Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:42:58.754Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

26 - Infant Emotion Development and Temperament

from Part VI - Emotional and Social Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2020

Jeffrey J. Lockman
Affiliation:
Tulane University, Louisiana
Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Caregivers of young infants are often well practiced in detecting and interpreting the presence or absence of infant emotion. This is particularly true in the case of negative emotions, motivating caregivers to take on the mantel of detective. Why is the baby crying? Is he/she hungry? Cold? Too hot? Angry? Gassy? Tired? Bored? A caregiver’s need to search for clues reflects infants’ rather limited communicative repertoire, coupled with a restricted behavioral toolbox. Over the first 2 years of life, children’s expression and experience of emotion becomes more expansive, providing greater insight into the cause of any one emotional experience and the needed response. However, even at this point, parents and caregivers play an important role in modulating infants’ emotional experiences, since much of emotion regulation is first implemented externally until the child can internalize and develop effective stand-alone regulatory responses.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development
Brain, Behavior, and Cultural Context
, pp. 715 - 741
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aktar, E., & Bögels, S. M. (2017). Exposure to parents’ negative emotions as a developmental pathway to the family aggregation of depression and anxiety in the first year of life. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 20, 369390.Google Scholar
Aktar, E., Colonnesi, C., de Vente, W., Majdandžić, M., & Bögels, S. M. (2017). How do parents’ depression and anxiety, and infants’ negative temperament relate to parent–infant face-to-face interactions? Development and Psychopathology, 29, 697710.Google Scholar
Aktar, E., Majdandžić, M., de Vente, W., & Bögels, S. M. (2013). The interplay between expressed parental anxiety and infant behavioural inhibition predicts infant avoidance in a social referencing paradigm. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 144156.Google Scholar
Barrett, K. C., & Campos, J. J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 555578). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Bates, J. E., Goodnight, J. A., & Fite, J. E. (2008). Temperament and emotion. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. J., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 485496). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bayet, L., Quinn, P. C., Tanaka, J. W., Lee, K., Gentaz, É., & Pascalis, O. (2015). Face gender influences the looking preference for smiling expressions in 3.5-month-old human infants. PLOS ONE, 10, e0129812. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300304.Google Scholar
Bennett, D. S., Bendersky, M., & Lewis, M. (2005). Does the organization of emotional expression change over time? Facial expressivity from 4 to 12 months. Infancy, 8(2), 167187.Google Scholar
Bowman, L. C., & Fox, N. A. (2018). Distinctions between temperament and emotion: Examining reactivity, regulation, and social understanding. In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. (pp. 5458). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early childhood. In Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), Social development (pp. 185211). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359374.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D. (Ed.). (2015). Handbook of infant biopsychosocial development. New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), The handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 229248). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149219.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 7(5), 317333. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00681.xGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A. (2011). Differentiation, dynamical integration, and functional emotional development. Emotion Review, 3, 138146.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28(1), 6277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X. (2010). Shyness-inhibition in childhood and adolescence: A cross-cultural perspective. In Rubin, K. H. & Coplan, R. J. (Eds.), The development of shyness and social withdrawal (pp. 213235). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, Z. Y. (1995). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 31(4), 531.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Li, B. S., & Li, D. (1999). Adolescent outcomes of social functioning in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23(1), 199223.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Bendezú, J. J., Ram, N., & Chow, S. M. (2017). Dynamical systems modeling of early childhood self-regulation. Emotion, 17, 684699.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., & Hollenstein, T. (Eds.). (2018). Emotion regulation: A matter of time. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2004). Infant and maternal behaviors regulate infant reactivity to novelty at 6 months. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1123.Google Scholar
Degnan, K. A., Almas, A. N., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Temperament and the environment in the etiology of childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 497517.Google Scholar
Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social–emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 765.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. (2007). The socialization of emotional competence. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 614637). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dennis, T. A., O’Toole, L. J., & DeCicco, J. M. (2013). Emotion regulation from the perspective of developmental neuroscience: What, where, when, and why. In Barrett, K. C., Fox, N. A., Morgan, G. A., Fidler, D. J., & Daunhauer, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives (pp. 135172). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Diaz, A., & Bell, M. A. (2011). Information processing efficiency and regulation at five months. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 239247.Google Scholar
Diego, M. A., Field, T., Jones, N. A., Hernandez-Reif, M., Cullen, C., Schanberg, S., & Kuhn, C. (2004). EEG responses to mock facial expressions by infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 150162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekas, N. V., Braungart-Rieker, J. M., & Messinger, D. S. (2018). The development of infant emotion regulation: Time is of the essence. In Cole, P. M & Hollenstein, T. (Eds.), Emotion regulation: A matter of time (pp. 4969). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ekas, N. V., Lickenbrock, D. M., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (2013). Developmental trajectories of emotion regulation across infancy: Do age and the social partner influence temporal patterns. Infancy, 18, 729754.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728.Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, S., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The perception of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 213. http://doi.org/10.1080/17405620601046832Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2003). Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, R., Granat, A., Pariente, C., Kanety, H., Kuint, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2009). Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 919927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 2230231.Google Scholar
