Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-qdpjg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-23T01:48:33.185Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Grounding Moral Psychology in Evolution, Neurobiology, and Culture

from Part IV - Origins, Development, and Variation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Bertram Malle
Affiliation:
Brown University, Rhode Island
Philip Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Get access

Summary

This chapter of the handbook proposes a developmental ethics, an organic moral theory grounded in (1) humanity’s deep evolutionary history, (2) the malleability of the child’s neurobiological structures that undergird moral functioning, and (3) the influence of cultural practices on neurobiological development. The chapter addresses the following questions: What kind of creature are we? What qualities do we need to live a full life? What kinds of capacities make each a proper member of the species? What influences our development? Answers center around perhaps the most critical influence on human development, our species’ evolved nest. In humanity’s ancestral context, nestedness is a lifelong experience with particular import in early life. Moral virtue emerges from holistically coordinated physiological, psychological, spiritual systems oriented toward holistic communal harmony, social attunement, receptivity, and interpersonal flexibility. Understanding how the evolved nest scaffolds biopsychosocial and moral development reveals why antisocial behavior is so pervasive in modern Western culture – and it provides a baseline for redesigning society to promote prosociality.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdolmaleky, H. M., Thiagalingam, S., & Wilcox, M. (2005). Genetics and epigenetics in major psychiatric disorders: Dilemmas, achievements, applications, and future scope. American Journal of Pharmacogenomics, 5(3), 149160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abram, D. (1996). Spell of the sensuous. Vintage Press.Google Scholar
Adams, D. W. (2020). Education for extinction: American Indians and the boarding school experience, 1875–1928 (2nd ed.). University Press of Kansas.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basso, K. (1996). Wisdom sits in places: Landscape and language among the western Apache. University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Bethlehem, R. A. I., Seidlitz, J., White, S. R., Vogel, J. W., Anderson, K. M., Adamson, C., Adler, S., Alexopoulos, G. S., Anagnostou, E., Areces-Gonzalez, A., Astle, D. E., Auyeung, B., Ayub, M., Bae, J., Ball, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Beare, R., Bedford, S. A., Benegal, V., … & Alexander-Bloch, A. F. (2022). Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature, 604, 525533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88(1), 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bram, M. (2002). The recovery of the west: An essay in symbolic history. Xlibris.Google Scholar
Bram, M. (2018). A history of humanity. Primus Books.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18(5), 175186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, C. S., & Porges, S. W. (2013). Neurobiology and the evolution of mammalian social behavior. In Narvaez, D., Panksepp, J., Schore, A., & Gleason, T. (Eds.), Evolution, early experience and human development (pp. 132151). Oxford.Google Scholar
Chua, A. (2011). Battle hymn of the tiger mother. Penguin.Google Scholar
Crittenden, P. M. (1995). Attachment and psychopathology. In Goldberg, S., Muir, R., & Kerr, J. (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives (pp. 367406). The Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1962). The origin of species. Collier Books. (Original work published 1859)Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man. Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1871)Google Scholar
deMause, L. (1995). The history of childhood. Psychohistory Press.Google Scholar
Doidge, N. (2007). The brain that changes itself. Viking.Google Scholar
Doris, J. (2005). Lack of character. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, R. (2011). The wild life of our bodies: Predators, parasites, and partners that shape who we are today. Harper.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Eggum, N. D. (2009). Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 7183). Boston Review.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisler, R., & Fry, D. P. (2019). Nurturing our humanity. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. Norton.Google Scholar
Felitti, V. (2010). Adverse childhood experiences and their relation to adult health and well-being: Turning gold into lead [Conference session]. Human Nature and Early Experience: Addressing the Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness. University of Notre Dame.Google Scholar
Felitti, V. J., & Anda, R. F. (2005). The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Centers for Disease Control and Kaiser Permanente.Google Scholar
