a “Natural Sequence” in Adult Second Language Learning? Language Learning, 4 (2), 235243.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2000). Tense and Aspect in Second Language Acquisition: Form, Meaning, and Use. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Berko Gleason, J. and Bernstein Ratner, N. (2016). The Development of Language (9th edn). Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Valenzuela, E. (2008). Eventive and Stative Passives in Spanish: A Matter of Aspect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 323–36.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. and Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical Processing in Language Learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27 (1), 342.Google Scholar
Dulay, H. and Burt, M. (1974). Natural Sequences in Child Second Language Acquisition. Language Learning, 24 (1), 3753.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. and Wulff, S. (2015). Usaged-Based Approaches to SLA. In VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition (2nd edn). New York: Routledge, pp. 7593.Google Scholar
Finnemann, M. D. (1990). Markedness and Learner Strategy: Form- and Meaning-Oriented Learners in the Foreign Language Context. The Modern Language Journal, 74 (2), 176187.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (2000). A New Approach to the Second Language Acquisition of Copula Choice in Spanish. In Leow, R. P. and Sanz, C. (eds.), Spanish Applied Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium: Papers from the 1999 Conference on the L1 and L2 Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 5066.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2005). The Acquisition of Copula Choice in Instructed Spanish: The Role of Individual Characteristics. In Eddington, D. (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6677.Google Scholar
Gregg, K. R. (1989). Second Language Acquisition Theory: The Case for a Generative Perspective. In Gass, S. M. and Schachter, J. (eds.), Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1540.Google Scholar
Guntermann, G. (1992). An Analysis of Interlanguage Development over Time: Part II, ser and estar. Hispania, 75 (5), 12941303.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R. (2001). Second Language Syntax: A Generative Introduction. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Herschensohn, J. (2007). Language Development and Age. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Holmberg, A. (2010). Null Subject Parameters. In Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A., Roberts, I., and Sheehan, M. (eds.), Parametric Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 88124.Google Scholar
Jegerski, J., VanPatten, B., and Keating, G. D. (2011). Who Was Walking on the Beach? Anaphora Resolution in Spanish Heritage Speakers and Adult Second Language Learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33 (2), 193221.Google Scholar
Jordan, G. (2004). Theory Construction in Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and Practice in Second Language Acquisition. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M. (1989). On Some Properties of the “Pro-Drop” Parameter: Looking for Missing Subjects in Non-Native Spanish. In Gass, S. M. and Schachter, J. (eds.), Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 109133.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Díaz, L., and Mawell, D. (1998). Null Arguments in Non-Native Grammars: The Spanish L2 of Chinese, English, French, German, Japanese and Korean Speakers. In Klein, E. and Martohardjono, G. (eds.), The Development of Second Language Grammar: A Generative Approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 113149.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1990). The Least a Second Language Acquisition Theory Needs to Explain. TESOL Quarterly, 24 (4), 649666.Google Scholar
Losos, J. B., Schoener, T. W., Langerhans, B., and Spiller, D. A. (2006). Rapid Temporal Reversal in Predator-Driven Natural Selection. Science, 314 (Nov.), 1111.Google Scholar
Lubbers Quesada, M. and Blackwell, S. E. (2009). The L2 Acquisition of Null and Overt Spanish Subject Pronouns: A Pragmatic Approach. In Collentine, J., García, M., Lafford, B., and Marcos Marín, F. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 117130.Google Scholar
Lust, B. (2006). Child Language: Acquisition and Growth. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montalbetti, Mario, M. (1984). After Binding. On the Interpretation of Pronouns (Doctoral dissertation). MIT.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Glass, W. R. (1999). Null Anaphora in Spanish Second Language Acquisition: Probabilistic versus Generative Approaches. Second Language Research, 15 (2), 220249.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1987). Psychological Constraints on the Teachability of Languages. In Pfaff, C. (ed.), First and Second Language Acquisition Processes. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 143168.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language Processing and Second Language Development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Roby, D. B. (2007). Aspect and the Categorization of States: The Case of ser and estar in Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and Iverson, M. (2007). On Parameter Clustering and Resetting the Null-Subject Parameter in L2 Spanish: Implications and Observations. Hispania, 90 (2), 328341.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and VanPatten, B. (2013). On Multiplicity and Mutual Exclusivity: The Case for Different SLA Theories. In García-Mayo, M. del P., Junkal Gutiérrez-Mangado, M., and Martínez Adrián, M. (eds.), Contemporary Approaches to Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 243256.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (2014). Understanding Child Language Acquisition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ryan, J. and Lafford, B. (1992). The Acquisition of Lexical Meaning in a Study Abroad Environment. Hispania, 75 (3), 714–22.Google Scholar
Schmitt, C. (2005). Semi-Copulas: Event and Aspectual Composition. In Kempchinsky, P. and Slabakova, R. (eds.), Syntax, Semantics and the Acquisition of Aspect. New York: Springer, pp. 121145.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2016). Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toribio, J. (2000). Setting Parametric Limits on Dialectal Variation in Spanish. Lingua, 10, 315341.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2015). The Declarative/Procedural Model: A Neurobiologically Motivated Theory for First and Second Language. In VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition (2nd edn). New York: Routledge, pp. 135158.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1987). Classroom Learners’ Acquisition of ser and estar: Accounting for the Data. In VanPatten, B., Dvorak, T. R., and Lee, J. F. (eds.), Foreign Language Learning: A Research Perspective. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House, pp. 6176.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2010). Some Verbs are More Perfect than Others: Why Learners have Difficulty with ser and estar and What it Means for Instruction. Hispania, 93 (1), 2938.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2013). Mental Representation and Skill in Instructed SLA. In Schwieter, J. (ed.), Innovations in SLA, Bilingualism, and Cognition: Research and Practice. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 322.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2014). On the Limits of instruction: 40 Years after “Interlanguage.” In Han, Z.-H. and Tarone, E. (eds.), Interlanguage: 40 Years Later. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 105126.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2015). Input Processing in Adult SLA. In VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition (2nd edn). New York: Routledge, pp. 111134.Google Scholar
VanPatten, Bill. (1985). The Acquisition of ser and estar by Adult Learners of Spanish: A Preliminary Investigation of Transitional Stages of Competence. Hispania, 68 (2), 399406.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., Keating, G. D., and Leeser, M. J. (2012). Missing Verbal Inflections as a Representational Problem: Evidence from Self-Paced Reading. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2 (2), 109140.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. and Rothman, J. (2014). Against “Rules.” In Benati, A., Laval, C., and Arche, M. J. (eds.), The Grammar Dimension in Instructed Second Language Acquisition: Theory, Research, and Practice. London: Bloomsbury, pp. 1535.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.). (2015). Theories in Second Language Acquisition (2nd edn). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
White, L. (2016). Prodrop then and now: Changing Perspectives on Null Subjects in Second Language Acquisition. In Alba de la Fuente, A., Valenzuela, E., and Martínez-Sanz, C. (eds.), Language Acquisition beyond Parameters: Studies in Honour of Juana M. Liceras. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1735.Google Scholar
Whong, M., Gil, K-H., and Marsden, H. (2013). Universal Grammar and the Second Language Classroom. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar

References

Alvord, S. M. and Christiansen, D. E. (2012). Factors Influencing the Acquisition of Spanish Voiced Stop Spirantization during an Extended Stay Abroad. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 5, 239276.Google Scholar
Amengual, M. (2012). Interlingual Influence in Bilingual Speech: Cognate Status Effect in a Continuum of Bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 517530.Google Scholar
Best, C. (1995). A Direct Realist View of Cross-Language Speech Perception. In Strange, W. (ed.), Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 171206.Google Scholar
Best, C. T. and Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and Second-Language Speech Perception: Commonalities and Complementarities. In Bohn, O.-S. and Munro, M. J. (eds.), Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1334.Google Scholar
Bongiovanni, S., Long, A. Y., Solon, M., and Willis, E. W. (2015). The Effect of Short-Term Study Abroad on Second Language Spanish Phonetic Development. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 8, 243283.Google Scholar
Bradlow, A. R. (1995). A Comparative Acoustic Study of English and Spanish Vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 19161924.Google Scholar
Carranza, M. (2008). Fenómenos de interferencia fónica relacionados con la vocal /u/ en la interlingua de estudiantes de español como lengua extranjera. Estudios Lingüísticos Hispánicos, 23, 121.Google Scholar
Cobb, K. and Simonet, M. (2015). Adult Second Language Learning of Spanish Vowels. Hispania, 98, 4760.Google Scholar
Daidone, D. and Darcy, I. (2014). Quierro comprar una guitara: Lexical Encoding of the Tap and Trill by L2 Learners of Spanish. In Miller, R. T. et al. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2012 Second Language Research Forum. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 3950.Google Scholar
Derwing, T. M. and Munro, M. J. (2015). Pronunciation Fundamentals: Evidence-Based Perspectives for L2 Teaching and Research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Díaz, M. and Simonet, M. (2015). Second Language Acquisition of Spanish /e/ and /ei/ by Native English Speakers. Hispania, 98, 750761.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. (2004). Context of Learning in the Acquisition of Spanish Second Language Phonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 249273.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. and Lazar, N. (2003). Acoustic Analysis of Voiceless Initial Stops in the Speech of Study Abroad and Regular Class Students: Context of Learning as a Variable in Spanish Second Language Acquisition. In Kempchinsky, P. and Piñeros, C.-E. (eds.), Theory, Practice, and Acquisition. Papers from the 6th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium and the 5th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 352370.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2008). The Study of Second Language Acquisition (2nd edn). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. and Boersma, P. (2002). The Subset Problem in L2 Perceptual Development: Multiple-Category Assimilation by Dutch Learners of Spanish. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S. and Do, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 208219.Google Scholar
