Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T12:40:12.742Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

35 - Mating Intelligence

from Part VI - Kinds of Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access

Summary

From an evolutionary perspective, psychological factors that bear on reproductive success are of particular importance as such factors directly pertain to Darwin’s bottom line. The psychology surrounding human mating, then, is particularly important from a Darwinian perspective. Mating intelligence is a construct that integrates work on mating psychology with work on intelligence. This broad construct is divided into two general sets of abilities: cognitive mating mechanisms (such as the ability to detect romantic interest on the part of a potential mate) and mental fitness indicators (which are outward behavioral displays of intelligence that facilitate successful courtship).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arden, R., Gottfredson, L. S., Miller, G., & Pierce, A. (2009). Intelligence and semen quality are positively correlated. Intelligence, 37, 277282.Google Scholar
Arnocky, S., Piché, T., Albert, G., Ouellette, D., & Barclay, P. (2017). Altruism predicts mating success in humans. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 416435. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12208Google Scholar
Banks, G. C., Batchelor, J. H., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Smarter people are (a bit) more symmetrical: A meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and fluctuating asymmetry. Intelligence, 4, 393401.Google Scholar
Bates, T. C. (2007). Fluctuating asymmetry and intelligence. Intelligence, 35, 4146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaussart, M. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Creative activity, personality, mental illness, and short-term mating success. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 151167.Google Scholar
Bleske-Recheck, A. L., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Opposite-sex friendship: Sex differences and similarities in initiation, selection, and dissolution. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 13101327.Google Scholar
Borsboom, D., & Dolan, C. V. (2006). Why g is not an adaptation: A comment on Kanazawa (2004). Psychological Review, 113, 433437.Google Scholar
Brackett, M. A., Alster, B., Wolfe, C. J., Katulak, N. A., & Fale, E. (2007). Creating an emotionally intelligent school district: A skill based approach In Bar-On, R., Maree, J. G., & Elias, M. J. (Eds.), Educating people to be emotionally intelligent (pp. 123137). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate selection: Evolutionary hypothesis tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2008). The future of mating intelligence. Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Weston, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychological Science, 3, 251255.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1996). Strategic self-promotion and competition derogation: Sex and conflict effects on perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 11851204.Google Scholar
Calvin, C. M., Deary, I. J., Fenton, C., Roberts, B. A., Der, G., Leckenby, N., & Batty, G. D. (2010). Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: Systematic review with meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyq190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camargo, M. A., Geher, G., Fisher, M. L., & Arrabaca, A. (2013). The relationship between hypothesized psychological genetic fitness indicators and indices of mating success. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 7(2), 138162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099205Google Scholar
Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). Personality and social intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Casey, J. J., Garrett, J., Brackett, M. A., & Rivers, S. (2008). Emotional intelligence, relationship quality, and partner selection. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 263282). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chiappe, D., & MacDonald, K. (2005). The evolution of domain-general mechanisms in intelligence and learning. Journal of General Psychology, 132, 540.Google Scholar
Crocchiola, D. (2014). Art as an indicator of male fitness: Does prenatal testosterone influence artistic ability? Evolutionary Psychology, 12(3), 521533.Google Scholar
Cyrus, K., Schwarz, S., & Hassebrauck, M. (2011). Systematic cognitive biases in courtship context: Women’s commitment–skepticism as a life-history strategy? Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 1320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2 vols.). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2005). Afterword. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dillon, H. M., Adair, L. E., Geher, G., Wang, Z., & Strouts, P. H. (2016). Playing smart: The mating game and mating intelligence. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 35(3), 414420. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9309-yGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309.Google Scholar
Feist, G. (2001). Natural and sexual selection in the evolutionary of creativity. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 2, 1116.Google Scholar
Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., Jones, D. N., Sefcek, J. A., Vasquez, G., & Jacobs, W. J. (2008). Ecological constraints on mating tactics. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 335361). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fisher, M. L. (2004). Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 271, 283285.Google Scholar
Furlow, B., Armijo-Prewitt, T., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). Fluctuating asymmetry and psychometric intelligence. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 264, 823829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabora, L., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). Evolutionary approaches to creativity. Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 279301). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S.W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573644.Google Scholar
Garcia, J. R., & Reiber, C. (2008). Hook-up behavior: A biopsychosocial perspective. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 2, 192208.Google Scholar
Garcia, J. R., Reiber, C., Massey, S. G., & Merriwether, A. M. (2012). Sexual hookup culture: A review. Review of General Psychology, 16(2),161.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2004). Origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2009). Evolution of general fluid intelligence. In Platek, S. M. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), Foundations in evolutionary cognitive neuroscience (pp. 2526). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Geher, G. (Ed.). (2004). Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy. New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Geher, G. (2009). Accuracy and oversexualization in cross-sex mind-reading: An adaptationist approach. Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 331347.Google Scholar