Fisher, M. M., & Eugster, E. A. (2014). What is in our environment that effects puberty? Reproductive Toxicology, 44, 714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foot, P. (2001). Natural goodness. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frensch, P. A. (1998). One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. In Stadler, M. A. & Frensch, P. A. (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 47104). Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (2006). The human potential for peace: An anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (Ed.). (2013). War, peace, and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabel, P. (2018). The desire for mutual recognition. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, A., Yogman, M., & Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Council on Early Childhood. (2021). Preventing childhood toxic stress: Partnering with families and communities to promote relational health. Pediatrics, 148(2), Article e2021052582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershoff, E. T., & Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2016). Spanking and child outcomes: Old controversies and new meta-analyses. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(4), 453469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gielen, U. P., & Markoulis, D. C. (1994). Preference for principled moral reasoning: A developmental and cross-cultural perspective. In Adler, L. L. & Gielen, U. P. (Eds.), Cross-cultural topics in psychology (pp. 7387). Praeger.Google Scholar
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J., & Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(48), 1479914804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gowdy, J. (1998). Limited wants, unlimited means: A reader on hunter-gatherer economics and the environment. Island Press.Google Scholar
Graeber, D., & Wengrow, D. (2021). The dawn of everything: A new history of humanity. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Greer, A. (Ed.). (2000). The Jesuit relations: Natives and missionaries in seventeenth-century North America. Bedford/St. Martin’s.Google Scholar
Hambrick, E. P., Brawner, T. W., Perry, B. D., Brandt, K., Hofmeister, C., & Collins, J. O. (2019). Beyond the ACE score: Examining relationships between timing of developmental adversity, relational health and developmental outcomes in children. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 33(3), 238247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., & Yamamoto, S. (2017). Bonobos: Unique in mind, brain and behavior. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, G. (2017). Animism: Respecting the living world (2nd ed.). C. Hurst & Co.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., & Coxworth, J. E. (2013). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity: A review of findings and future directions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 22(6), 294302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, V. (1993). Feminist morality: Transforming culture, society, and politics. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Henley, T., & Rossano, M. (Eds.). (2022). Psychology and cognitive archaeology: An interdisciplinary approach to the study of the human mind. Routledge.Google Scholar
Henley, T., Rossano, M., & Kardas, E. (Eds.). (2019). Handbook of cognitive archaeology: A psychological framework. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. P., & Wang, S. (1998). Effects of early stress on adult affiliative behavior, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23(8), 863875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2005). Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Aldine Transaction.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2005). On the social relations of the hunter-gatherer band. In Lee, R. B. & Daly, R. (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (pp. 399410). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2011). The perception of the environment: Essays on livelihood, dwelling and skill. Routledge.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2013). Prospect. In Ingold, T. & Palsson, G. (Eds.), Biosocial becomings: Integrating social and biological anthropology (pp. 121). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2006). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (2014). Morality for humans: Ethical understanding from the perspective of cognitive science. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 167225). Wiley.Google Scholar
Klinenberg, E. (2012). Going solo: The extraordinary rise and surprising appeal of living alone. Penguin.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Essays on moral development (Vol. 1). Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: Essays on moral development (Vol. 2). Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Krebs, D. L. (2005). The evolution of morality. In Buss, D. (Ed.), Evolutionary psychology handbook (pp. 747774). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & Daly, R. (Eds.). (2005). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lodge, R. C. (1944). Balanced philosophy and eclecticism. Journal of Philosophy, 41(4), 8591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Otin, C., & Kroemer, G. (2021). Hallmarks of health. Cell, 184(1), 3363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations (M. Lopez Morillas & L. Solataroff, Trans.). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Maté, G. (2010). In the realm of hungry ghosts. North Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
Maté, G., & Maté, D. (2022). The myth of normal: Trauma, illness, and healing in a toxic culture. Avery.Google Scholar
McPherson, D. H., & Rabb, J. D. (2011). Indian from the inside: Native American philosophy and cultural renewal (2nd ed.). MacFarland & Co.Google Scholar