Face, T. L. (2000). The Role of Syllable Weight in the Perception of Spanish Stress. In Campos, H., Herburger, E., Morales-Front, A., and Walsh, T. J. (eds.), Hispanic Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 113.Google Scholar
Face, T. L. (2005). Syllable Weight and the Perception of Spanish Stress Placement by Second Language Learners. Journal of Language and Learning, 3, 90103.Google Scholar
Face, T. L. (2006). Intervocalic Rhotic Pronunciation by Adult Learners of Spanish as a Second Language. In Klee, C. A. and Face, T. L. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 4758.Google Scholar
Face, T. and Menke, M. (2009). Acquisition of the Spanish Voiced Spirants by Second Language Learners. In Collentine, J. (ed.), Selected proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 3952.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995) Second Language Speech Learning: Theory, Findings, and Problems. In Strange, W. (ed.), Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 233277.Google Scholar
García Bayonas, M. (2007). The Acquisition of Vowels in Spanish and English as Second Language. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. and Gudmestad, A. (2011). The Acquisition of Variation in Second-Language Spanish: An Agenda for Integrating Studies of the L2 Sound System. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5, 137157.Google Scholar
George, A. (2014). Study Abroad in Central Spain: The Development of Regional Phonological Features. Foreign Language Annals, 47, 97114.Google Scholar
González-Bueno, M. (1995). Adquisición de los alófonos fricativos de las oclusivas sonoras españolas por aprendices de español como segunda lengua. Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada, 13, 6479.Google Scholar
González-Bueno, M. (1997). The Effects of Formal Instruction on the Acquisition of Spanish Phonology. In Glass, W. R. and Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 5775.Google Scholar
Gordon, L. S. (2008). Factors Affecting English Speakers’ Perception of L2 Spanish Vowels (Doctoral dissertation). Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Green, K. P., Zampini, M. L., and Magliore, J. (1997). An Examination of Word-Initial-Stop Closure Interval in English, Spanish, and Spanish-English Bilinguals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 3136.Google Scholar
Henriksen, N. C., Geeslin, K. L., and Willis, E. W. (2010). The Development of L2 Spanish Intonation during a Study Abroad Immersion Program in León, Spain: Global Contours and Final Boundary Movements. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 3, 113162.Google Scholar
Hualde, J. I. (2005). The Sounds of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurtado, L. M. and Estrada, C. (2010). Factors Influencing the Second Language Acquisition of Spanish Vibrants. Modern Language Journal, 94, 7486.Google Scholar
Knouse, S. K. (2012). The Acquisition of Dialectal Phonemes in a Study Abroad Context: The Case of the Castilian Theta. Foreign Language Annals, 45, 512542.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (2006). A Course in Phonetics (5th edn). Boston, MA: Thomson.Google Scholar
Lisker, L. and Abramson, A. S. (1964). A Cross-Language Study of Voicing in Initial Stops: Acoustical Measurements. Word, 20, 384422.Google Scholar
Long, A. Y. (2016). The Acquisition of Sociolinguistic Competence by Korean Learners of Spanish: Development and Use of the Copula, Subject Expression, and Intervocalic Stops (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Lord, G. (2005). (How) Can we Teach Foreign Language Pronunciation? On the Effects of a Spanish Phonetics Course. Hispania, 88, 557567.Google Scholar
Lord, G. (2007). The Role of the Lexicon in Learning Second Language Stress Patterns. Applied Language Learning, 17 (1–2), 114.Google Scholar
Lord, G. (2010). The Combined Effects of Instruction and Immersion on Second Language Pronunciation. Foreign Language Annals, 43, 488503.Google Scholar
Martínez Celdrán, E. (1993). La percepción categorial de /b-p/ en español basada en las diferencias de duración. Estudios de Fonética Experimental, 5, 223239.Google Scholar
Menke, M. R. (2010). The Acquisition of Spanish Vowels by Native English-Speaking Students in Spanish Immersion Programs (Doctoral dissertation). University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Menke, M. R. and Face, T. L. (2010). Second Language Spanish Vowel Production: An Acoustic Analysis. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 3, 181214.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2017). Autonomy in Second Language Phonology: Choice vs. Limits. Language Teaching, 50 (3), 395411. doi: 10.1017/S0261444815000191. Published online May 19, 2015.Google Scholar
Nibert, H. J. (2005). The Acquisition of the Phrase Accent by Intermediate and Advanced Adult Learners of Spanish as a Second Language. In Eddington, D. (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 108122.Google Scholar
Olive, J. P., Greenwood, A., and Coleman, J. (1993). Acoustics of American English Speech: A Dynamic Approach. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Olsen, M. K. (2012). The L2 Acquisition of Spanish Rhotics by L2 English Speakers: The Effect of L1 Articulatory Routines and Phonetic Context for Allophonic Variation. Hispania, 95, 6582.Google Scholar
Ringer-Hilfinger, K. (2012). Learner Acquisition of Dialect Variation in a Study Abroad Context: The Case of the Spanish [θ]. Foreign Language Annals, 45, 430446.Google Scholar
Rogers, B. M. A. and Alvord, S. M. (2014). The Gradience of Spirantization: Factors Affecting L2 Production of Intervocalic Spanish [ß̞, ð̞, ɣ̞]. Spanish in Context, 11, 402424.Google Scholar
Rose, M. (2010a). Intervocalic Tap and Trill Production in the Acquisition of Spanish as a Second Language. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 3, 379419.Google Scholar
Rose, M. (2010b). Differences in Discriminating L2 Consonants: A Comparison of Spanish Taps and Trills. In Prior, M. T., Watanabe, Y., and Lee, S.-K. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2008 2nd Language Research Forum. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 181196.Google Scholar
Schmidt, L. B. (2011). Acquisition of Dialectal Variation in Second Language: L2 Perception of Aspiration of Spanish /s/ (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Schmidt, L. B. (2014). Contextual Variation in L2 Spanish: Voicing Assimilation in Advanced Learner Speech. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 7, 79113.Google Scholar
Schoonmaker-Gates, E. (2012). Foreign Accent Perception in L2 Spanish: The Role of Proficiency and L2 Experience. In Levis, J. and LeVelle, K. (eds.), Social Factors in Pronunciation Acquisition: Proceedings of the 3rd Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching Conference. Ames, IA: Iowa State University, pp. 8492.Google Scholar
Solon, M. (2017). Do Learners Lighten Up? Phonetic and Allophonic Acquisition of Spanish /l/ by English-Speaking Learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39 (4), 801832.Google Scholar
Solon, M., Linford, B., and Geeslin, K. L. (forthcoming). Acquisition of Sociophonetic Variation: Intervocalic /d/ Reduction in Native and Nonnative Spanish. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada.Google Scholar
Solon, M. and Long, A. Y. (2016). Context of Learning and the Development of Pronunciation: A Systematic Narrative Review. Paper presented at the Second Language Research Forum, Columbia Teachers’ College, New York.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. J. (2011). Vowel Duration in Second Language Spanish Vowels: Study Abroad versus At-Home Learners. Arizona Working Papers in SLA and Teaching, 18, 77104.Google Scholar
Trimble, J. C. (2013). Acquiring Variable L2 Spanish Intonation in a Study Abroad Context (Doctoral dissertation). University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Zampini, M. L. (1994). The Role of Native Language Transfer and Task Formality in the Acquisition of Spanish Spirantization. Hispania, 77, 470481.Google Scholar
Zampini, M. L. (1998). The Relationship between the Production and Perception of L2 Spanish Stops. Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education, 3, 85100.Google Scholar
Zampini, M. L., Clarke, C. M., and Green, K. P. (2000). Language Experience and the Perception of Stop Consonant Voicing in Spanish: The Case of Late English-Spanish Bilinguals. In Leow, R. P. and Sanz, C. (eds.), Spanish Applied Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium: Papers from the 1999 Conference on the L1 and L2 Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 194209.Google Scholar
Zampini, M. L. and Green, K. P. (2001). The Voicing Contrast in English and Spanish: The Relationship between Perception and Production. In Nicol, J. (ed.), One Mind, Two Languages: Bilingual Language Processing. Malden, MA: Blackwell, pp. 2348.Google Scholar

References

Alarcón, I. (2009). The Processing of Gender Agreement in L1 and L2 Spanish: Evidence from Reaction Time Data. Hispania, 92 (4), 814828.Google Scholar
Alarcón, I. (2011). Spanish Grammatical Gender under Complete and Incomplete Acquisition: Early and Late Bilinguals’ Linguistic Behavior within the Noun Phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 332350.Google Scholar
Alarcón, I. (2014). Grammatical Gender in Second Language Spanish. In Geeslin, K. L. (ed.), The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 202218.Google Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J., Fiorentino, R., and Gabriele, A. (2014). Morphosyntactic Processing in Advanced Second Language (L2) Learners: An Event-Related Potential Investigation of the Effects of L1–L2 Similarity and Structural Distance. Second Language Research, 30 (3), 275306.Google Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J., Miller, D., and Rothman, J. (2017). Examining Morphological Variability in the Online Comprehension and Production of Number and Gender Agreement in L2 Spanish: An ERP Study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43 (10), 15091536. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000394.Google Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J. and Rothman, J. (2016). The role of Morphological Markedness in the Processing of Number and Gender Agreement in Spanish: An Event-Related Potential Investigation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31 (10), 12721298. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032. Published online August 12, 2016.Google Scholar
Alexiadou, A. and Anagnostopoulou, E. (1998). Parameterizing Agr: Word Order, V-Movement and EPP-Checking. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 16 (3), 491539.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. (1986). El desarrollo de la morfología verbal en el español como segundo idioma. In Meisel, J. M. (ed.), Adquisición de lenguaje/Aquisição da linguagem. Frankfurt: Vervuert, pp. 115138.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. W. and Shirai, Y. (1994). Discourse Motivations for Some Cognitive Acquisition Principles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16 (2), 133156.Google Scholar
Bardovi‐Harlig, K. (1999). Exploring the Interlanguage of Interlanguage Pragmatics: A Research Agenda for Acquisitional Pragmatics. Language Learning, 49 (4), 677713.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2000). Tense and Aspect in Second Language Acquisition: Form, Meaning, and Use. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Comajoan, L. (2008). Order of Acquisition and Developmental Readiness. In Spolsky, B. and Hult, F. M. (eds.), The Handbook of Educational Linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 383397.Google Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J., and Prévost, P. (2005). Acquisition of Mood Distinctions in L2 Spanish. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., and Ha, S. (eds.), BUCLD 29: Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 97108.Google Scholar
