Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T12:35:48.623Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

29 - Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Parent–Infant Interactions

from Part VI - Emotional and Social Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2020

Jeffrey J. Lockman
Affiliation:
Tulane University, Louisiana
Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Each day more than three-quarters of a million adults around the world experience the joys and heartaches just as they do the rewards and fears of becoming parents to a newborn infant. Each infant is an individual, of course, as is each parent and each parent–infant dyad. Yet, parents and infants around the globe share a large number of commonalities. No matter their homeland, parents have the same responsibilities to guide their infants’ survival and success in life, and their infants have the same biological needs and must meet and succeed at the same developmental tasks and challenges. Although infancy encompasses only a small fraction of the life span, it is a period that parents the world over attend to and invest in. Parenting an infant is a 168-hour-a-week job. With good reason: Parenting responsibilities are arguably the greatest during the time of their child’s infancy because human infants are totally dependent on caregiving and their ability to cope alone is minimal.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development
Brain, Behavior, and Cultural Context
, pp. 805 - 832
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of attachment. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Allik, J., & McCrae, R. R. (2004). Toward a geography of personality traits: Patterns of profiles across 36 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 1328.Google Scholar
Azhari, A., Leck, W. Q., Gabrieli, G., Bizzego, A., Rigo, P., Setoh, P., … Esposito, G. (2019). Parenting stress undermines mother–child brain-to-brain synchrony: A hyperscanning study. Scientific Reports, 9, 11407.Google Scholar
Bales, K. L. (2014). Comparative and developmental perspectives on oxytocin and vasopressin. In Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Mechanisms of social connection: From brain to group (pp. 1531). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Myers, A. (2019). The evolution of parenting and evolutionary approaches to childrearing. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (3rd ed., pp. 329). New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 179197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Form and function: Implications for studies of culture and human development. Culture & Psychology, 1, 123137.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2001). Arnold Lucius Gesell. Pediatrics and Related Topics/Pädiatrie und Grenzgebiete, 40, 395409.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Vols. 1–5). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2012). Cultural approaches to parenting. Parenting, Science and Practice, 12, 212221.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2013). Parenting and child mental health: A cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry, 12(3), 258265.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Lerner, R. M., Bornstein, M. H., & Leventhal, T. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 4: Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems (7th ed., pp. 55132). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Determinants of parenting. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (3rd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 180270). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Parenting in acculturation: Two contemporary research designs and what they tell us. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 195200.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2019). Immigrant parenthood. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 4: Social conditions and applied parenting (3rd ed., pp. 198233). New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Haynes, O. M., Suwalsky, J. T., & Bakeman, R. (2012). Modalities of infant–mother interaction in Japanese, Japanese American immigrant, and European American dyads. Child Development, 83, 20732088.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Esposito, G., & Motti-Stephanidi, F. (in press). Coregulation: A multilevel approach via biology and behavior. Journal of Self-Regulation and Regulation.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., Haynes, O. M., Belsky, J., Azuma, H., Kwak, K., … Galperín, C. (2007). Maternal personality and parenting cognitions in cross-cultural perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 193209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Azuma, H., Galperín, C., Maital, S., Ogino, M., … Wright, B. (1998). A cross-national study of self-evaluations and attributions in parenting: Argentina, Belgium, France, Israel, Italy, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 34, 662676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., & Lansford, J. E. (2010). Parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), The handbook of cultural developmental science. Part 1: Domains of development across cultures (pp. 259277). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Cote, L. R., Haynes, O. M. H., & Suwalsky, J. T. D. (2015). Mother–infant contingent vocalizations in 11 countries. Psychological Science, 26, 12721284.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Rigo, P., Esposito, G., Swain, J. E., Suwalsky, J. T. D., … Venuti, P. (2017). Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114(45), E9465E9473.Google ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Suwalsky, J. T. D. (2012). A longitudinal process analysis of mother–child emotional relationships in a rural Appalachian European American community. American Journal of Community Psychology, 50, 89100.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Sawyer, J. (2005). Family systems. In McCartney, K. & Phillips, D. (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 381398). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., Rahn, C., Galperin, C. Z., Pecheux, M. G., Amour, M., … Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 28, 593603.Google Scholar