Menakem, R. (2017). My grandmother’s hands: Racialized trauma and the pathway to mending our hearts and bodies. Central Recovery Press.Google Scholar
Midgely, M. (2010). The solitary self: Darwin and the selfish gene. Acumen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Milburn, M. A., & Conrad, S. D. (2016). Raised to rage: The politics of anger and the roots of authoritarianism. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Montagu, A. (1968). Brains, genes, culture, immaturity, and gestation. In Montagu, A. (Ed.), Culture: Man’s adaptive dimension (pp. 102113). Oxford.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2008). Triune ethics: The neurobiological roots of our multiple moralities. New Ideas in Psychology, 26(1), 95119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2010). Moral complexity: The fatal attraction of truthiness and the importance of mature moral functioning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(2), 163181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D. (2013). The 99% – Development and socialization within an evolutionary context: Growing up to become “A good and useful human being.” In Fry, D. (Ed.), War, peace and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views (pp. 643672). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2014). Neurobiology and the development of human morality: Evolution, culture and wisdom. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2016). Baselines for virtue. In Annas, J., Narvaez, D., & Snow, N. (Eds.), Developing the virtues: Integrating perspectives (pp. 1433). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2017). Are we losing it? Darwin’s moral sense and the importance of early experience. In Joyce, R. (Ed.), Routledge handbook of evolution and philosophy (pp. 322332). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (Ed.). (2018). Basic needs, wellbeing and morality: Fulfilling human potential. Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2019). In search of baselines: Why psychology needs cognitive archaeology. In Henley, T., Rossano, M. & Kardas, E. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive archaeology: A psychological framework (pp. 104119). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2020a). Ecocentrism: Resetting baselines for virtue development. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 23(3), 391406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2020b). Moral education in a time of human ecological devastation. Journal of Moral Education, 50(1), 5567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2021). Species-typical phronesis for a living planet. In De Caro, M. & Vaccarezza, M. S. (Eds.), Practical wisdom: Philosophical and psychological perspectives (pp. 160180). Routledge.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D., Arrows, Four, Halton, E., Collier, B., & Enderle, G. (Eds.). (2019). Indigenous sustainable wisdom: First Nation know-how for global flourishing. Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Gettler, L., Braungart-Rieker, J., Miller Graff, L., & Hastings, P. (2016). The flourishing of young children: Evolutionary baselines. In Narvaez, D., Braungart-Rieker, J., Miller, L., Gettler, L., & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Contexts for young child flourishing: Evolution, family and society (pp. 327). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Gleason, T., Tarsha, M., Woodbury, R., Cheng, A., & Wang, L. (2021). Sociomoral temperament: A mediator between wellbeing and social outcomes in young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 5111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D., Moore, D. S., Witherington, D. C., Vandiver, T. I., & Lickliter, R. (2022). Evolving evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist, 77(3), 424438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D., Panksepp, J., Schore, A., & Gleason, T. (Eds.). (2013). Evolution, early experience, and human development: From research to practice and policy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D., Wang, L, & Cheng, A. (2016). Evolved developmental niche history: Relation to adult psychopathology and morality. Applied Developmental Science, 20(4), 294309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., & Witherington, D. (2018). Getting to baselines for human nature, development and wellbeing. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 6(1), 205213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Woodbury, R., Gleason, T., Kurth, A., Cheng, A., Wang, L., Deng, L., Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E., Christen, M., & Näpflin, C. (2019). Evolved development niche provision: Moral socialization, social maladaptation and social thriving in three countries. Sage Open, 9(2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newen, A., De Bruin, L., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4e cognition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niehoff, D. (1999). The biology of violence: How understanding the brain, behavior, and environment can break the vicious circle of aggression. Free Press.Google Scholar
Ong, W. (2002). Orality and literacy. Routledge.Google Scholar
Oster, E. (2019). Cribsheet: A data-driven guide to better, more relaxed parenting, from birth to preschool. Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child (M. Gabain, Trans.). Free Press. (Originally published in 1932)Google Scholar
Porges, S. (2011). Polyvagal theory. Norton.Google Scholar