Bowles, M. and Montrul, S. (2009). Instructed L2 Acquisition of Differential Object Marking in Spanish. In Leow, R., Campos, H., and Lardiere, D. (eds.), Little Words. Their History, Phonology, Syntax, Semantics, Pragmatics and Acquisition. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 199210.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, E. and Cortés-Torres, M. (2012). Syntactic and Pragmatic Usage of the [estar + adjective] Construction in Puerto Rican Spanish: ¡Está brutal! In Geeslin, K. L. and Díaz-Campos, M. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 14th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6174.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. (2009). Eventive and Stative Passives: The Role of Transfer in the Acquisition of ser and estar by German and English L1 Speakers. In Collentine, J., García, M., Lafford, B. A., and Marcos Marín, F. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 2738.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2002). L2 Acquisition of Indefinite Object Drop. In Costa, J. and Freitas, M. J. (eds.), GALA 2001 Proceedings. Lisbon, Portugal: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, pp. 6067.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Montrul, S. (1996). Verb Movement and Clitic Placement in French and Spanish as a Second Language. In Stringfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E., and Zukowski, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. 1. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 111122.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Valenzuela, E. (2008). Eventive and Stative Passives in Spanish L2 Acquisition: A Matter of Aspect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 323336.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and White, L. (2002). L2 Acquisition of Spanish DPs: The Status of Grammatical Features. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Liceras, J. M. (eds.), The Acquisition of Spanish Morphosyntax: The L1/L2 Connection. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 153178.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1985). Morphology. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cadierno, T. (2000). The Acquisition of Spanish Grammatical Aspect by Danish Advanced Language Learners. Spanish Applied Linguistics, 4 (1), 153.Google Scholar
Camacho, J. (2012). Ser and Estar: The Individual/Stage-level Distinction and Aspectual Predication. In Hualde, J. I., Olarrea, A., and O’Rourke, E. (eds.), The Handbook of Hispanic Linguistics. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 453475.Google Scholar
Camps, J. (2002). Aspectual Distinctions in Spanish as a Foreign Language: The Early Stages of Oral Production. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 40 (3), 179210.Google Scholar
Collentine, J. and Asención‐Delaney, Y. (2010). A Corpus‐Based Analysis of the Discourse Functions of Ser/Estar + Adjective in Three Levels of Spanish as FL Learners. Language Learning, 60 (2), 409445.Google Scholar
Comajoan, L. (2014). Tense and Aspect in Second Language Spanish. In Geeslin, K. L. (ed.), The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 235252.Google Scholar
Comajoan, L. and Pérez Saldanya, M. (2005). Grammaticalization and Language Acquisition: Interaction of Lexical Aspect and Discourse. In Eddington, D. (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 4455.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Pires, A., and Rothman, J. (2013a). The Syntax-Semantics of Bare and Definite Plural Subjects in the L2 Spanish of English Natives. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17 (5), 634652.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., and Sánchez, L. (2013b). The Role of Semantic Transfer in Clitic-Drop among Chinese L1–Spanish L2 Bilinguals. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (1), 93125.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The Robustness of Critical Period Effects in Second Language Acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22 (4), 499533.Google Scholar
Demonte, V. (1999). A Minimal Account of Spanish Adjective Position and Interpretation. In Franco, J., Landa, A., and Martín, J. (eds.), Grammatical Analyses in Basque and Romance Linguistics: Papers in Honor of Mario Saltarelli. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 4576.Google Scholar
Demonte, V. (2008). Meaning-Form Correlations and Adjective Position in Spanish. In Kennedy, C. and McNally, L. (eds.), The Semantics of Adjectives and Adverbs. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 71100.Google Scholar
Dietrich, R., Klein, W., and Noyau, C. (1995). The Acquisition of Temporality in a Second Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Domínguez, L., Tracy-Ventura, N., Arche, M. J., Mitchell, R., and Myles, F. (2013). The Role of Dynamic Contrasts in the L2 Acquisition of Spanish Past Tense Morphology. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (3), 558577.Google Scholar
Dorado, D. (2011). Second Language Variation of Ser and Estar: A Comparative Analysis of Advanced Second Language Learners (Doctoral dissertation). University of Florida.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., and Gerfen, C. (2013). When Gender and Looking Go Hand in Hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (2), 353387.Google Scholar
Eubank, L. (1993). On the Transfer of Parametric Values in L2 Development. Language Acquisition, 3 (3), 182208.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Römer, U., and O’Donnell, M. B. (2016). Usage-Based Approaches to Language Acquisition and Processing: Cognitive and Corpus Investigations of Construction Grammar. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Farley, A. P. and McCollam, K. (2004). Learner Readiness and L2 Production in Spanish: Processability Theory on Trial. Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada, 22, 4769.Google Scholar
Fernández-García, M. (1999). Patterns of Gender Agreement in the Speech of Second Language Learners. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. and Martínez-Gil, F. (eds.), Advances in Hispanic Linguistics: Papers from the 2nd Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 315.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2002). Case and Φ-Feature Agreement in Advanced L2 Spanish Grammars. EUROSLA Yearbook, 2, 7186.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2005). Fossilized Second Language Grammars: The Acquisition of Grammatical Gender. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gabriele, A., Fiorentino, R., and Alemán Bañón, J. (2013). Examining Second Language Development Using Event-Related Potentials: A Cross-Sectional Study on the Processing of Gender and Number Agreement. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3 (2), 213232.Google Scholar
Gass, S. M. (1996). Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory: The Role of Language Transfer. In Ritchie, W. C. and Bhatia, T. K. (eds.), Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. London: Academic Press, pp. 317348.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (2003). A Comparison of Copula Choice: Native Spanish Speakers and Advanced Learners. Language Learning, 53 (4), 703764.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (ed.) (2014a). The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (2014b). The Acquisition of the Copula Contrast in Second Language Spanish. In Geeslin, K. L. (ed.), The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 219234.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., García-Amaya, L. J., Hasler-Barker, M., Henriksen, N., and Killam, J. (2010). The SLA of Direct Object Pronouns in a Study Abroad Immersion Environment where Use is Variable. In Borgonovo, C., Español-Echevarría, M., and Prévost, P. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 12th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 246259.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., García-Amaya, L. J., Hasler-Barker, M., Henriksen, N., and Killam, J. (2012). The L2 Acquisition of Variable Perfective Past Time Reference in Spanish in an Overseas Immersion Setting. In Geeslin, K. L. and Díaz-Campos, M. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 14th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 197213.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Gudmestad, A. (2008). Variable Subject Expression in Second-Language Spanish: A Comparison of Native and Non-Native Speakers. In Bowles, M., Foote, R., Perpiñán, S., and Bhatt, R., Selected Proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Research Forum. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6985.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Gudmestad, A. (2011). Using Sociolinguistic Analyses of Discourse-Level Features to Expand Research on L2 Variation in Forms of Spanish Subject Expression. In Plonsky, L. and Schierloh, M. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2009 Second Language Research Forum: Diverse Contributions to SLA. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 1630.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2006). A Longitudinal Study of Copula Choice: Following Development in Variable Structures. In Sagarra, N. and Toribio, A. J. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 144156.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2008). Variation in Contemporary Spanish: Linguistic Predictors of estar in Four Cases of Language Contact. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 365380.Google Scholar
Gillon Dowens, M., Guo, T., Guo, J., Barber, H. A., and Carreiras, M. (2011). Gender and Number Processing in Chinese Learners of Spanish – Evidence from Event Related Potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49 (7), 16511659.Google Scholar
Gillon Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., and Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic Processing in Late Second-Language Learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (8), 18701887.Google Scholar
Grice, P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L. (eds.), Syntax and Semantics III: Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press, pp. 4158.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., and Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical Gender in L2: A Production or a Real-Time Processing Problem? Second Language Research, 28 (2), 191215.Google Scholar
Guasti, M., Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Foppolo, F., Gualmini, A., and Meroni, L. (2005). Why Children and Adults Sometimes (but not Always) Compute Implicatures. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20 (5), 667696.Google Scholar
Gudmestad, A. (2012). Acquiring a Variable Structure: An Interlanguage Analysis of Second-Language Mood Use in Spanish. Language Learning, 62 (2), 373402.Google Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2012). The Acquisition of Interpretable Features in L2 Spanish: Personal a. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (4), 701720.Google Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2014). Adjectival Modification in L2 Spanish Noun Phrases. EUROSLA Yearbook, 14, 143172.Google Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Judy, T., and Rothman, J. (2009). On Transfer, Proficiency and Cross-Individual/Aggregate SLA Differences: Examining Adjectival Semantics in L2 Spanish. In Benati, A. (ed.), Issues in Second Language Proficiency. London: Continuum, pp. 233253.Google Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P. and Marinis, T. (2007). Acquiring Phenomena at the Syntax/Semantics Interface in L2 Spanish. EUROSLA Yearbook, 7, 6787.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. (2001). The Semantics of Spanish Plural Existential Determiners and the Dynamics of Judgment Types. Probus, 13 (1), 113154.Google Scholar
Halloran, B. and Rothman, J. (2015). The Acquisition of Clitics in L2 Spanish. In Smith, J. and Ihsane, T. (eds.), Romance Linguistics 2012: Selected Papers from the 42nd Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 316.Google Scholar
