Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T17:31:11.674Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Symptom perception

from Psychology, health and illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2014

Elizabeth Broadbent
Affiliation:
The University of Auckland
Keith J. Petrie
Affiliation:
The University of Auckland
Susan Ayers
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Andrew Baum
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Chris McManus
Affiliation:
St Mary's Hospital Medical School
Stanton Newman
Affiliation:
University College and Middlesex School of Medicine
Kenneth Wallston
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University School of Nursing
John Weinman
Affiliation:
United Medical and Dental Schools of Guy's and St Thomas's
Robert West
Affiliation:
St George's Hospital Medical School, University of London
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Experiencing physical symptoms is very common. General population surveys show most people experience some form of symptom like headaches, aching joints and muscles or upper respiratory complaints two to three times a week (Dunnell & Cartwright, 1972; Hannay, 1978; Kroenke & Spitzer, 1998). Studies also show that rates of symptom reporting differ across various demographic groups. For instance, females generally report more symptoms than males, unemployed people more than employed, people living alone more than people living with a few others (Pennebaker & Epstein, 1983). These findings suggest that there is more to the reporting of symptoms than just underlying changes in physiology.

Understanding the factors that influence the perception of symptoms is important because symptom reporting forms such a central part of most medical encounters and diagnosis. The (mis)perception of symptoms typically forms the basis of delay in seeking treatment, the overuse of medical services and the inappropriate use of medication. Symptom perception is also important in determining responses in chronic illnesses. People with diabetes or asthma, for example, must regularly manage their illness by monitoring their symptoms and responding with self-medication and behavioural responses. This chapter explores the psychological processes involved in the perception and reporting of physical symptoms.

How accurately do people perceive symptoms?

