Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T19:37:31.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - The Evolution of Pride and Shame

from Part IV - Group Living

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

What drives us to climb that highest mountain? And what do we subsequently feel when we realize we are extremely bad at mountain climbing and decide to fake an injury to get helicoptered back to base camp? The emotions that shape these events and our responses to them – pride and shame – play a central role in motivating and regulating many of people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (Tangney & Tracy, 2012). These self-conscious emotions drive people to work hard to succeed (Stipek, 1995; Weiner, 1985) and to behave in moral and pro-social ways in their relationships (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994; Leith & Baumeister, 1998; Retzinger, 1987). Yet despite their centrality to psychological functioning, pride and shame did not receive the same attention from early emotion researchers as the so-called basic emotions, such as joy, fear, and sadness (Campos, 1995; Fischer & Tangney, 1995).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D. B. (1981). Motivational systems of social behavior in male rats and monkeys: Are they homologous? Aggressive Behavior, 7, 518.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, white women. Health Psychology, 19, 586592.Google Scholar
Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Human emotions: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review, 8, 173186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 13621377.Google Scholar
Ashton-James, C. E., & Tracy, J. L. (2012). Pride and prejudice: How feelings about the self influence judgments of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 466476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babcock, M. K., & Sabini, J. (1990). On differentiating embarrassment from shame. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 151169.Google Scholar
Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction Process Analysis: A Method for the Study of Small Groups. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology, 16, 553572.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, Sex, and Status. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science, 17, 319325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243267.Google Scholar
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2014). The puzzling attractiveness of male shame. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 2947.Google Scholar
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2017). Emotivational psychology: How distinct emotions facilitate fundamental motives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11, e12303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Domitrovich, C. E., & Crnic, K. (1997). Temperament and parenting antecedents of individual differences in 3-year-old pride and shame reactions. Child Development, 68, 456466.Google Scholar
Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M. (1980). Status organizing processes. Annual Review of Sociology, 6, 479508.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1993). Egalitarian society and reverse dominance hierarchy. Current Anthropology, 34, 227254.Google Scholar
Boiger, M., Mesquita, B., Uchida, Y., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). Condoned or condemned the situational affordance of anger and shame in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 540553.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2008). Human nature and individual differences: Evolution of human personality. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A., eds., Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 2960.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J. (1995). Foreword. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. ixxi.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In Haith, M. M. & Campos, J. J., eds., Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. II): Infancy and Developmental Psychobiology. New York: Wiley, pp. 783915.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 1935.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Sinclair, S., & Johnson, S. L. (2010). Authentic and hubristic pride: Differential relations to aspects of goal regulation, affect, and self-control. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 698703.Google Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 334347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D. (2003). The American national conversation about (everything but) shame. Social Research, 70, 10751108.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. Handbook of Emotions, 2, 91115.Google Scholar
Cowlishaw, G., & Dunbar, R. I. (1991). Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Animal Behaviour, 41, 10451056.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2001). The neural circuitry of emotion and affective style: Prefrontal cortex and amygdala contributions. Social Science Information, 40, 1137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt, Cognition and Emotion, 21, 10251042.Google Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2011). A functionalist account of shame-induced behavior. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 939946.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1989). Peacemaking among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaner, R. O., Khera, A. V., & Platt, M. L. (2005). Monkeys pay per view: Adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques. Current Biology, 15, 543548.Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004). Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 124131.Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., Mycek, P. J., & Zaldivar, F. (2008). Negative social evaluation – but not mere social presence – elicits cortisol responses in the laboratory. Health Psychology, 27, 116121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Psychobiological responses to social self threat: Functional or detrimental? Self and Identity, 8, 270285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eibl-Eisenfeldt, I. (1989). Human Ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic? Emotion Review, 3, 364370.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164, 8688.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 12081210.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgment of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257333.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 301331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2004). Shame in two cultures: Implications for evolutionary approaches. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 207262.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2007). From appeasement to conformity: Evolutionary and cultural perspectives on shame, competition, and cooperation. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 174193.Google Scholar