Hasbún, L. M. (1995). The Role of Lexical Aspect in the Acquisition of the Tense/Aspect System in L2 Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R. and Chan, C. Y.-H. (1997). The Partial Availability of Universal Grammar in Second Language Acquisition: The “Failed Functional Features Hypothesis.” Second Language Research, 13, 187226.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R. and Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the Acquisition and Non-Acquisition of Determiner–Noun Gender Concord in French and Spanish. In Prévost, P. and Paradis, J. (eds.), The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 175205.Google Scholar
Hengeveld, K. (2004). Illocution, Mood and Modality. In Booij, G., Lehmann, C., and Mugdan, J. (eds.), Morphology: An International Handbook on Inflection and Word-Formation, Vol. 2. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 11901201.Google Scholar
Herschensohn, J. and Young-Scholten, M. (eds.) (2013). The Cambridge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R. (1972). On the Semantic Properties of Logical Operators in English (Doctoral dissertation). UCLA.Google Scholar
Ionin, T., Montrul, S., and Crivos, M. (2013). A Bidirectional Study on the Acquisition of Plural Noun Phrase Interpretation in English and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34 (3), 483518.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. (2009). N-Drop at the Initial State of L3 Portuguese: Comparing Simultaneous and Additive Bilinguals of English/Spanish. In Pires, A. and Rothman, J. (eds.), Minimalist Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition: Case Studies across Portuguese. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 221244.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. (2010). Informing the Age of Acquisition Debate: L3 as a Litmus Test. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48 (2–3), 221243.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. and Rothman, J. (2015). Object Drop in L2 Spanish, (Complex) Feature Reassembly and L1 Pre-emption: Comparing English, Chinese, European and Brazilian Portuguese Learners. In Judy, T. and Perpiñán, S. (eds.), The Acquisition of Spanish in Understudied Language Pairings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 255280.Google Scholar
Judy, T., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., and Rothman, J. (2008). Adult Accessibility to L2 Representational Features: Evidence from the Spanish DP. In Bowles, M., Foote, R., Perpiñán, S., and Bhatt, R. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2nd Language Research Forum 2007. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 121.Google Scholar
Judy, T. and Perpiñán, S. (eds.) (2015). The Acquisition of Spanish in Understudied Language Pairings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Keating, G. D. (2009). Sensitivity to Violation of Gender Agreement in Native and Nonnative Spanish. Language Learning, 59 (3), 503535.Google Scholar
Keating, G. D. (2010). The Effects of Linear Distance and Working Memory on the Processing of Gender Agreement in Spanish. In VanPatten, B. and Jegersky, J. (eds.), Research in Second Language Processing and Parsing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 113134.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2009). Some Thoughts on the Contrastive Analysis of Features in Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 25 (2), 173227.Google Scholar
Lee, F. (2003). Anaphoric R-Expressions as Bound Variables. Syntax, 6 (1), 84114.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C. and Fernald, A. (2007). Young Children Learning Spanish Make Rapid Use of Grammatical Gender in Spoken Word Recognition. Psychological Science, 18 (3), 193198.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C. and Fernald, A. (2010). Real-Time Processing of Gender-Marked Articles by Native and Non-Native Spanish Speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (4), 447464.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M. (1985). The Value of Clitics in Non-Native Spanish. Second Language Research, 1 (2), 151168.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M. (1989). On Some Properties of the “Pro-Drop” Parameter: Looking for Missing Subjects in Non-Native Spanish. In Gass, S. and Schachter, J. (eds.), Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 109133.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M. and Díaz, L. (1999). Topic-Drop versus Pro-Drop: Null Subjects and Pronominal Subjects in the Spanish L2 of Chinese, English, French, German and Japanese Speakers. Second Language Research, 15 (1), 140.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Díaz, L., and Mongeon, C. (2000). N-Drop and Determiners in Native and Non-Native Spanish: More on the Role of Morphology in the Acquisition of Syntactic Knowledge. In Leow, R. P. and Sanz, C. (eds.), Spanish Applied Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium: Papers from the 1999 Conference on the L1 and L2 Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6796.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Fernández-Fuertes, R., Perales, S., Pérez-Tattam, R., and Spradlin, K. T. (2008). Gender and Gender Agreement in Bilingual Native and Non-Native Grammars: A View from Child and Adult Functional-Lexical Mixings. Lingua, 118 (6), 827851.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Maxwell, D., Laguardia, B., Fernández, Z., Fernández, R., and Díaz, L. (1997). A Longitudinal Study of Spanish Non-Native Grammars: Beyond Parameters. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Glass, W. R. (eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish, Vol. 1. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 99132.Google Scholar
Liskin-Gasparro, J. (2000). The Use of Tense-Aspect Morphology in Spanish Oral Narratives: Exploring the Perceptions of Advanced Learners. Hispania, 83 (4), 830844.Google Scholar
Long, D. and Rothman, J. (2013). Generative Approaches and the Competing Systems Hypothesis: Formal Acquisition to Practical Application. In. Schweiter, J. (ed.), Theoretical and Pedagogical Innovations in SLA and Bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 6383.Google Scholar
Long, M. (2007). Problems in SLA. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lopez, L. (2012). Indefinite Objects. Scrambling, Choice Functions and Differential Marking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lozano, C. (2002a). Knowledge of Expletive and Pronominal Subjects by Learners of Spanish. ITL Review of Applied Linguistics, 135, 3760.Google Scholar
Lozano, C. (2002b). The Interpretation of Overt and Null Pronouns in Non-Native Spanish. Durham Working Papers in Linguistics, 8, 5366.Google Scholar
Malovrh, P. A. (2008). A Multifaceted Analysis of the Interlanguage Development of Spanish Direct-Object Clitic Pronouns Observed in L2-Learner Production (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Malovrh, P. A. and Lee, J. F. (2010). Connections between Processing, Production and Placement. In VanPatten, B. and Jegersky, J. (eds.), Research in Second Language Processing and Parsing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 231256.Google Scholar
McCarthy, C. (2008). Morphological Variability in the Comprehension of Agreement: An Argument for Representation over Computation. Second Language Research, 24 (4), 459486.Google Scholar
Miller, D., Giancaspro, D., Iverson, M., Rothman, J., and Slabakova, R. (2016). Not Just algunos, but indeed unos L2ers Can Acquire Scalar Implicatures in L2 Spanish. In Alba de la Fuente, A., Valenzuela, E., and Martínez-Sanz, C. (eds.), Language Acquisition beyond Parameters: Studies in Honour of Juana M. Liceras. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 125145.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2002). Incomplete Acquisition and Attrition of Spanish Tense/Aspect Distinctions in Adult Bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (1), 3968.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Psycholinguistic Evidence for Split Intransitivity in Spanish L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 239267.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2010a). How Similar are L2 Learners and Heritage Speakers? Spanish Clitics and Word Order. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31 (1), 167207.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2010b). Dominant Language Transfer in Spanish L2 Learners and Heritage Speakers. Second Language Research, 26 (3), 293327.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2011). Morphological Errors in Spanish Second Language Learners and Heritage Speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33 (2), 155161.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Bruhn de Garavito, J. (1999). The L2 Acquisition of Spanish: Generative Perspectives. Second Language Research, 15 (2), 111114.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., de la Fuente, I., Davidson, J., and Foote, R. (2013). The Role of Experience in the Acquisition and Production of Diminutives and Gender in Spanish: Evidence from L2 Learners and Heritage Speakers. Second Language Research, 29, (1), 87118.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., and Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender Agreement in Adult Second Language Learners and Spanish Heritage Speakers: The Effects of Age and Context of Acquisition. Language Learning, 58 (3), 503553.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Gürel, A. (2015). The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking in Spanish by Turkish speakers. In Judy, T. and Perpiñán, S. (eds.), The Acquisition of Spanish in Understudied Language Pairings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 281308.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Ionin, T. (2012). Dominant Language Transfer in Spanish Heritage Speakers and L2 Learners in the Interpretation of Definite Articles. The Modern Language Journal, 96 (1), 7094.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Rodríguez Louro, C. (2006). Beyond the Syntax of the Null Subject Parameter. In Torrens, V. and Escobar, L. (eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax in Romance Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 401418.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Slabakova, R. (2003). Competence Similarities between Native and Nearnative Speakers: An Investigation of the Preterite/Imperfect Contrast in Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25 (3), 351398.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2009). Understanding Second Language Acquisition. London: Hodder.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2013). SLA for the 21st Century: Disciplinary Progress, Transdisciplinary Relevance, and the Bi/Multilingual Turn. Language Learning, 63 (Supplement 1), 124.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., and Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar Implicatures: Experiments at the Semantics–Pragmatics Interface. Cognition, 86, 253282.Google Scholar
Perales, S. and Liceras, J. M. (2010). Looking for Universals in the Acquisition of L2 Spanish Object Clitics. In Guijarro-Fuertes, P. and Domínguez, L. (eds.), New Directions in Language Acquisition: Romance Languages in the Generative Perspective. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 323355.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Glass, W. R. (1997). OPC Effects on the L2 Acquisition of Spanish. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Glass, W. R. (eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish, Vol. 1. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 149165.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. and Glass, W. R. (1999). Null Anaphora in Spanish Second Language Acquisition: Probabilistic versus Generative Approaches. Second Language Research, 15 (2), 220249.Google Scholar
Prévost, P. and White, L. (2000). Accounting for Morphological Variation in L2 Acquisition: Truncation or Missing Inflection? In Friedemann, M. A. and Rizzi, L. (eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax: Issues in Comparative Developmental Linguistics. London: Longman, pp. 232235.Google Scholar