Bovet, M. C., Dasen, P. R., & Inhelder, B. (1974). Etapes de l’intelligence sensori-mortice chez l’enfant Baoulé. Archives de Psychologie, 41, 363386.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The evolution of environmental models in developmental research. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), The handbook of child psychology. Vol. 1: Theories of development (pp. 358414). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Lerner, R. M. (Ed.), Theoretical models of human development. Volume 1 of the Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., pp. 793828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Caria, A., de Falco, S., Venuti, P., Lee, S., Esposito, G., Rigo, P., … Bornstein, M.H. (2012). Species-specific response to human infant faces in the premotor cortex. NeuroImage, 60, 884893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohler, B., & Paul, S. (2019). Psychoanalysis and parenthood. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 563600). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232.Google Scholar
Cummings, E. E., & Warmuth, K. (2019). Parenting and attachment. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 4: Social conditions and applied parenting (3rd ed., pp. 374400). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Davis, O. S. P., Haworth, C. M. A., Lewis, C. M., & Plomin, R. (2012). Visual analysis of geocoded twin data puts nature and nurture on the map. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 867874.Google Scholar
Dennis, W., & Dennis, M. G. (1940). The effect of cradling practices upon the onset of walking in Hopi children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 56, 77.Google Scholar
Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1988). A sociobiological perspective on the development of human reproductive strategies. In MacDonald, K. B. (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp. 340372). New York, NY: Springer New York.Google Scholar
Dudin, A., McGowan, P., Wu, R., Fleming, A. S., & Li, M. (2019). Psychobiology of maternal behavior in nonhuman mammals. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (3rd ed., pp. 3077). New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, R. R., Davies, T. J., Harris, N. C., & Gavin, M. C. (2010). Global drivers of human pathogen richness and prevalence. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 277(1694), 25872595.Google ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R. M., & Hundert, E. M. (2002). Defining and assessing professional competence. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 226235.Google Scholar
Esposito, G., Nakazawa, J., Ogawa, S., Stival, R., Kawashima, A., Putnick, D L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2014). Baby, you light-up my face: Culture-general physiological responses to infants and culture-specific cognitive judgements of adults. PloS One, 9, e106705.Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Schneiderman, I., Weisman, O., & Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2010). Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent–infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 11331141.Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., & Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2011). Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent–infant synchrony: Considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Developmental Science, 14, 752761.Google Scholar
French, V. (2019). Ancient history of parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (3rd ed., pp. 287319). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psycho-analysis. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 5577.Google Scholar
Geber, M. (1956). Développement psycho-moteur de l’enfant Africain. Courrier, 6, 1729.Google Scholar
Geber, M. (1958). The psychomotor development of African children in the first year, and the influence of maternal behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 185195.Google Scholar
Geber, M., & Dean, R. F. (1957a). Gesell tests on African children. Pediatrics, 20, 10551065.Google Scholar
Geber, M., (1957b). The state of development of newborn African children. Lancet, 272, 12161219.Google Scholar
Gesell, A., & Amatruda, C. S. 1945. The embryology of behavior. New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. (2010). Culture. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 320). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. J., Cashmore, J., Cotton, S., & Knight, R. (1984). Mothers’ developmental timetables in two cultural groups. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 193205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, R. (1954). The abilities of babies. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Harwood, R., Leyendecker, B., Carlson, V., Asencio, M., & Miller, A. (2002). Parenting among Latino families in the U.S. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 4: Applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 2146). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Lamb, M. E., Shannon, D., Leyendecker, B., & Schölmerich, A. (1998). Culture and early infancy among Central African foragers and farmers. Developmental Psychology, 34, 653661.Google Scholar
Holden, G. W. (1997). Parents and the dynamics of child rearing. New York, NY: Perseus.Google Scholar
Holden, G. W., & Smith, M. M. (2019). Parenting cognitions. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 681–721). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hopkins, B., & Westra, T. (1989). Maternal expectations of their infants’ development: Some cultural differences. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 384390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, B., (1990). Motor development, maternal expectations, and the role of handling. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 117122.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature: A history of mothers, infants, and natural selection. New York, NY: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Hsu, H. C., & Fogel, A. (2003). Social regulatory effects of infant nondistress vocalization on maternal behavior. Developmental Psychology, 39, 976991.Google Scholar