Field, A. P., & Lester, K. J. (2010). Is there room for “development” in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13, 315332.Google Scholar
Field, T. M., Cohen, D., Garcia, R. & Collins, R. (1983). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by term and preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 485489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. M., Pickens, J., Fox, N. A., Gonzalez, J., & Nawrocki, T. (1998). Facial expression and EEG responses to happy and sad faces/voices by 3-month-old infants of depressed mothers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 485494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 726.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., Hane, A. A., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2006). Psychophysiological methods for the study of developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., 381426. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedlmeier, W., & Trommsdorff, G. (1999). Emotion regulation in early childhood: A cross-cultural comparison between German and Japanese toddlers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30(6), 684711.Google Scholar
Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2015). Theories of temperament development. In Wright, J. D. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of social & behavioral sciences (2nd ed., pp. 191–198. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Galati, D., & Lavelli, M. (1997). Neonate and infant emotion expression perceived by adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21(1), 5783.Google Scholar
García-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55(3), 10051019.Google Scholar
Gewirtz, J. L., & Peláez-Nogueras, M. (1992). Social referencing as a learned process. In Feinman, S. (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 151173). New York, NY: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1982). Toward a theory of infant temperament. In Emde, R. N. & Harmon, R. J. (Eds.), The development of attachment and affiliative systems (pp. 161193). New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). The laboratory temperament assessment battery (LAB-TAB): Locomotor version. Technical manual. Madison, WI: Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Graham, A. M., Fisher, P. A., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2013). What sleeping babies hear: A functional MRI Study of interparental conflict and infants’ emotion processing. Psychological Science, 24, 782789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granat, A., Gadassi, R., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., & Feldman, R. (2017). Maternal depression and anxiety, social synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive emotions. Emotion, 17, 1127.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3, 816.Google Scholar
Grossmann, T. (2010). The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 219236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunning, M., Halligan, S. L., & Murray, L. (2013). Contributions of maternal and infant factors to infant responding to the still face paradigm: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 319328.Google Scholar
Haley, D. W., & Stansbury, K. (2003). Infant stress and parent responsiveness: Regulation of physiology and behavior during still-face and reunion. Child Development, 74(5), 15341546.Google Scholar
Hane, A. A., Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., & Marshall, P. J. (2008). Behavioral reactivity and approach–withdrawal bias in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1491.Google Scholar
Hart, S., Field, T., Del Valle, C., & Peláez-Nogueras, M. (1998). Depressed mothers’ interactions with their one-year-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 519525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herschkowitz, N. (2000). Neurological bases of behavioral development in infancy. Brain and Development, 22, 411416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez-Reif, M., Field, T., Diego, M., Vera, Y., & Pickens, J. (2006). Happy faces are habituated more slowly by infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 131135.Google Scholar
Hiatt, S. W., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. N. (1979). Facial patterning and infant emotional expression: Happiness, surprise, and fear. Child Development, 50(4), 10201035.Google Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P. (2008). Developmental psychopathology as a scientific discipline. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P., (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 328). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emotion regulation (Vol. 8). New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Ingram, R. E., & Luxton, D. (2005). Vulnerability-stress models. In Hankin, B. L. & Abela, J. R. Z. (Eds.), Development of psychopathology: A vulnerability-stress perspective. (pp. 3246). New York, NY: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1979). The maximally discriminative facial movement coding system (MAX). Newark: Instructional Resources Center, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E., Dougherty, L., & Hembree, E. (1983). A system for identifying affect expressions by holistic judgments (AFFEX). Newark: Instructional Resources Center, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development I: Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In Osofsky, J. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 494554). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Infant predictors of inhibited and uninhibited profiles. Psychological Science, 2(1), 4044.Google Scholar
Kiel, E. J., & Kalomiris, A. E. (2015). Current themes in understanding children’s emotion regulation as developing from within the parent–child relationship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiff, C. J., Lengua, L. J., & Zalewski, M. (2011). Nature and nurturing: Parenting in the context of child temperament. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 251301.Google Scholar
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83, 12751289.Google Scholar
Kim, B. R., Stifter, C. A., Philbrook, L. E., & Teti, D. M. (2014). Infant emotion regulation: Relations to bedtime emotional availability, attachment security, and temperament. Infant Behavior and Development, 37, 480490.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199214.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343354.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (2002). Commentary: The codevelopments of attention and emotion regulation. Infancy, 3, 199208.Google Scholar