Ramsay, V. (1990). Developmental Stages in the Acquisition of the Perfective and the Imperfective Aspects by Classroom L2 Learners of Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Roby, D. B. (2009). Aspect and the Categorization of States: The Case of ser and estar in Spanish. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural Categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328350.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2007). Pragmatic Solutions for Syntactic Problems: Understanding Some L2 Syntactic Errors in Terms of Pragmatic Deficits. In Baauw, S., Dirjkoningen, F., and Pinto, M. (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2005. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 299320.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2008). Aspect Selection in Adult L2 Spanish and the Competing Systems Hypothesis: When Pedagogical and Linguistic Rules Conflict. Languages in Contrast, 8 (1), 74106.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2009). Pragmatic Deficits with Syntactic Consequences?: L2 Pronominal Subjects and the Syntax–Pragmatics Interface. Journal of Pragmatics, 41 (5), 951973.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Iverson, M., and Judy, T. (2009). Noun-Raising and Adjectival Interpretative Reflexes in the L2 Spanish of Germanic and Italian Learners. In Chandlee, J., Franchini, M., Lord, S., and Rheiner, G.-M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 444455.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and Iverson, M. (2013). Islands and Objects in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (4), 589618.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Judy, T., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., and Pires, A. (2010). On the (Un)-Ambiguity of Adjectival Interpretations in L2 Spanish: Informing Debates on the Mental Representations of L2 Syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 4777.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N. and Herschensohn, J. (2010). The Role of Proficiency and Working Memory in Gender and Number Agreement Processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120 (8), 20222039.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N. and Herschensohn, J. (2011). Proficiency and Animacy Effects on L2 Gender Agreement Processes during Comprehension. Language Learning, 61 (1), 80116.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N. and Herschensohn, J. (2013). Processing of Gender and Number Agreement in Late Spanish Bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17 (5), 607627.Google Scholar
Salaberry, R. (1999). The Development of Past Tense Verbal Morphology in Classroom L2 Spanish. Applied Linguistics, 20 (2), 151178.Google Scholar
Salaberry, R. (2003). Tense Aspect in Verbal Morphology. Hispania, 86 (3), 559573.Google Scholar
Salaberry, R. (2005). Evidence for Transfer of Knowledge about Aspect from L2 Spanish to L3 Portuguese. In Ayoun, D. and Salaberry, M. R. (eds.), Tense and Aspect in the Romance Languages: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 179210.Google Scholar
Salaberry, R. (2008). Marking Past Tense in Second Language Acquisition: A Theoretical Model. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Salaberry, R. (2011). Assessing the Effect of Lexical Aspect and Grounding on the Acquisition of L2 Spanish Past Tense Morphology among L1 English Speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (2), 184202.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Alonso, S., Deo, A., and Piñango, M. (2016). Copular Variation in Spanish: Implications for the Study of Heritage Speaker Spanish. Paper presented at the Workshop on Heritage Language Acquisition, UiT–The Arctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. and Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 Cognitive States and the Full Transfer/Full Access Model. Second Language Research, 12 (1), 4072.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2006). Learnability in the Second Language Acquisition of Semantics: A Bidirectional Study of a Semantic Parameter. Second Language Research, 22 (4), 498523.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2010). Scalar Implicatures in Second Language Acquisition. Lingua, 120 (10), 24442462.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. and Montrul, S. (2003). Genericity and Aspect in L2 Acquisition. Language Acquisition, 11 (3), 165196.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. and Montrul, S. (2007). L2 Acquisition at the Grammar–Discourse Interface: Aspectual Shifts in L2 Spanish. In Liceras, J., Zobl, H., and Goodluck, H. (eds.), The Role of Formal Features in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 452483.Google Scholar
Snyder, W., Senghas, A., and Inman, K. (2001). Agreement Morphology and the Acquisition of N-Drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition, 9, 157173.Google Scholar
Torrego, E. (1998). The Dependencies of Objects. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. and Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The Interpretability Hypothesis: Evidence from wh-Interrogatives in Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 23 (2), 215242.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1985). The Acquisition of ser and estar by Adult Learners of Spanish: A Preliminary Investigation of Transitional Stages Of Competence. Hispania, 68 (2), 399406.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1987). Classroom Learners’ Acquisition of ser and estar: Accounting for Developmental Patterns. In VanPatten, B., Dvorak, T., and Lee, J. F. (eds.), Foreign Language Learning: A Research Perspective. Cambridge. MA: Newbury House, pp. 6175.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1990). Attending to Form and Content in the Input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12 (3), 287301.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional Infinitives, Head Movement and the Economy of Derivations. In Lightfoot, D. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Verb Movement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 305350.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., and Leung, Y. K.-I. (2004). Gender and Number Agreement in Nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (1), 105133.Google Scholar
Woolsey, D. (2008). From Theory to Research: Contextual Predictors of “estar + adjective” and the Study of the SLA of Spanish Copula Choice. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 277295.Google Scholar
Wulff, S., Ellis, N. C., Römer, U., Bardovi-Harlig, K., and LeBlanc, C. J. (2009). The Acquisition of Tense-Aspect: Converging Evidence from Corpora and Telicity Ratings. The Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 354369.Google Scholar
Zyzik, E. (2004). Encoding Meaning with Polyfunctional Forms: The Acquisition of Clitics in L2 Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). University of California at Davis.Google Scholar
Zyzik, E. (2006). Learners’ Overgeneralization of Dative Clitics to Accusative Contexts: Evidence for Prototype Effects in SLA. In Klee, C. A. and Face, T. L. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 122134.Google Scholar
Zyzik, E. (2008). Null Objects in Second Language Acquisition: Grammatical vs. Performance Models. Second Language Research, 24 (1), 65110.Google Scholar
Zyzik, E. and Gass, S. (2008). Epilogue: A Tale of Two Copulas. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 383385.Google Scholar

References

Aaron, J. (2006). Me voy a tener que ir yendo: A Corpus-Based Study of the Grammaticalization of the ir a + INF Construction in Spanish. In Sagarra, N. and Toribio, A. J. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 263272.Google Scholar
Adamson, H. D. and Regan, V. (1991). The Acquisition of Community Speech Norms by Asian Immigrants Learning English as a Second Language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 122.Google Scholar
Allen, H. (2010). Language-Learning Motivation during Short-Term Study Abroad: An Activity Theory Perspective. Foreign Language Annals, 43, 2749.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. W. (1984). The One-to-One Principle of Interlanguage Construction. Language Learning, 34, 7795.Google Scholar
Baayen, H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2004). The Emergence of Grammaticalized Future Expression in Longitudinal Production Data. In Overstreet, M., Rott, S., VanPatten, B., and Williams, J. (eds.), Form and Meaning in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 115137.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2015). One Functional Approach to SLA: The Concept-Oriented Approach. In VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction (2nd edn). New York: Routledge, pp. 5474.Google Scholar
Bayley, R. and Langman, J. (2004). Variation in the Group and the Individual: Evidence from Second Language Acquisition. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 42 (4), 303318.Google Scholar
Bayley, R. and Pease-Álvarez, L. (1997). Null Pronoun Variation in Mexican-Descent Children’s Narrative Discourse. Language Variation and Change 9 (3), 349371.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. (2004). Becoming Interculturally Competent. In Wurzel, J. (ed.), Toward Multiculturalism: A Reader in Multicultural Education (2nd edn). Newton, MA: Intercultural Resource, pp. 6277.Google Scholar
Blas Arroyo, J. L. (2008). The Variable Expression of Future Tense in Peninsular Spanish: The Present (and Future) of Inflectional Forms in the Spanish Spoken in a Bilingual Region. Language Variation and Change, 20, 85126.Google Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J., and Prévost, P. (2005). Acquisition of Mood Distinctions in L2 Spanish. In Burgos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., and Ha, S. (eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 97108.Google Scholar
Breiman, L. (2001). Random Forests. Machine Learning, 45, 532.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Valenzuela, E. (2006). The Status of ser and estar in Late and Early Bilingual L2 Spanish. In Klee, C. A. and Face, T. L. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 100109.Google Scholar
Cameron, R. (1995). The Scope and Limits of Switch Reference as a Constraint on Pronominal Subject Expression. Hispanic Linguistics, 6/7, 128.Google Scholar
Canale, M. and Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical Bases of Communicative Approaches to Second Language Teaching and Testing. Applied Linguistics, 1, 147.Google Scholar
Chambers, J. K. (2008). Sociolinguistic Theory (3rd edn). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D. and Shively, R. L. (2007). Acquisition of Requests and Apologies in Spanish and French: Impact of Study Abroad and Strategy-Building Intervention. Modern Language Journal, 91 (2), 189212.Google Scholar
Collentine, J. (1995). The Development of Complex Syntax and Mood Selection Abilities by Intermediate Level Learners of Spanish. Hispania, 78, 122135.Google Scholar
Corder, P. (1967). The Significance of Learners’ Errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 5 (4), 161170.Google Scholar
Cubillos, J. (2013). Community Engagement and Proficiency Gains in Short Term Study Abroad Programs. Northeast Conference on the Teaching of Foreign Languages Review, 71, 1736.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2010). Monitoring Processes in Spanish as a Second Language during a Study Abroad Program. Foreign Language Annals, 43, 8092.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. (ed.) (2011). The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. and Geeslin, K. L. (2011). Copula Use in the Spanish of Venezuela: Social and Linguistic Sources of Variation in Spanish. Spanish in Context, 8, 7394.Google Scholar
Dietrich, R., Klein, W., and Noyau, C. (1995). The Acquisition of Temporality in a Second Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dumont, J. (2013). Another Look at the Present Perfect in an Andean Variety of Spanish: Grammaticalization and Evidentiality in Quiteño Spanish. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Lord, G., de Prada Pérez, A., and Aaron, J. E. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 16th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 279291.Google Scholar
Eckert, P. (2012). Three Waves of Variation Study: The Emergence of Meaning in the Study of Sociolinguistic Variation. The Annual Review of Anthropology, 41, 87100.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., and Wulff, S. (2015). Usage-Based Approaches to SLA. In VanPatten, B. and Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction (2nd edn). New York: Routledge, pp. 7593.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Planning and Task-Based Research: Theory and Research. In Ellis, R. (ed.), Planning and Task-Performance in a Second Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 334.Google Scholar