Geher, G. (2014). Evolutionary Psychology 101. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Geher, G., Betancourt, K., & Jewell, O. (2017). The link between emotional intelligence and creativity. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 37(1), 522. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236617710029Google Scholar
Geher, G., Camargo, M. A., & O’Rourke, S. (2008). Future directions in research on mating intelligence. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 395425). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geher, G., & Kaufman, S. B. (2007). The mating intelligence scale. Psychology Today, 40, 7879.Google Scholar
Geher, G., & Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Mating intelligence unleashed: The role of the mind in sex, dating, and love. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Geher, G., & Miller, G. F. (Eds.). (2008). Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geher, G., Miller, G., & Murphy, J. (2008). Mating intelligence: Toward an evolutionarily informed construct. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 334). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geher, G., & Renstrom, K. L. (2004). Measurement issues in emotional intelligence research. In Geher, G. (Ed.), Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy. New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Giosan, C. (2006). High-k strategy scale: A measure of the high-k independent criterion of fitness. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 394405.Google Scholar
Girotto, V., & Tentori, K. (2008). Is domain-general thinking a domain-specific adaptation? Mind & Society, 7, 167175.Google Scholar
Greengross, G., & Miller, G. F. (2008). Dissing oneself versus one’s rivals. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 393408.Google Scholar
Greengross, G., & Miller, G. (2011). Humor ability reveals intelligence, predicts mating success, and is higher in males. Intelligence, 39(4), 188192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.006Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Peacocks, Picasso, and parental investment: The effects of romantic motives on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 6376.Google Scholar
Gottschall, J., & Wilson, D. S. (2005). The literary animal: Evolution and the nature of narrative. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Harrison, A., Moore, M., & Rucker, M. (1985). Further evidence on career and family compatibility among eminent women and men. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 46, 140155.Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 8191.Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., Buss, D. M., Oubaid, V., & Angleitner, A. (2005). Sex, lies, and strategic interference: The psychology of deception between the sexes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 323.Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., & Miller, G. F. (2006). Women’s fertility across the cycle increases the short-term attractiveness of creative intelligence compared to wealth. Human Nature, 17, 5073.Google Scholar
Hasson, O. (2006). The role of amplifiers in sexual selection: An integration of the amplifying and the Fisherian mechanisms. Evolutionary Ecology, 4, 277289.Google Scholar
Hughes, S., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2003). Sex differences in morphological predictors of sexual behavior: Shoulder to hip and waist to hip ratios. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 173178.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. London: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2004). General intelligence as a domain-specific adaptation. Psychological Review, 111, 512523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanazawa, S. (2008). The independence of mating intelligence and general intelligence. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2010). Evolutionary psychology and intelligence research. American Psychologist, 65, 279289.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Reis, D. L., & Gray, J. R. (2011). General intelligence predicts reasoning ability even for evolutionarily familiar content. Intelligence, 39, 311322.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Erickson, J. E., Huang, J. Y., Ramesh, S., Thompson, S., Kozbelt, A., Paul, E., & Kaufman, J. C. (2009). Art as an aphrodisiac. Paper presented at the Northeastern Evolutionary Psychology Society Conference, Oswego, NY.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Erickson, J. E., Ramesh, S., Kozbelt, A., Magee, M., & Kaufman, J. C. (2010). What are funny people like? Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Conference, Eugene, OR.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.). (2009). The psychology of creative writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Kozbelt, A., Bromley, M. L., & Miller, G. F. (2008). The role of creativity and humor in human mate selection. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 227263). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Keller, M., & Miller, G. F. (2006). An evolutionary framework for mental disorders: Integrating adaptationist and evolutionary genetics models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 429452.Google Scholar
Kirsner, B. R., Figueredo, A. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2003). Self, friends, and lovers: Structural relations among Beck Depression Inventory scores and perceived mate values. Journal of Affective Disorders, 75(2), 131148. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00048-4Google Scholar
Klasios, J. (2013). Cognitive traits as sexually selected fitness indicators. Review of General Psychology, 17(4), 428442. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kordsmeyer, T. L., & Penke, L. (2017). The association of three indicators of developmental instability with mating success in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(6), 704713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.08.002Google Scholar
Kruger, D. J., Reischl, T. M., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2008). Time perspective as a mechanism for functional developmental adaptation. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 2, 122.Google Scholar
Lange, B. P., & Euler, H. A. (2014). Writers have groupies, too: High quality literature production and mating success. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(1), 2030. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0097246Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (2017). Emancipation of the voice: Vocal complexity as a fitness indicator. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 232237. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1105-7Google Scholar
Lynn, C. D., Pipitone, R. N., & Keenan, J. P. (2014). To thine own self be false: Self-deceptive enhancement and sexual awareness influences on mating success. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(2), 109122. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0097255Google Scholar
MacArthur, R. H., & Wilson, E. O. (1967). Theory of island biogeography. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional intelligence: Science and myth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., & Geher, G. (1996). Emotional intelligence and the identification of emotion. Intelligence, 22, 89113.Google Scholar
