Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:52:28.842Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Pictorial Irony and Sarcasm

from Part VI - Irony in Expressive, Nonlinguistic Media

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2023

Herbert L. Colston
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Get access

Summary

Pictorial irony is another example of nonverbal irony. This chapter addresses the need for experimental work on these topics in light of the view that irony is deeply tied to human cognition and not just language. Katz examines different ways of distinguishing between irony and sarcasm, particularly in terms of “vector space theory,” which suggests that sarcasm is more aggressive, dark, and mocking than is irony. Additional empirical analyses note important distinctions in defining the notions of verbal vs. situational irony. Katz then applies his “constraint-satisfaction” model to create an open-ended list of visual features that likely signal the presence of irony or sarcasm in visual, including pictorial, displays. Katz argues that basic psychological processes involved in scene perception, which have deep evolutionary roots, are employed when people infer either sarcastic or ironic intents in pictures (including pictures with and without accompanying words). At the same time, similar psychological processes used in detecting pretense or echoic mention within language can also be adopted for understanding visual scenes as conveying sarcasm or irony. Expertise with some visual medium, such as painting, may enhance people’s abilities to readily interpret these as expressing irony in different ways.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attardo, S. (2000). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793826.Google Scholar
Baldwin, B. (1981). The cakewalk: A study in stereotype and reality. Journal of Social History, 15(2), 205218.Google Scholar
Barbe, K. (1995). Irony in context. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Barbieri, M. (2008). What is Biosemiotics? Biosemiotics, 1, 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bashir, N., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A., Nadolny, D., & Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic impact of activists: Negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 614626, https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1983Google Scholar
Blasko, D., Kazmerski, V., & Dawood, S (2021). Saying what you don’t mean: A cross-cultural study of perceptions of sarcasm. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 114119. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000258Google Scholar
Boime, A. (1990). Art in an Age of Bonapartism, 1800–1815. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bourne, J. (2016). Perceiving irony in music: The problem in Beethoven’s string quartets. Music Theory Online, 22(3). https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.16.22.3/mto.16.22.3.bourne.htmlGoogle Scholar
Boylan, J., & Katz, A. N. (2013). Ironic expressions can simultaneously enhance and dilute perceptions of criticism. Discourse Processes, 50, 187209.Google Scholar
Brooks, K. R. (2017). Depth perception and the history of three-dimensional art: Who produced the first stereoscopic images? I Perception 8(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669516680114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caffarra, S., Haeri, A. M., Michell, E., & Martin, C. D. (2019). When is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive models. European Journal of Neuroscience, 50, 35663577.Google Scholar
Campbell, J., & Katz, A. N. (2012). Are there necessary and sufficient conditions for inducing a sense of sarcasm? Discourse Processes, 49, 459480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caucci, G., & Kreuz, R. (2012). Social and paralinguistic cues to sarcasm. Humor, 25, 122.Google Scholar
Colston, H., & Gibbs, R. (2021). Figurative language communicates directly because it precisely demonstrates what we mean. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 228233. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000254Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2011). The irony in pictures. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 51(2), 149167.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. (1997). How the eye measures reality and virtual reality. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29(1), 2736.Google Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Owren, M., & Zimmermann, E. (2009). Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans Current Biology, 19, 11061111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dress, M. L., Kreuz, R. J., Link, K. E., & Caucci, G. M. (2008). Regional variation in the use of sarcasm. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27(1), 7185.Google Scholar
Eckert, J., Winkler, S. L., & Cartmill, E. A. (2020). Just kidding: The evolutionary roots of playful teasing. Biology Letters, 16(9). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2020.0370Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. M., Clift, J. B., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Social disappointment explains chimpanzees’ behavior in the inequity aversion task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 284. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferretti, T. R., Hall, D. C., & Mansour, F. (2021). Interpreting pragmatic markers following proverbs. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 197203. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000231Google Scholar
Filik, R., Turcan, A., Nearman, C., & Pitiot, A. (2019). What is the difference between irony and sarcasm? An fMRI study. Cortex, 115, 112122.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2018a). Irony as Echo. In Irony (pp. 4264). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2018b). Irony in Pretense. In Irony (pp. 6587). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., & Fein, O. (2015). Defaultness reigns: The case of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 30(4), 290231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., & Levinson, S. (2019). Multimodal language processing in human communication. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 639652.Google Scholar