Escobar, A. M. (1997a). Contrastive and Innovative Uses of the Present Perfect and the Preterite in Spanish in Contact with Quechua. Hispania, 80, 859870.Google Scholar
Escobar, A. M. (1997b). From Time to Modality in Spanish in Contact with Quechua. Hispanic Linguistics, 9 (1), 6499.Google Scholar
Español-Echevarría, M. and Prévost, P. (2004). Acquiring Number Specification on L2 Spanish Quantifiers: Evidence against the Rich Agreement Hypothesis. In van Kampen, J. and Baauw, S. (eds.), Proceedings of the 2003 Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition. Utrecht: LOT, pp. 151162.Google Scholar
Fafulas, S. (2012). Nuevas perspectivas sobre la variación de las formas presente simple y presente progresivo en español y en inglés. Spanish in Context, 9, 5887.Google Scholar
Fafulas, S. (2015). Progressive Constructions in Native-Speaker and Adult-Acquired Spanish. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 8 (1), 85133.Google Scholar
Fafulas, S., and Díaz-Campos, M. (2010). Variación morfosintáctica y lenguas en contacto: Las formas analíticas y sintéticas del presente progresivo en el español monolingüe y bilingüe. Boletín de Filología, 45 (2), 7189.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. and Hasler Barker, M. (2015). Complimenting in Spanish in a Short-Term Study Abroad Context. System, 48, 7585.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. and Koike, D. (2014). Perspectives on Spanish SLA from Pragmatics and Discourse. In Lacorte, M. (ed.), Handbook of Hispanic Applied Linguistics. New York: Routledge, pp. 2543.Google Scholar
Fernández Ordóñez, I. (1999). Leísmo, laísmo y loísmo. In Bosque, I. and Demonte, V. (eds.), Gramática descriptiva de la lengua española. Madrid: Espasa Calpe, pp. 13171398.Google Scholar
Flores-Ferrán, N. (2002). A Sociolinguistic Perspective on the Use of Subject Personal Pronouns in Spanish Narratives of Puerto Ricans in New York City. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Freed, B. (1990). Language Learning in a Study Abroad Context: The Effects of Interactive and Non-Interactive Out-of-Class Contact on Grammatical Achievement and Oral Proficiency. In Alatis, J. (ed.), Linguistics, Language Teaching and Language Acquisition: The Interdependence of Theory, Practice and Research. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 459477.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (2000). A New Approach to the Second Language Acquisition of Copula Choice in Spanish. In Leow, R. and Sanz, C. (eds.), Spanish Applied Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium: Papers from the 1999 Conference on the L1 and L2 Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 5066.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. (2003). A Comparison of Copula Choice in Advanced and Native Spanish. Language Learning, 53, 703764.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Fafulas, S. (2012). Variation of the Simple Present and Present Progressive Forms: A Comparison of Native and Non-Native Speakers. In Geeslin, K. and Diaz-Campos, M. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 14th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 179196.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., Fafulas, S., and Kanwit, M. (2013a). Acquiring Geographically-Variable Norms of Use: The Case of the Present Perfect in Mexico and Spain. In Howe, C., Blackwell, S., and Quesada, M. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 205220.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Gudmestad, A. (2008a). Comparing Interview and Written Elicitation Tasks in Native and Non-Native Data: Do Speakers Do what we Think they Do? In Bruhn de Garavito, J. and Valenzuela, E. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6477.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Gudmestad, A. (2008b). Variable Subject Expression in Second-Language Spanish: A Comparison of Native and Non-Native Speakers. In Bowles, M., Foote, R., Perpiñán, S., and Bhatt, R. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Research Forum. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6985.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Gudmestad, A. (2010). An Exploration of the Range and Frequency of Occurrence of Forms in Potentially-Variable Structures in Second Language Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 433463.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2006). Second Language Acquisition of Variable Structures in Spanish by Portuguese Speakers. Language Learning, 51, 53107.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., Linford, B., Fafulas, S., Long, A. Y., and Díaz-Campos, M. (2013b). The L2 Development of Subject Form Variation in Spanish: The Individual vs. the Group. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Lord, G., de Prada Pérez, A., and Aaron, J. E. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 16th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 156174.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L. and Long, A. Y. (2015). The Development and Use of the Spanish Copula with Adjectives by Korean-Speaking Learners. In Pérez-Jiménez, I., Leonetti, M., and Gumiel Molina, S. (eds.), New Perspectives on the Study of ser and estar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 293323,Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
González-Lloret, M. (2011). Conversation Analysis of Computer-mediated Communication. CALICO Journal, 28 (2), 308325.Google Scholar
Gries, S. T. (2009). Statistics for Linguistics with R. New York: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Gries, S. T. and Wulff, S. (2005). Do Foreign Language Learners also Have Constructions? Evidence from Priming, Sorting, and Corpora. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3, 182200.Google Scholar
Gudmestad, A. (2006). L2 Variation and the Spanish Subjunctive: Linguistic Features Predicting Mood Selection. In Face, T. L. and Klee, C. L. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 170184.Google Scholar
Gudmestad, A. (2012). Acquiring a Variable Structure: An Interlanguage Analysis of Second Language Mood Use in Spanish. Language Learning, 62, 373402.Google Scholar
Gudmestad, A. and Geeslin, K. (2011). Assessing the Use of Multiple Forms in Variable Contexts: The Relationship between Linguistic Factors and Future-Time Reference in Spanish. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 4, 333.Google Scholar
Gudmestad, A. and Geeslin, K. (2013). Second-Language Development of Variable Forms of Future-Time Expression in Spanish. In Carvalho, A. M. and Beaudrie, S. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Spanish Sociolinguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 6375.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez, M. (1992). The Extension of estar: A Linguistic Change in Progress in the Spanish of Morelia, Mexico. Hispanic Linguistics, 5, 109141.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez, M. (1995). On the Future of the Future Tense in the Spanish of the Southwest. In Silva-Corvalán, C. (ed.), Spanish in Four Continents: Studies in Language Contact and Bilingualism. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 214223.Google Scholar
Howe, C. and Schwenter, S. (2003). Present Perfect for Preterit across Spanish Dialects. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, 9, 6175.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. E. (2009). Getting off the GoldVarb Standard: Introducing Rbrul for Mixed Effects Variable Rule Analysis. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3 (1), 359383.Google Scholar
Judy, T. and Perpiñán, S. (eds.) (2015). The Acquisition of Spanish in Understudied Language Pairings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. (2014). The Acquisition of Future Expression in L2 Spanish (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. (2017). What we Gain by Combining Variationist and Concept-Oriented Approaches: The Case of Acquiring Spanish Future-Time Expression. Language Learning, 67 (2), 461498.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. (in press). Encantado de conocerte virtualmente: Native/Non-Native Speaker Electronic Chats in Spanish. International Journal of the Linguistic Association of the Southwest, 33 (2).Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. and Geeslin, K. L. (2014). The Interpretation of Spanish Subjunctive and Indicative Forms in Adverbial Clauses. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 487533.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. and Geeslin, K. L. (2017). Exploring Lexical Effects in Second Language Interpretation: The Case of Mood in Spanish Adverbial Clauses. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, FirstView, 125.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M., Geeslin, K. L., and Fafulas, S. (2015). Study Abroad and the SLA of Variable Structures: A Look at the Present Perfect, the Copula Contrast, and the Present Progressive in Mexico and Spain. Probus, 27 (2), 307348.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M. and Solon, M. (2013). Acquiring Variation in Future-Time Expression Abroad in Valencia, Spain and Mérida, Mexico. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Lord, G., de Prada Pérez, A., and Aaron, J. E. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 16th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 206221.Google Scholar
Klee, C. A. and Lynch, J. (2009). El español en contacto con otras lenguas. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Klein-Andreu, F. (1999). Variación actual y reinterpretación histórica: le/s, la/s, lo/s en Castilla. In Serrano, M. J. (ed.), Estudios de variación sintáctica. Frankfurt and Madrid: Vervuert, pp. 197220.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1966). The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Lafford, B. A. and Uscinski, I. (2013). Study Abroad and Second Language Spanish. In Geeslin, K. L. (ed.), The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 386403.Google Scholar
Larson-Hall, J. and Plonsky, L. (2015). Reporting and Interpreting Quantitative Research Findings: What Gets Reported and Recommendations for the Field. Language Learning, 65, Supp. 1, 127159.Google Scholar
Linford, B. and Shin, N. L. (2013). Lexical Frequency Effects on L2 Spanish Subject Pronoun Expression. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Lord, G., de Prada Pérez, A., and Aaron, J. E. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 16th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 175189.Google Scholar
Linford, B., Solon, M., Long, A. Y., Whatley, M., and Geeslin, K. (2016). Lexical Frequency and Subject Expression in Native and Non-Native Spanish: A Closer Look at Independent and Mediating Effects. In Sessarego, S. and Tejedo-Herrero, F. (eds.), Spanish Language and Sociolinguistic Analysis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 197216.Google Scholar
Lope Blanch, J. M. (1972). Estudios sobre el español de México. Mexico City: UNAM.Google Scholar
Malovrh, P. (2006). L2 Sentence Processing of Spanish OVS Word Order and Direct Object Pronouns: An Analysis of Contextual Constraints. In Sagarra, N. and Toribio, A. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 169179.Google Scholar
Malovrh, P. (2014). Variability and Systematicity in Interlanguage Development: An Analysis of Mode and its Effect on L2 Spanish Morphology. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 7 (1), 4378.Google Scholar
Mayberry, M. (2011). Synchronous Narratives in Spanish: The Simple Present/Present Progressive Aspectual Contrast. Hispania, 94, 462482.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (1987). Reference to Past Events and Actions in the Development of Natural Language Acquisition. In Pfaff, C. W. (ed.), First and Second Language Acquisition Processes. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House, pp. 206224.Google Scholar
Montes Giraldo, J. (1962). Sobre la categoría de futuro en el español de Colombia. Boletín del Instituto Caro y Cuervo, 16, 527555.Google Scholar