McKeown, G. J. (2013). The Analogical Peacock Hypothesis: The sexual selection of mind-reading and relational cognition in human communication. Review of General Psychology, 17(3), 267287. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032631Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (1998). How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution. In Crawford, C. B. & Krebs, D. L. (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 87129). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C., & Power, C. (Eds.), The evolution of culture (pp. 7191). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2000a). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2000b). Mental traits as fitness indicators: Expanding evolutionary psychology’s adaptationism. In LeCroy, D. & Moller, P. (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human reproductive behavior (pp. 6274). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2000c). Sexual selection for indicators of intelligence. Novartis Foundation Symposium, 233, 260270.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2001). Aesthetic fitness: How sexual selection shaped artistic virtuosity as a fitness indicator and aesthetic preferences as mate choice criteria. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 2, 2025.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2007). Sexual selection for moral virtues. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(2), 97125.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2008). Mating intelligence: Frequently asked questions. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system (pp. 367393). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F., Tybur, J., & Jordan, B. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap-dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus? Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 375381.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (2006). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind, and body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2009). The evolution of creative writing. In Kaufman, S. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), The psychology of creative writing (pp. 101117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D., & Clegg, H. (2006). Schizotypy, creativity and mating success in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society: B, 273, 611615.Google Scholar
Nettle, D., & Clegg, H. (2008). Personality, mating strategies, and mating intelligence. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O’Brien, D., Geher, G., Gallup, A., Garcia, J., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). Self-perceived mating intelligence predicts sexual behavior in college students: Empirical validation of a theoretical construct. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29, 341362.Google Scholar
O’Sullivan, M. (2008). Deception and self-deception as strategies in short- and long-term mating. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Penke, L., Denissen, J. J., & Miller, G. F. (2007). The evolutionary genetics of personality [target article]. European Journal of Personality, 21, 549587.Google Scholar
Penke, L., Todd, P., Lenton, A. P., & Fasolo, B. (2008). How self-related cognitions can guide human mating decisions. In Geher, G. & Miller, G. F. (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perina, K. (2007). Love’s loopy logic. Psychology Today, 40, 6877.Google Scholar
Pérusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship at the proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 267322.Google Scholar
Peterson, A., Carmen, R., & Geher, G. (2013). Ovulatory shifts in mating intelligence. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 7(1), 6675. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099173Google Scholar
Peterson, A., Geher, G., & Kaufman, S. B. (2011). Predicting preferences for sex acts: Which traits matter most, and why? Evolutionary Psychology, 9(3), 371389. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491100900308Google Scholar
Pipitone, R. N., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). Women’s voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268274.Google Scholar
Prokosch, M. D., Coss, R. G., Scheib, J. E., & Blozis, S. A. (2009). Intelligence and mate choice: Intelligent men are always appealing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 1120.Google Scholar
Prokosch, M. D., Yeo, R. A., & Miller, G. F. (2005). Intelligence tests with higher g-loadings show higher correlations with body symmetry: Evidence for a general fitness factor mediated by developmental stability. Intelligence, 33, 203213.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, J., & Tunney, R. J. (2008). Human vocabulary use as display. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 538549.Google Scholar
Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Fundamentals of human mating strategies. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology. NewYork: Wiley.Google Scholar
Silventoinen, K., Posthuma, D., van Beijsterveldt, T., Bartels, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2006). Genetic contributions to the association between height and intelligence: Evidence from Dutch twin data from childhood to middle age. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5, 585595.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., & Gangstead, S.W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 870883.Google Scholar
Śmieja, M., & Stolarski, M. (2018). Assortative mating for emotional intelligence. Current Psychology, 37(1), 180187.Google Scholar
Springer, J. (2009). Evolution’s Match.com. Psy-chCRITIQUES, 53.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Successful intelligence. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sundet, J. M., Tambs, K., Harris, J. R., Magnus, P., & Torjussen, T. M. (2005). Resolving the genetic and environmental sources of the correlation between height and intelligence: A study of nearly 2600 Norwegian male twin pairs. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8, 307311.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. D., Hart, C. L., Smith, G. D., Whal-ley, L. J., Hole, D. J., Wilson, V., & Deary, I. J. (2005). Childhood IQ and marriage by mid-life: The Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 16211630.Google Scholar
Tornquist, M., & Chiappe, D. (2015). Effects of humor production, humor receptivity, and physical attractiveness on partner desirability. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(4), 113. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704915608744Google Scholar
Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wagstaff, D. L., Sulikowski, D., & Burke, D. (2015). Sex-differences in preference for looking at the face or body in short-term and long-term mating contexts. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 13(1), 117.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (2007). Evolution for everyone. New York: Bantam.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zebrowitz, L.A., & Rhodes, G. (2004). Sensitivity to “bad genes” and the anomalous face over-generalization effect: Cue validity, cue utilization, and accuracy in judging intelligence and health. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 167185.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×