Jared, D., & Pandolfo, A. (2021). The effect of speaker age on the perception of ironic insults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 146154. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgensen, J., (1996). The functions of sarcastic irony in speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 26, 613634.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N. (2005). Discourse and sociocultural factors in understanding nonliteral language. In Colston, H. & Katz, A. N. (Eds.), Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences (pp. 183207). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N. (2009). On the science and art of sarcasm. In Boldt-Irons, L., Federici, C., & Virguilti, E. (Eds.), Disguise, deception, trompe-l’oeil: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 8196). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Katz, A., Blasko, D., & Kazmerski, V (2004). Saying what you don’t mean: Social influences on sarcastic language processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 186189.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Hussey, K. (2017). Do people hear a sarcastic tone of voice when silently reading sarcastic text? Metaphor and Symbol, 32(2), 84102.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Lee, C. (1993). The role of authorial intent in determining verbal irony and metaphor. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 8, 257279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Thompson, M. (1993). On judging creativity: By one’s acts shall ye be known (and vice-versa). Creativity Research Journal, 6, 345364.Google Scholar
Kesner, L., Grygarová, D., Fajnerová, I., Lukavský, J., Nekovářová, T., Tintěra, J., Zaytseva, Y., & Horáček, J. (2018). Perception of direct vs. averted gaze in portrait paintings: An fMRI and eye-tracking study. Brain and Cognition, 125, 8899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354, 110114.Google Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(1), 321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mantovan, L., Giustolisi, B., & Panzeri, F.(2019). Signing something while meaning its opposite: The expression of irony in Italian Sign Language (LIS). Journal of Pragmatics, 142, 4761.Google Scholar
Mauchand, M., Caballero, J. A., Jiang, X., & Pell, M. D. (2021). Immediate online use of prosody reveals the ironic intentions of a speaker: Neurophysiological evidence. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscence, 21, 7492. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00849-7Google Scholar
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2021). Processing of irony in text: A systematic review of eye-tracking studies. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 99106. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000216Google Scholar
Palinkas, I. (2014). Blending and folk theory in an explanation of irony. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 12(1), 6498.Google Scholar
Pennington, J., Socher, R., & Manning, C (2014). GloVe: Global vectors for word representation. Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP) (pp. 1532–1543). Association for Computational Linguistics. www.aclweb.org/anthology/D14-1162/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P., Ferretti, T., & Katz, A. (2000). Discourse factors that influence on-line reading of metaphor and irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 201222.Google Scholar
Pexman, P., & Olineck, K. (2002). Does sarcasm always sting? Investigating the impact of ironic insults and ironic compliments. Discourse Processes, 33(3), 199217.Google Scholar
Reid, J. N., Al-Azary, H., & Katz, A. N. (2020). Metaphors: Where the neighborhood in which one resides interacts with (interpretive) diversity. Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 551–556).Google Scholar
Reid, J. N., & Katz, A. (2018). Vector space applications in metaphor comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 33(4), 280294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermich, K., Simmons, E., Makarla, P., Benson, L., Kim, H., & Joergensen, G (2021). Tracking nonliteral language processing using audiovisual scenarios. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 211220. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000223Google Scholar
Scott, B. (2004). Picturing irony: The subversive power of photography. Visual Communication, 3, 3159.Google Scholar
Sebeok, T. (1991). A sign is just a sign. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Tausczik, R., & Pennebaker, J. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 2454.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., Leuthold, H., & Filik, R. (2021). Examining the influence of perspective and prosody on expected emotional responses to irony: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 107113. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiv, M., Deodato, F., Rouillard, V., Wiebe, S., & Titone, D. (2021). Second language experience impacts first language irony comprehension among bilingual adults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 75(2), 126132. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toplak, M., & Katz, A. N. (2000). On the uses of sarcastic irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 14671488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 17771806.Google Scholar
Wallace, B. C. (2015). Computational irony: A survey and new perspectives. Artificial Intelligence Review, 43, 467483Google Scholar
Wheeler, W. (2016). Expecting the earth: Life, culture, biosemiotics. Lawrence and Wishart.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×