Moses, J. (2002). The Expression of Futurity by English-Speaking Learners of French (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Orozco, R. (2005). Distribution of Future Tense Forms in Northern Colombian Spanish. In Eddington, D. (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 5665.Google Scholar
Ortiz-López, L. A. (2000). Extensión de estar en contextos de ser en el español de Puerto Rico: ¿Evolución interna o contacto de lenguas? Boletín de la Academia Puertorriqueña de la Lengua Española, 98118.Google Scholar
Otheguy, R. and Zentella, A. C. (2012). Spanish in New York: Language Contact, Dialectal Leveling, and Structural Continuity. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrino Aveni, V. (2005). Study Abroad and Second Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Preston, D. (1993). Variationist Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 9, 153172.Google Scholar
Preston, D. (2000). Three Kinds of Sociolinguistics and SLA: A Psycholinguistic Perspective. In Swierzbin, B. et al. (eds.), Social and Cognitive Factors in SLA. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 330.Google Scholar
Quesada, M. L. (1998). L2 Acquisition of the Spanish Subjunctive Mood and Prototype Schema Development. Spanish Applied Linguistics, 2, 123.Google Scholar
Quesada, M. L. and Blackwell, S. E. (2009). The L2 Acquisition of Null and Overt Spanish Subject Pronouns: A Pragmatic Approach. In Collentine, J., García, M., Lafford, B., and Marcos Martín, F. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 117130.Google Scholar
Ranson, D. L. (1991). Person Marking in the Wake of /s/ Deletion in Andalusian Spanish. Language Variation and Change, 3, 133152.Google Scholar
Regan, V. (1995). The Acquisition of Sociolinguistic Native Speech Norms. In Freed, B. (ed.). Second Language Acquisition in a Study Abroad Context. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 245267.Google Scholar
Regan, V. (2004). The Relationship between the Group and the Individual and the Acquisition of Native Speaker Variation Patterns: A Preliminary Study. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 42 (4), 335348.Google Scholar
Rehner, K. (2002). The Development of Aspects of Linguistic and Discourse Competence by Advanced Second Language Learners of French (Doctoral dissertation). University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Rehner, K., Mougeon, R., and Nadasdi, T. (2003). The Learning of Sociolinguistic Variation by Advanced FSL Learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 127156.Google Scholar
Rodríguez Louro, C. (2009). Perfect Evolution and Change: A Study of Preterit and Present Perfect Usage in Contemporary and Earlier Argentina (Doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Salgado-Robles, F. (2014a). Desarrollo de la competencia estratégica oral en español como segunda lengua mediante el Aprendizaje-Servicio en el Bluegrass de Mexington. Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada, 59, 125149.Google Scholar
Salgado-Robles, F. (2014b). Variación dialectal por aprendientes de español en un contexto de inmersión en el extranjero: Un análisis cuantitativo del uso leísta en el discurso oral y escrito. Lenguas Modernas, 43, 97112.Google Scholar
Sankoff, D., Tagliamonte, S., and Smith, E. (2005). GoldVarb X: A Variable Rule Application for Macintosh and Windows. Available from http://individual.utoronto.ca/tagliamonte/goldvarb.html (last access December 24, 2017).Google Scholar
Schwenter, S. A. (2006). Null Objects across South America. In Face, T. L. and Klee, C. A. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 8th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 2336.Google Scholar
Schwenter, S. A. and Torres Cacoullos, R. (2008). Defaults and Indeterminacy in Temporal Grammaticalization: The “Perfect” Road to Perfective. Language Variation and Change, 20, 139.Google Scholar
Sedano, M. (1994). El futuro morfológico y la expresión ir a + infinitivo en el español hablado de Venezuela. Verba: Anuario Galego de Filoloxia, 21, 225240.Google Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10, 209231.Google Scholar
Shin, N. L. and Otheguy, R. (2013). Social Class and Gender Impacting Change in Bilingual Settings: Spanish Subject Pronoun Use in New York. Language in Society, 42, 429452.Google Scholar
Shirai, Y. (1995). Tense-Aspect Marking by L2 Learners of Japanese. In MacLaughlin, D. and McEwen, S. (eds.), Proceedings of 19th Annual Boston University Conference in Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 575586.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. (2015). Developing Interactional Competence during Study Abroad: Listener Responses in L2 Spanish. System, 48, 8698.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language Contact and Change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. and Terrell, T. (1989). Notas sobre la expresión de futuridad en el español del Caribe. Hispanic Linguistics, 2, 191208.Google Scholar
Solon, M. and Kanwit, M. (2014). The Emergence of Future Verbal Morphology in Spanish as a Foreign Language. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 7 (1), 115148.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, S. (2012). Variationist Sociolinguistics: Change, Observation, Interpretation. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tarone, E. (1983). On the Variability of Interlanguage Systems. Applied Linguistics, 4 (2), 142163.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R. (2000). Grammaticization, Synchronic Variation, and Language Contact: A Study of Spanish Progressive -ndo Constructions. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
von Stutterheim, C. and Klein, W. (1987). A Concept-Oriented Approach to Second Language Studies. In Pfaff, C. W. (ed.), First and Second Language Acquisition Processes. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House, pp. 191205.Google Scholar
Walker, J. (2010). Variation in Linguistic Systems. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Woolsey, D. (2008). From Theory to Research: Contextual Predictors of estar + adjective and the Study of SLA of Spanish Copula Choice. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (3), 277296.Google Scholar
Young, D. J. (2013). Affective Factors and Second Language Spanish. In Geeslin, K. L. (ed.), The Handbook of Spanish Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 369385.Google Scholar
Zentella, A. C. (1997). Growing up Bilingual: Puerto Rican Children in New York. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar

References

Bardel, C. and Falk, Y. (2007). The Role of the Second Language in Third Language Acquisition: The Case of Germanic Syntax. Second Language Research, 23, 459484.Google Scholar
Bentahila, A. (1975). The Influence of the L2 on the Learning of the L3 (MA thesis). Bangor University.Google Scholar
Berkes, É. and Flynn, S. (2012). Further Evidence in Support of the Cumulative-Enhancement Model: CP Structure Development. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., and Rothman, J.(eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 143164.Google Scholar
Bild, E. R. and Swain, M. (1989). Minority Language Students in a French Immersion Programme: Their French Proficiency. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 10 (3), 255274.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2012). L3 Phonology: An Understudied Domain. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., and Rothman, J.(eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3360.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2013a). The Phonological Permeability Hypothesis: Measuring Regressive L3 Influence to Test L1 and L2 Phonological Representations (Doctoral Dissertation). University of Florida.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2013b). Methodological Issues in L3 Phonological Acquisition Research. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 6 (1).Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2015). Does the Source of Transfer Affect the Rate of L3 Morphosyntactic Development? Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, USA.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017a). Testing the Phonological Permeability Hypothesis: L3 Phonological Effects on L1 versus L2 Systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 698717. doi: 1367006916637287.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017b). Regressive Transfer in L3 Syntactic Development. In Hahn, A. and Angelovska, T. (eds.), L3 Syntactic Transfer: Models, New Developments and Implications. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 173194.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Amaro, J. F., and Rothman, J. (2015). The Relationship between L3 Transfer and Structural Similarity across Development: Raising across an Experiencer in Brazilian Portuguese. In Peukert, H. (ed.), Transfer Effects in Multilingual Language Development. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 2152.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Iverson, M., Giancaspro, D., and Halloran González, B. (in press). Implications of L1 versus L2 Transfer in L3 Rate of Acquisition. In Molsing, K., Becker Lopes Perna, C., and Tramunto Ibaños, A. M. (eds.), Linguistic Approaches to Portuguese as an Additional Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. and Rothman, J. (2010). On L3 Acquisition and Phonological Permeability: A New Test Case for Debates on the Mental Representation of Non-Native Phonological Systems. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48, 275296.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. and Wrembel, M. (2016). Investigating the Acquisition of Phonology in a Third Language – a State of the Science and an Outlook for the Future. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13 (4), 395409.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J. (2003). The Additive Effect of Bilingualism on Third Language Acquisition: A Review. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7 (1), 7187.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J. and Valencia, J. F. (1994). Additive Trilingualism: Evidence from the Basque Country. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15 (2), 195207.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., Conway, C. M., and Onnis, L. (2012). Similar Neural Correlates for Language and Sequential Learning: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27 (2), 231256.Google Scholar
Chumbow, B. S. (1981). The Mother Tongue Hypothesis in a Multilingual Setting. In Savard, J. G. and Laforge, L. (eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Congress of the International Association of Applied Linguistics. Quebec: Les Presses de l’Université Laval, pp. 367388.Google Scholar
de Angelis, G. (2007). Third or Additional Language Acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M. (1998). Lexical Inventions: French Interlanguage as L2 versus L3. Applied Linguistics, 19 (4), 471490.Google Scholar
Falk, Y. and Bardel, C. (2011). Object Pronouns in German L3 Syntax: Evidence for the L2 Status Factor. Second Language Research, 27, 5982.Google Scholar
Fouser, R. (1995). Problems and Prospects in Third Language Acquisition Research. Language Research, 31 (2), 387414.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K., and Pfeifer, E. (2002). Brain Signatures of Artificial Language Processing: Evidence Challenging the Critical Period Hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 (1), 529534.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., and Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The Cumulative-Enhancement Model for Language Acquisition: Comparing Adults’ and Children’s Patterns of Development in First, Second and Third Language Acquisition of Relative Clauses. The International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 316.Google Scholar
García-Mayo, M. del P. (2012). Cognitive Approaches to L3 Acquisition. International Journal of English Studies, 12 (1), 129146.Google Scholar
García-Mayo, M. del P. and Rothman, J. (2012). L3 Morphosyntax in the Generative Tradition: The Initial Stages and beyond. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., and Rothman, J.(eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 932.Google Scholar