Kärtner, J., Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R. D., Chaudhary, N., & Su, Y. (2008). Similarities and differences in contingency experiences of 3-month-olds across sociocultural contexts. Infant Behavior & Development, 31, 488500.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Jensen, H., … Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories: A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 155172.Google Scholar
Klahr, A. M., & Burt, S. A. (2014). Elucidating the etiology of individual differences in parenting: A meta-analysis of behavioral genetic research. Psychology Bulletin, 140, 544586.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Development of mutual responsiveness between parents and their young children. Child Development, 75, 16571676.Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2016). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 545558.Google Scholar
Lambert, K. G., & Kinsley, C. H. (2012). Brain and behavioral modifications that accompany the onset of motherhood. Parenting: Science and Practice, 12, 7489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leiderman, P. H., Tulkin, S. R., & Rosenfeld, A. H. (Eds.). (1977). Culture and infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lusk, D., & Lewis, M. (1972). Mother–infant interaction and infant development among the Wolof of Senegal. Human Development, 15, 5869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 10061017.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K., & MacDonald, T. M. (2010). The peptide that binds: A systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 18, 121.Google Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2003). The mating system of foragers in the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 282306.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new Big Five: Fundamental principles for an integrative science of personality. American Psychologist, 61, 204217.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Terraciano, A., & 78 members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547561.Google Scholar
Mead, M., & MacGregor, F. C. (1951). Growth and culture. New York, NY: Putnam’s Sons.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Wang, S. -H., Sandel, T., & Cho, G. E. (2002). Self-esteem as folk theory: A comparison of European American and Taiwanese mothers’ beliefs. Parenting, Science and Practice, 2, 209239.Google Scholar
Murray, L., Richards, M. P. M., & Nihouarn-Sigurdardottir, J. (2019). Mothering. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 3663). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. (1982). Talking to children in Western Samoa. Language in Society, 11, 77104.Google Scholar
Packer, M., & Cole, M. (2015). Culture in development. In Bornstein, M. H. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp. 43111). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (2002). Intuitive parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 183203). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 7190.Google Scholar
Rebelsky, F. G. (1967). Infancy in two cultures. Nederlands Tijdschrift Voor de Psychologie En Haar Grensgebieden, 22, 379385.Google Scholar
Rebelsky, F. G. (1972). Cross-cultural studies of mother–infant interaction. Human Development, 15, 128130.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, M. K., & Roer-Strier, D. (2001). Cultural differences in mothers’ developmental goals and ethnotheories. International Journal of Psychology, 36, 2031.Google Scholar
Serpell, R. (1990). Audience, culture and psychological explanation: A reformulation of the emic-etic problem in cross-cultural psychology. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 12, 99132.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: From input to interactions. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 3149). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27, 501532.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., … Levinson, S. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1058710592.Google Scholar
Super, C. M. (1976). Environmental effects on motor development: The case of “African Infant Precocity.” Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 18, 561567.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Cyphers, L., Toda, S., & Ogino, M. (1992). Language and play at one year: A comparison of toddlers and mothers in the United States and Japan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 1942.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Kahana-Kalman, R. (2009). Mothers’ views at the transition to a new baby: Variation across ethnic groups. Parenting, Science and Practice, 9, 3655.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. M. (1970). Physical growth. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 77155). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Tomlinson, M., Bornstein, M. H., Marlow, M., & Swartz, L. (2014). Imbalances in the knowledge about infant mental health in rich and poor countries: Too little progress in bridging the gap. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35, 624629.Google Scholar
UNICEF (2007). The state of the world’s children 2008: Child survival. New York, NY: UNICEF.Google Scholar
van Egeren, L. A., Barratt, M. S., & Roach, M. A. (2001). Mother–infant responsiveness: Timing, mutual regulation, and interactional context. Developmental Psychology, 37, 684697.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wachs, T. D. (2015). Assessing bioecological influences. In Lerner, R. M., Bornstein, M. H., & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 4: Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems (7th ed., pp. 811846). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Werner, E. E. (1972). Infants around the world: Cross-cultural studies of psychomotor development from birth to two years. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3, 111134.Google Scholar
West, M. (1914). Infant care. Washington, DC: US Children’s Bureau.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M., & Whiting, B. B. (1960). Contributions of anthropology to the methods of studying child rearing. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in child development (pp. 918944). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Wilson, T. P. (2005). An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 957968.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×