Lamb, M. E. (2015). Processes underlying social, emotional, and personality development. In Lamb, M. E. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, socioemotional processes (Vol. 3, pp. 110). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Malphurs, J. E., Raag, T., Field, T., Pickens, J., & Pelaez-Nogueras, M. (1996). Touch by intrusive and withdrawn mothers with depressive symptoms. Early Development and Parenting: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 5, 111115.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matias, R., & Cohn, J. (1993). Are MAX-specified infant facial expressions during face-to-face interaction consistent with differential emotions theory? Developmental Psychology, 29, 524531.Google Scholar
Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29, 120162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, S., Brown, K. M., Taber-Thomas, B. C., LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2017). Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion, 17, 874883.Google Scholar
Morales, S., Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 2641.Google Scholar
Morales, S., Ram, N., Buss, K. A., Cole, P. M., Helm, J. L., & Chow, S. M. (2017). Age-related changes in the dynamics of fear-related regulation in early childhood. Developmental Science, 21, e12633.Google Scholar
Moore, G. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Infants’ vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother–infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40, 10681080.Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Robinson, L. R., & Eisenberg, N. (2006). Applying a multimethod perspective to the study of developmental psychology. In Eid, M. & Diener, E. (Eds.), Handbook of multimethod measurement in psychology (pp. 371384). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., & Cooper, P. J. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes in infancy. Child Development, 79, 10491064.Google Scholar
Nicol-Harper, R., Harvey, A. G., & Stein, A. (2007). Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 161167.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. R., Hughes, B., McCrae, K., Cooper, J. C., Weber, J., … Gross, J. J. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: Common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychological Science, 20, 13221331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02459.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oster, H., Hegley, D., & Nagel, L. (1992). Adult judgments and fine-grained analysis of infant facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 28, 11151131Google Scholar
Parritz, R. H. (1996). A descriptive analysis of toddler coping in challenging circumstances. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 171180.Google Scholar
Paulson, J. F., Dauber, S., & Leiferman, J. A. (2006). Individual and combined effects of postpartum depression in mothers and fathers on parenting behavior. Pediatrics, 118, 659668.Google Scholar
Peltola, M. J., Hietanen, J. K., Forssman, L., & Leppänen, J. M. (2013). The emergence and stability of the attentional bias to fearful faces in infancy. Infancy, 18, 905926.Google Scholar
Pérez-Edgar, K. (2019). Through the looking glass: Temperament and emotion as separate and interwoven constructs. In LoBue, V., Pérez-Edgar, K., & Buss, K. (Eds.), Handbook of emotional development (pp. 139168). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Pérez-Edgar, K., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Application of cognitive-neuroscience techniques to the study of anxiety-related processing biases in children. In Hadwin, J. & Field, A. (Eds.), Information processing biases in child and adolescent anxiety (pp. 183206). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Pérez-Edgar, K., & Hastings, P. D. (2018). Emotion development from an experimental and individual differences lens. In Wixted, J. T. (Ed.), The Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 4, 4th ed., pp. 289321. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Pérez-Edgar, K., Kujawa, A., Nelson, S. K., Cole, C., & Zapp, D. J. (2013). The relation between electroencephalogram asymmetry and attention biases to threat at baseline and under stress. Brain and Cognition, 82(3), 337343.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2014). Developing attention: behavioral and brain mechanisms. Advances in Neuroscience, 1, 405094. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/405094Google Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B., & Mason, J. (2011). Do facial expressions develop before birth? PloS One, 6, e24081.Google Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B., (2013). Can healthy fetuses show facial expressions of “pain” or “distress”? PloS One, 8, e65530.Google Scholar
Reznik, S. J., & Allen, J. J. (2018). Frontal asymmetry as a mediator and moderator of emotion: An updated review. Psychophysiology, 55(1), e12965.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3786). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silvers, J. A., Buhle, J. T., & Ochsner, K. N. (2013). The neuroscience of emotion regulation: Basic mechanisms and their role in development, aging and psychopathology. In Ochsner, K. N. and Kosslyn, S. M. (Eds.). The handbook of cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 1, pp. 5278). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Corey, J. M. (2001). Vagal regulation and observed social behavior in infancy. Social Development, 10, 189201.Google Scholar
Sylvester, C. M., Smyser, C. D., Smyser, T., Kenley, J., Ackerman Jr, J. J., Shimony, J. S., … Rogers, C. E. (2017). Cortical functional connectivity evident after birth and behavioral inhibition at age 2. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(2), 180187.Google Scholar
Thomas, J. C., Letourneau, N., Campbell, T. S., Tomfohr-Madsen, L., & Giesbrecht, G. F. (2017). Developmental origins of infant emotion regulation: Mediation by temperamental negativity and moderation by maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 53, 611628.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112119.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383403.Google Scholar
Vaughn, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The temporal relationship between infant heart rate acceleration and crying in an aversive situation. Child Development, 50, 565567.Google Scholar
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1998). The impact of maternal psychiatric illness on infant development. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 5361.Google Scholar
Yrttiaho, S., Forssman, L., Kaatiala, J., & Leppänen, J. M. (2014). Developmental precursors of social brain networks: The emergence of attentional and cortical sensitivity to facial expressions in 5 to 7 months old infants. PloS One, 9(6), e100811.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×