Fischer, J., Fischer, P., Englich, B., Aydin, N., & Frey, D. (2011). Empower my decisions: The effects of power gestures on confirmatory information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 11461154.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Tangney, J. P. (1995). Self-conscious emotions and the affect revolution: Framework and overview. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 324.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300319.Google Scholar
Fried, M. H. (1967). The Evolution of Political Society: An Essay in Political Anthropology. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and brain sciences, 23, 573587.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (1997). The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 113147.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (2007). The evolution of shame as a marker for relationship security: A biopsychosocial approach. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 283309.Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., Castano, E., Espinosa, P., & Brown, R. (2008). Shame expressions reduce recipient’s insult from outgroup reparations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 519526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, H. J. (1980). Playground as laboratory: Naturalistic studies of appeasement, altruism, and the omega child. In Omark, D. R., Strayer, F. F., & Freeman, D. G., eds., Dominance Relations: An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interaction. New York: Garland, pp. 341357.Google Scholar
Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (1986). From an attributional theory of emotion to developmental psychology: A round-trip ticket? Social Cognition, 4, 152179.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Positive emotional traits and ambitious goals among people at risk for mania: The need for specificity. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2, 179190.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., Culver, J. L., Johnson, S. L., et al. (2009). Do positive emotions predict symptomatic change in bipolar disorder? Bipolar Disorders, 11, 330336.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Johnson, S. L. (2011). A discrete emotions approach to positive emotion disturbance in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 4052.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more expressions and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 225266.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Olthof, T., Terwogt, M. M., & Hardman, C. E. (1987). Children’s knowledge of the situations that provoke emotion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 319343.Google Scholar
Hart, D., & Karmel, M. P. (1996). Self-awareness and self-knowledge in humans, apes, and monkeys. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., & Parker, S. T., eds., Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 325347.Google Scholar
Hart, D., & Matsuba, M. K. (2007). The development of pride and moral life. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 114133.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspective on the self-system. In Hetherington, E. M., ed., Mussen, P. H., series ed., Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol 4. Socialization, Personality, and Social Development, 4th ed. New York: Wiley, pp. 275385.Google Scholar
Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., & Leeson, P. (2009). The longitudinal links between shame and increasing hostility during adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 841844.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766794.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.Google Scholar
Hill, J. (1984). Prestige and reproductive success in man. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 7795.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1989). Hunter–gatherers of the New World. American Scientist, 77, 436443.Google Scholar
Huang, L., Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Guillory, L. E. (2010). Powerful postures versus powerful roles: Which is the proximate correlate of thought and behavior? Psychological Science, 22, 95102.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The Face of Emotion. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Jacquet, J. (2011). Is shame necessary? In Brockman, M., ed., Future Science: Essays from the Cutting Edge. New York: Vintage Books, pp. 128140.Google Scholar
Jacquet, J., Hauert, C., Traulsen, A., & Milinski, M. (2011). Shame and honor drive cooperation. Biology Letters, 7, 899901.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: Analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 81123.Google Scholar
Keltner, D. (1995). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 441454.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250270.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Young, R. C., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Appeasement in human emotion, social practice, and personality. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 359374.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006). Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A., & Kenrick, D. T., eds., Evolution and Social Psychology. Madison, CT: Psychosocial Press, pp. 115142.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 890903.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1968). Social Organization of Hamadryas Baboons. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lagattuta, K. H., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). The development of self-conscious emotions: Cognitive processes and social influences. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 91113.Google Scholar
Lancaster, J. B. (1976). Primate Behavior and the Emergence of Human Culture. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129141.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518530.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, Women and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. Journal of Personality, 66, 137.Google Scholar