García-Mayo, M. del P. and Villarreal Olaizola, I. (2011). The Development of Suppletive and Affixal Tense and Agreement Morphemes in the L3 English of Basque–Spanish Bilinguals. Second Language Research, 27 (1), 129149.Google Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B., and Iverson, M. (2015). Transfer at the Initial Stages of L3 Brazilian Portuguese: A Look at Three Groups of English/Spanish Bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 191207.Google Scholar
Hammarberg, B. (2001). Roles of L1 and L2 in L3 Production and Acquisition. In Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., and Jessner, U. (eds.), Cross-Linguistic Influence in Third Language Acquisition: Psycholinguistic Perspectives. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, pp. 2141.Google Scholar
Hermas, A. (2010). Language Acquisition as Computational Resetting: Verb Movement in L3 Initial State. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7, 343362.Google Scholar
Hermas, A. (2014). Multilingual Transfer: L1 Morphosyntax in L3 English. International Journal of Language Studies, 8.Google Scholar
Hermas, A. (2015). The Categorization of the Relative Complementizer Phrase in Third-Language English: A Feature Re-Assembly Account. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19 (5), 587607.Google Scholar
Hui, B. (2010). Backward Transfer from L3 French to L2 English Production of Relative Clauses by L1 Cantonese Speakers in Hong Kong. Hong Kong Journal of Applied Linguistics, 12 (2), 4560.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. (2009). N-Drop at the Initial State of L3 Portuguese: Comparing Simultaneous and Additive Bilinguals of English/Spanish. In Pires, A. and Rothman, J. (eds.), Minimalist Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition: Case Studies across Portuguese. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 221244.Google Scholar
Jin, F. (2009). Third Language Acquisition of Norwegian Objects: Interlanguage Transfer or L1 Influence? In Leung, Y.-K. I. (ed.), Third Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, pp. 144161.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1983). Now You See It, Now You Don’t. In Gass, S. and Selinker, L. (eds.), Language Transfer in Language Learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 112134.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1986). An Eye for an Eye: Crosslinguistic Constraints on the Development of the L2 Lexicon. In Kellerman, E. and Smith, M. S. (eds.), Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition. New York: Pergamon, pp. 3548.Google Scholar
Kopečková, R. (2016). The Bilingual Advantage in L3 Learning: A Developmental Study of Rhotic Sounds. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13 (4), 410425.Google Scholar
Kulundary, V. and Gabriele, A. (2012). Examining the Role of L2 Syntactic Development in L3 Acquisition: A Look at Relative Clauses. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., and Rothman, J. (eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 195222.Google Scholar
Llama, R., Cardoso, W., and L., C. (2010). The Influence of Language Distance and Language Status on the Acquisition of L3 Phonology. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7, 3957.Google Scholar
Llama, R. and López-Morelos, L. P. (2016). VOT Production by Spanish Heritage Speakers in a Trilingual Context. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13 (4), 444458.Google Scholar
Llinàs-Grau, M. and Mayenco, E. P. (2016). Regressive Transfer from L4 German to L3 English: The Case of that-Deletion. In Ibarrola-Armendariz, A. and Ortiz de Urbina Arruabarrena, J. (eds.), Glancing Backwards to Build a Future in English Studies. Bilbao: University of Deusto, pp. 281288.Google Scholar
Llisterri, J. and Poch-Olivé, D. (1987). Phonetic Interference in Bilingual’s Learning of a Third Language. In Gamqrelidze, Tamaz et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the 11th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Tallinn: Academy of Sciences of the Estonian SSR, pp. 137147.Google Scholar
Lloyd-Smith, A., Gyllstad, H., and Kupisch, T. (2017). Transfer into L3 English: Global Accent in German-Dominant Heritage Speakers of Turkish. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7 (2), 131162.Google Scholar
Lozano, C. (2002). The Interpretation of Overt and Null Pronouns in Non-Native Spanish. In Marsden, H., Pourcel, S., and Whong-Bharr, M. (eds.), Durham Working Papers in Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 5366.Google Scholar
Matthews, S. J., Cheung, S. C., and Tsang, W. L. (2014). Anti-Transfer Effects in Third Language Acquisition. Paper presented at the 9th International Conference on Third Language Acquisition and Multilingualism, Uppsala, Sweden.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (1983). Transfer as a Second Language Strategy. Language and Communication, 3, 1146.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., and Santos, H. (2011). Clitics and Object Expression in the L3 Acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese: Structural Similarity Matters for Transfer. Second Language Research, 27, 2158.Google Scholar
Mykhaylyk, R., Mitrofanova, N., Rodina, Y., and Westergaard, M. (2015). The Linguistic Proximity Model: The Case of Verb-Second Revisited. In Grillo, E. and Jepson, K. (eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla, pp. 337349.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). The Acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic Development in Monolingual and Bilingual L1 Acquisition and Adult L2 Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. A. (2008). Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-Examining the Age Factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Onnis, L. (2012). Artificial Language Learning. In Binson, P. J. (ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Second Language Acquisition. New York: Routledge, pp. 3541.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2004). The Relevance of Specific Language Impairment in Understanding the Role of Transfer in Second Language Acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 6782.Google Scholar
Rabinovitch, M. S. and Parver, L. M. (1966). Auditory Discrimination in Monolinguals and Polyglots. Paper Presented at Meeting of Canadian Psychological Association, Montreal.Google Scholar
Ringbom, H. (1987). The Role of First Language in Foreign Language Acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ringbom, H. (2001). Lexical Transfer in L3 Production. In Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., and Jessner, U. (eds.), Cross-Linguistic Influence in Third Language Acquisition: Psycholinguistic Perspectives. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, pp. 5968.Google Scholar
Rivers, W. M. (1979). Learning a Sixth Language: An Adult Learner’s Daily Diary. Canadian Modern Language Review. La Revue Canadienne des Langues Vivantes, 36 (1), 6782.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2010). On The Typological Economy of Syntactic Transfer: Word Order and Relative Clause High/Low Attachment Preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Teaching, 48 (2/3), 245273.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 Syntactic Transfer Selectivity and Typological Determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27, 107127.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2013). Cognitive Economy, Non-Redundancy and Typological Primacy in L3 Acquisition: Evidence from Initial Stages of L3 Romance. In Baauw, S., Dirjkoningen, F., and Pinto, M. (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2011. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 217248.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2015). Linguistic and Cognitive Motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of Third Language (L3) Transfer: Timing of Acquisition and Proficiency Considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 179190.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Alemán Bañón, J., and Alonso, J. G. (2015). Neurolinguistic Measures of Typological Effects in Multilingual Transfer: Introducing an ERP Methodology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 114.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2010). What Variables Condition Syntactic Transfer? A Look at the L3 Initial State. Second Language Research, 26, 198218.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and Slabakova, R. (2017). The Generative Approach to SLA and its Place in Modern Second Language Studies. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. doi: 10.1017/S0272263117000134.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Zarobe, Y. (2005). Age and Third Language Production: A Longitudinal Study. International Journal of Multilingualism, 2 (2), 105112.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Zarobe, Y. (2008). CLIL and Foreign Language Learning: A Longitudinal Study in the Basque Country. International Content and Language Integrated Learning Research Journal, 1 (1), 6073.Google Scholar
Sánchez, L. (2015). L2 Activation and Blending in Third Language Acquisition: Evidence of Crosslinguistic Influence from the L2 in a Longitudinal Study on the Acquisition of L3 English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (2), 252269.Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Park, H. I., and Lado, B. (2015). A Functional Approach to Cross-Linguistic Influence in ab initio L3 Acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (2), 236251.Google Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2016). First Language Attrition. Language Teaching, 49 (2), 186212.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. and Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 Cognitive States and the Full Transfer/Full Access Model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.Google Scholar
Singh, R. and Carroll, S. (1979). L1, L2 and L3. Indian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5, 5163.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (1987). Mother and Other Tongue Influence on Learner French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 9 (3), 327345.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2017). The Scalpel Model of Third Language Acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 651665. doi: 1367006916655413.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. and García-Mayo, M. del P. (2015). The L3 Syntax–Discourse Interface. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 208226.Google Scholar
Stedje, A. (1977). Tredjespråksinterferens i fritt tal-en jämförande studie. In Palmberg, R. and Ringbom, H. (eds.), Papers from the Conference on Contrastive Linguistics and Error Analysis. Åbo: Åbo Akademi, pp. 141158.Google Scholar
Sypiańska, J. (2016). Multilingual Acquisition of Vowels in L1 Polish, L2 Danish and L3 English. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13 (4), 476495.Google Scholar
Tsang, W. L. (2015). Acquisition of English Number Agreement: L1 Cantonese–L2 English–L3 French Speakers versus L1 Cantonese–L2 English speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism. doi: 1367006915576398.Google Scholar
Vildomec, V. (1963). Multilingualism: General Linguistics and Psychology of Speech. Leiden: A. W. Sythoff.Google Scholar
Vogel, T. (1992). “Englisch und Deutsch gibt es immer Krieg”: Sprachverarbeitungsprozesse beim Erwerb des Deutschen als Drittsprache. Zielsprache Deutsch, 23 (2), 9599.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., and Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic Influence in the Acquisition of a Third Language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 666682. doi: 1367006916648859.Google Scholar
Williams, S. and Hammarberg, B. (1998). Language Switches in L3 Production: Implications for a Polyglot Speaking Model. Applied Linguistics, 19, 295333.Google Scholar
Wrembel, M. (2014). VOT Patterns in the Acquisition of L3 Phonology. Concordia Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, 5, 750770.Google Scholar
Wrembel, M. (2015). In Search of a New Perspective: Cross-linguistic Influence in the Acquisition of Third Language Phonology. Poznan: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×