People are generally accurate when perceiving extreme symptoms or bodily states that need attention immediately. For example, individuals can almost always tell when they need sleep, need to eat or to urinate.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S. & Higgins, P. (1992). Neuroticism and the pain mood relation in rheumatoid arthritis: insights from a prospective daily study. Journal of ConsultingandClinical Psychology, 60, 119–26.Google Scholar
Banks, M. H., Beresford, S. A., Morrell, D. C., Waller, J. J. & Watkins, C. J. (1975). Factors influencing demand for primary medical care in women aged 20–44 years: a preliminary report. International Journal of Epidemiology, 4, 189–95.Google Scholar
Bartholomew, R. E. (1994). Tarantism, dancing mania and demonopathy: The anthro-political aspects of “mass psychogenic illness.”Psychological Medicine, 24, 281–306.Google Scholar
Baumann, L. J. & Leventhal, H. (1985). “I can tell when my blood pressure is up, can't I?”Health Psychology, 4, 203–18.Google Scholar
Bleichhardt, G., Timmer, B. & Rief, W. (2004). Cognitive–behavioural therapy for patients with multiple somatoform symptoms – a randomised controlled trial in tertiary care. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56, 449–54.Google Scholar
Brondolo, E., Rosen, R. C., Kostis, J. B. & Schwartz, J. E. (1999). Relationship of physical symptoms and mood to perceived and actual blood pressure in hypertensive men: a repeated-measures design. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 311–18.Google Scholar
Chalder, T., Power, M. J. & Wessely, S. (1996). Chronic fatigue in the community: “A question of attribution.”Psychological Medicine, 26, 791–800.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Somatic complaints in males as a function of age and neuroticism: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 3, 245–57.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1987). Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: is the bark worse than the bite?Journal of Personality, 55, 299–316.Google Scholar
Cox, D. J., Carter, W. R., Gonder-Frederick, L.Clarke, W. & Pohl, S. (1988). Training awareness of blood glucose in IDDM patients. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 13, 201–17.Google Scholar
Cox, D. J., Clarke, W. L., Gonder-Frederick, L.et al. (1985). Accuracy of perceiving blood glucose in IDDM. Diabetes Care, 8, 529–36.Google Scholar
Cox, D. J., Gonder-Frederick, L., Julian, D., Carter, W. R. & Clarke, W. (1989). Effects and correlates of blood glucose awareness training among patients with IDDM. Diabetes Care, 12, 313–18.Google Scholar
Cox, D. J., Gonder-Frederick, L., Julian, D.et al. (1991). Intensive versus standard glucose awareness training (BGAT) with insulin-dependent diabetes: mechanisms and ancillary effects. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 453–62.Google Scholar
Croyle, R. T. & Sande, G. N. (1988). Denial and confirmatory search: paradoxical consequences of medical diagnosis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 473–90.Google Scholar
Dunnell, K. & Cartwright, A. (1972). Medicine takers, prescribers, and hoarders. London: Routledge.
Feldman, P. J., Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P. & Gwaltney, J. M. Jr. (1999). The impact of personality on the reporting of unfounded symptoms and illness. Journal of PersonalityandSocial Psychology, 77, 370–78.Google Scholar
Freer, C. B. (1980). Self-care: a health diary study. Medical Care, 18, 853–61.Google Scholar
FTT Group (1994). Indications for fibrinolytic therapy in suspected acute myocardial infarction: collaborative overview of early mortality and major morbidity results from all randomised controlled trials of more than 1000 patients. Lancet, 343, 311–22.
Hannay, D. R. (1978). Symptom prevalence in the community. Journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners, 28, 492–9.Google Scholar
Jewett, D. L., Fein, G. & Greenberg, M. H. (1990). A double-blind study of symptom provocation to determine food sensitivity. New England Journal of Medicine, 323, 429–33.Google Scholar
Kendrick, A. H., Higgs, C. M., Whitfield, M. J.&Laszlo, G. (1993). Accuracy of perception of severity of asthma: patients treated in general practice. BMJ, 307, 422–4.Google Scholar
Kroenke, K. & Spitzer, R. L. (1998). Gender differences in the reporting of physical and somatoform symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 150–5.Google Scholar
Meechan, G., Collins, J. & Petrie, K. J. (2003). The relationship of symptoms and psychological factors to delay in seeking medical care for breast symptoms. Preventive Medicine, 36, 374–78.Google Scholar
Meyer, D., Leventhal, H. & Gutmann, M. (1985). Common-sense models of illness: the example of hypertension. Health Psychology, 4, 115–35.Google Scholar
Myers, M. G., Cairns, J. A. & Singer, J. (1987). The consent form as a possible source of side effects. Clinical Pharmacology Therapy, 42, 250–3.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1980). Perceptual and environmental determinants of coughing. Basic&Applied Social Psychology, 1, 83–91.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1981). Stimulus characteristics influencing estimation of heart rate. Psychophysiology, 18, 540–8.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J.W. (1984). Accuracy of symptom perception. In Baum, A., Taylor, S. E. & Singer, J. E. (Eds.). Handbook of psychology and health: social psychological aspects of health. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Pennebaker, J.W. (2000). Psychological factors influencing the reporting of physical symptoms. In Stone, A. A., Turkkan, J. S., Bachrach, C. A., Jobe, J. B., Kurtzman, H. S. & Cain, V. S. (Eds.). The science of self-report: implications for research and practice. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Pennebaker, J. W. & Epstein, D. (1983). Implicit psychophysiology: effects of common beliefs and idiosyncratic physiological responses on symptom reporting. Journal of Personality, 51, 468–96.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Gonder-Frederick, L., Stewart, H., Elfman, L. & Skelton, J. A. (1982). Physical symptoms associated with blood pressure. Psychophysiology, 19, 201–10.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. & Lightner, J. M. (1980). Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 165–74.Google Scholar
Perry, K., Petrie, K. J., Ellis, C. J., Horne, R. & Moss-Morris, R. (2001). Symptom expectations and delay in acute myocardial infarction patients. Heart, 86, 91–2.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. J., Moss-Morris, R., Grey, C. & Shaw, M. (2004). The relationship of negative affect and perceived sensitivity to symptom reporting following vaccination. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 101–11.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. J. & Weinman, J. (2003). More focus needed on symptom appraisal. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 401–3.Google Scholar
Richards, M. A., Smith, P., Ramirez, A. J., Fentiman, I. S. & Ruben, R. D. (1999). The influence on survival of delay in the presentation of symptomatic breast cancer. British Journal of Cancer, 79, 858–64.Google Scholar
Robbins, J. M. & Kirmayer, L. J. (1991). Attributions of common somatic symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 21, 1029–45.Google Scholar
Roberts, T.A. & Pennebaker, J.W. (1995). Gender differences in perceiving internal state: toward a his-and-hers model of perceptual cue use. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.). (1995). Advances in experimental social psychology. California, USA: Academic Press.
Salovey, P. & Birnbaum, D. (1989). Influence of mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 539–51.Google Scholar
Sensky, T., MacLeod, A. K. & Rigby, M. F. (1996). Causal attributions about common somatic sensations among frequent general practice attenders. Psychological Medicine, 26, 641–6.Google Scholar
Siegel, K., Schrimshaw, E. W. & Dean, L. (1999). Symptom interpretation and medication adherence among late middle-aged and older HIV-infected adults. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 247–57.Google Scholar
Sigerist, H. E. (1943). Civilization and disease. New York: Cornell University Press.
Smith, G. R. Jr., Monson, R. A. & Ray, D. C. (1986). Patients with multiple unexplained symptoms. Their characteristics, functional health, and health care utilization. Archives of Internal Medicine, 146, 69–72.Google Scholar
Bergh, O., Winters, W., Devriese, S.et al. (2004). Accuracy of respiratory symptom perception in persons with high and low negative affectivity. PsychologyandHealth, 19, 213–22.Google Scholar
Verbrugge, L. M. (1985). Triggers of symptoms and health care. Social ScienceandMedicine, 20, 855–76.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×