Leresche, L. A. (1976). Dyadic play in Hamadryas baboons. Behaviour, 57, 190205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2011). Basic emotion questions. Emotion Review, 3, 379386.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M., eds., Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 623636.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a function of children’s gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, 630638.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). The emotional origins of a social learning bias does the pride expression cue copying? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 492499.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., Parr, L. A., & Price, S. (2010). Do the chimpanzee bluff display and human pride expression share evolutionary origins? Poster presented at the 33rd Society for Personality and Social Psychology Pre-Conference on Evolutionary Psychology, Las Vegas, NV.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., Tracy, J. L., & Shariff, A. F. (2012). Status signals: Adaptive benefits of displaying and observing the nonverbal expressions of pride and shame. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 390406.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1936). The role of dominance in the social and sexual behavior of infra-human primates: IV. The determination of hierarchy in pairs and in a group. Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 49, 161198.Google Scholar
Mazur, A. (1983). Hormones, aggression, and dominance in humans. In Svare, B. B, ed., Hormones and Aggressive Behavior. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 563576.Google Scholar
Mazur, A., & Lamb, T. A. (1980). Testosterone, status, and mood in human males. Hormones and Behavior, 14, 236246.Google Scholar
Mehta, P. H., & Josephs, R. A. (2010). Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: Evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 898906.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A sociodynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 298302.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2004). Self-conscious emotions as dynamic cultural processes. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 161166.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Wang, S.-H., Sandel, T., & Cho, G. E. (2002). Self-esteem as folk theory: A comparison of European American and Taiwanese mothers’ beliefs. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 209239.Google Scholar
Morrison, D., & Gilbert, P. (2001). Social rank, shame and anger in primary and secondary psychopaths. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 12, 330356.Google Scholar
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2011). When dynamic, the head and face alone can express pride. Emotion, 11, 990993.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (1991). What good is feeling bad? Sciences, 31, 3037.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009). Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. American Psychologist, 64, 129139.Google Scholar
Newman, M. L., Sellers, J. G., & Josephs, R. A. (2005). Testosterone, cognition, and social status. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 205211.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2006). Hiding from Humanity: Disgust, Shame, and the Law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and pride across the lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 10611071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Watt, D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emotion Review, 3, 387396.Google Scholar
Proeve, M. J., & Howells, K. (2006). Effects of remorse and shame and criminal justice experience on judgements about a sex offender. Psychology, Crime and Law, 12, 145161.Google Scholar
Randles, D., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). Nonverbal displays of shame predict relapse and declining health in recovering alcoholics. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 149155.Google Scholar
Randles, D., & Tracy, J. L. (2015). Shame. In Segal, R. & Stuckrad, K., eds., Vocabulary for the Study of Religion. Leiden: Brill, pp. 339343.Google Scholar
Retzinger, S. M. (1987). Resentment and laughter: Video studies of the shame–rage spiral. In Lewis, H. B., ed., The Role of Shame in Symptom Formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 151181.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2008). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A., eds., Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 375398.Google Scholar
Robinson, R., Roberts, W. L., Strayer, J., & Koopman, R. (2007). Empathy and emotional responsiveness in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents. Social Development, 16, 555579.Google Scholar
Rose, R. M., Gordon, T. P., & Bernstein, T. S. (1972). Plasma testosterone levels in the male rhesus: Influences of sexual and social stimuli. Science, 178, 643645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, E. L., & Ekman, P. (1994). Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 201229.Google Scholar
Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192199.Google Scholar
Schaller, G. E. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2009). Knowing who’s boss: Implicit perceptions of status from the nonverbal expression of pride. Emotion, 9, 631639.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 395399.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., & Henrich, J. (2010). Further thoughts on the evolution of pride’s two facets: A response to Clark. Emotion Review, 2, 399400.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Tracy, J. L., & Markusoff, J. L. (2012). (Implicitly) judging a book by its cover: The power of pride and shame expressions in shaping judgments of social status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 11781193.Google Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 10611086.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (1987). Correlates of agonistic and competitive interactions in pregnant baboons. American Journal of Primatology, 12, 479495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 2939.Google Scholar
Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A. (2009). The voice conveys specific emotions: Evidence from vocal burst displays. Emotion, 9, 838846.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1995). The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 237254.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1998). Differences between Americans and Chinese in the circumstances evoking pride, shame and guilt. Journal of Cross-Culture Psychology, 29, 616629.Google Scholar
Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 100.Google Scholar
Strayer, F. F., & Strayer, J. (1976). An ethological analysis of social agonism and dominance relations among preschool children. Child Development, 47, 980989.Google Scholar
Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Facial expression form and function. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 1, 148149.Google Scholar
Takahashi, H., Matsuura, M., Koeda, M., et al. (2008). Brain activations during judgments of positive self-conscious emotion and positive basic emotion: Pride and joy. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 898903.Google Scholar
Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., et al. (2004). Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23, 967974.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame and guilt: Development of the Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 102111.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. (2002). Shame and Guilt in Interpersonal Relationships. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Fisher, K. W., eds. (1995). Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Tracy, J. L. (2012). Self-conscious emotions. In Leary, M. & Tangney, J. P., eds., Handbook of Self and Identity, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 446478.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., Hill-Barlow, D., Marschall, D. E., & Gramzow, R. (1996). Relation of shame and guilt to constructive versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 797809.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1989). Causal attributions and children’s emotional understanding. In Saarni, C. & Harris, P. L., eds., Children’s Understanding of Emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 117150.Google Scholar
Tiedens, L., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Sentimental stereotypes: Emotional expectations for high- and low-status group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 560575.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect Imagery Consciousness: The Positive Affects, Vol. 1. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L. (2016). Take Pride: Why the Deadliest Sin Holds the Secret to Human Success. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. (2011). Happy guys finish last: The impact of emotional expressions on sexual attraction. Emotion, 11, 13791387.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., Robins, R. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2009). Authentic and hubristic pride: The affective core of self-esteem and narcissism. Self and Identity, 8, 196213.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous display of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1165511660.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Prehn, C. (2012). The use of contextual knowledge to differentiate hubristic and authentic pride from a single non-verbal expression. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 1424.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Paksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3, 397405.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004a). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 103125.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004b). Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15, 194197.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007a). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 506525.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007b). Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 147150.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007c). The prototypical pride expression: Development of a nonverbal behavioral coding system. Emotion, 7, 789801.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008a). The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for crosscultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516530.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008b). The automaticity of emotion recognition. Emotion, 7, 789801.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2005). Can children recognize the pride expression? Emotion, 5, 251257.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Schriber, R. A. (2009). Development of a FACS-verified set of basic and self-conscious emotion expressions. Emotion, 9, 554559.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., & Cheng, J. T. (2010). A naturalist’s view of pride. Emotion Review, 2, 163177.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., Zhao, W., & Henrich, J. (2013). Cross-cultural evidence that the pride expression is a universal automatic status signal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 163180.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In von Cranach, M. & Vine, I., eds., Social Communication and Movement: Studies of Interaction and Expression in Man and Chimpanzee. New York: Academic Press, pp. 75162.Google Scholar
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution: Some lessons from the past. American Psychologist, 63, 182196.Google Scholar
Verbeke, W., Belschak, F., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2004). The adaptive consequences of pride in personal selling. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 32, 386402.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.Google Scholar
Weidman, A. C., Cheng, J. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2018). The psychological structure of humility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 153178.Google Scholar
Weidman, A. C., Tracy, J. L., & Elliot, A. J. (2016). The benefits of following your pride: Authentic pride promotes achievement. Journal of Personality, 84, 607622.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548573.Google Scholar
Weisfeld, G. E., & Beresford, J. M. (1982). Erectness of posture as an indicator of dominance or success in humans. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 113131.Google Scholar
Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2008). Pride and perseverance: The motivational role of pride. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 10071017.Google Scholar
Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2009). Pride: Adaptive social emotion or seventh sin? Psychological Science, 20, 284288.Google Scholar
Yang, M. L., Yang, C. C., & Chiou, W. B. (2010). When guilt leads to other orientation and shame leads to egocentric self-focus: effects of differential priming of negative affects on perspective taking. Social Behavior and Personality, 38, 605614.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×