Porter, B. (2021). Unprotected: A memoir. Abrams.Google Scholar
Power, C., Finnegan, M., & Callan, H. (2017). Introduction. In Power, C., Finnegan, M., & Callan, H. (Eds.), Human origins: Contributions from social anthropology (pp. 134). Berghahn.Google Scholar
Pulcini, E. (2019). Is care a gift? In Vaughan, G. (Ed.), The maternal roots of the gift economy (pp. 7893). Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Redfield, R. (1953). The primitive world and its transformations. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Redfield, R. (1956). Peasant society and culture: An anthropological approach to civilization. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rest, J. (1983). Morality. In Flavell, J. & Markham, E. (Eds.), Cognitive development, from P. Mussen (Ed.), Manual of child psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 556629). Wiley.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1968). Notes on the original affluent society. In Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I. (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 8589). Aldine Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (2008). The Western illusion of human nature. Prickly Paradigm Press.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. Norton.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. Norton.Google Scholar
Sheldrake, M. (2021). Entangled life: How fungi make our worlds, change our minds & shape our futures. Random House.Google Scholar
Shepard, P. (1998). Coming home to the Pleistocene (Shepard, F. R., Ed.). Island Press/Shearwater Books.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Siepel, K. H. (2015). Conquistador voices: The Spanish conquest of the Americas as recounted largely by the participants: Christopher Columbus, Hernán Cortés. Spruce Tree.Google Scholar
Simard, S. (2021). Finding the Mother Tree: Discovering how the forest is wired for intelligence and healing. Knopf.Google Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. (2014). Trying not to try: Ancient China, modern science, and the power of spontaneity. Broadway Books.Google Scholar
Sorenson, E. R. (1998). Preconquest consciousness. In Wautischer, H. (Ed.), Tribal epistemologies (pp. 79115). Ashgate.Google Scholar
Tarsha, M. S., & Narvaez, D. (2021). Effects of adverse childhood experience on physiological regulation are moderated by evolved developmental niche history. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 35(4), 488500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarsha, M. S., & Narvaez, D. (2023). The developmental neurobiology of moral mindsets: Basic needs and childhood experience. In Berg, M. & Chang, E. (Eds.), Motivation & morality: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 187204). APA Books.Google Scholar
Thompson, M. (1995). The representation of life. In Hursthouse, R., Lawrence, G., & Quinn, W. (Eds.), Virtues and reasons (pp. 247296). Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. (1965). Affect and the psychology of knowledge. In Tomkins, S. S. & Izard, C. E. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and personality (pp. 7297). Springer.Google Scholar
Topa, W., & Narvaez, D. (2022). Restoring the kinship worldview: Indigenous voices introduce 28 precepts for rebalancing life on planet earth. North Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
Trevathan, W. R. (2011). Human birth: An evolutionary perspective (2nd ed.). Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C. M. (1984). The human cycle. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Varela, F. (1999). Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Vaughan, G. (2007). Introduction: A radically different worldview is possible. In Vaughan, G. (Ed.), Women and the gift economy (pp. 140). Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Vaughan, G. (Ed.). (2019). The maternal roots of the gift economy. Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Watson, J. (1988). The relation of philosophy to science. In Rabb, J. D. (Ed.), Religion and science in early Canada (pp. 1839). Ronald P. Frye.Google Scholar
Weaver, L. N., Varricchio, D. J., Sargis, E. J., Chen, M., Freimuth, W. J., & Wilson Mantilla, G. P. (2021). Early mammalian social behaviour revealed by multituberculates from a dinosaur nesting site. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 5(1), 3237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, G. (2012). Ayn Rand nation: The hidden struggle for America’s soul. Macmillan.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widlok, T. (2017). Anthropology and the economy of sharing. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wolynn, M. (2016). It didn’t start with you: How inherited family trauma shapes who we are and how to end the cycle. Penguin.Google Scholar
Worster, D. (1994). Nature’s economy: A history of ecological ideas (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., & Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence. Houghton, Mifflin and Company.Google Scholar
Young, J. (2019). Connection modeling: Metrics for deep nature-connection, mentoring, and culture repair. In Narvaez, D., Four Arrows, E. Halton, B. Collier, , & Enderle, G. (Eds.), Indigenous sustainable wisdom: First Nation knowhow for global flourishing (pp. 219243). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Young, J., Haas, E., & McGown, E. (2010). Coyote’s guide to connecting with nature (2nd ed.). Owlink Media.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×