Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T05:28:49.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction

Assembling the Building Blocks of Cognition in a Non-linear Dynamical System of Development

from Part II - Fundamentals of Cognitive Development from Infancy to Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2022

Olivier Houdé
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Grégoire Borst
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Get access

Summary

The scientific study of cognitive development in young children traces its roots back to Jean Piaget, a pioneer of this field in the twentieth century (Piaget, 1954, 1983). From infancy to adolescence, children progress through four psychological stages: (1) the sensorimotor stage, from birth to two years (when cognitive functioning is based primarily on biological reactions, motor skills, and perceptions); (2) the preoperational stage, from two to seven years (when symbolic thought and language become prevalent, but reasoning is illogical by adult standards); (3) the concrete operations stage, from seven to twelve years (when logical reasoning abilities emerge but are limited to concrete objects and events); and (4) the formal operations stage, at approximately twelve years (when thinking about abstract, hypothetical, and contrary-to-fact ideas becomes possible).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aron, A., Robbins, T., & Poldrack, R. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, A., Robbins, T., & Poldrack, R. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 177185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antell, S., & Keating, D. (1983). Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child Development, 54, 695701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infancy. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 181204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bell, M., & Fox, N. (1992). The relations between frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive development during infancy. Child Development, 63, 11421163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D., & Harnishfeger, K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 4871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borst, G., & Houdé, O. (2014). Inhibitory control as a core mechanism for cognitive development and learning at school. Perspectives on Language and Literacy, 17, 4144.Google Scholar
Borst, G., Moutier, S., & Houdé, O. (2013a). Negative priming in logicomathematical reasoning: The cost of blocking your intuition. In De Neys, W., & Osman, M. (eds.), New Approaches in Reasoning Research – Current Issues in Thinking & Reasoning (pp. 3450). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Borst, G., Pineau, A., Poirel, N., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O. (2013b). Inhibitory control efficiency in a Piaget-like class-inclusion task in school-age children and adults: A developmental negative priming study. Developmental Psychology, 49, 13661374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borst, G., Poirel, N., Pineau, A., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O. (2012). Inhibitory control in number-conservation and class-inclusion tasks: A neo-Piagetian inter-tasks priming study. Cognitive Development, 27, 283298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Neys, W., Rossi, S., & Houdé, O. (2013). Bats, balls, and substitution sensitivity: Cognitive misers are no happy fools. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 269273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demetriou, A. (ed.) (1988). The Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Dempster, F., & Brainerd, C. (eds.) (1995). Interference and Inhibition in Cognition. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In Carey, S., & Gelman, R. (eds.), The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition (pp. 67110). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1998). Understanding the A-not-B error: Working memory vs. reinforced response, or active trace vs. latent trace. Developmental Science, 1, 185189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s AB task. Experimental Brain Research, 74, 2440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. (1989). Bias in Human Reasoning. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Evans, J. (2003). In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, K., & Kaplan, U. (2003). Jean Piaget. In Nadel, L. (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (Vol. 1, pp. 679682). London: Nature Publishing Group, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1972). Logical capacity of very young children. Child Development, 43, 7590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, R. (1997). Constructing and using conceptual competence. Cognitive Development, 12, 305313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1983). Preschooler’s counting. Cognition, 13, 343359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, R., Meck, E., & Merkin, S. (1986). Young children’s numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 1, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy education. Science, 337, 16231627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P. (1999). The Scientist in the Crib. New York: William Morrow and Cie.Google Scholar
Harnishfeger, K. (1995). The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definition, and research evidence. In Dempster, F., & Brainerd, C. (eds.), Interference and Inhibition in Cognition (pp. 176204). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Houdé, O. (2000). Inhibition and cognitive development: Object, number, categorization, and reasoning. Cognitive Development, 15, 6373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O. (2007). First insights on neuropedagogy of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 8189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O. (2015). Cognitive development during infancy and early childhood across cultures. In Wright, J. D. (ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 4350). Oxford: Elsevier Science.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O. (2019). 3-System Theory of the Cognitive Brain: A Post-Piagetian Approach. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O., & Guichart, E. (2001). Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task. Developmental Science, 4, 7174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O., Pineau, A., Leroux, G., Poirel, N., Perchey, G., Lanoë, C., Lubin, A., Turbelin, M.-R., Rossi, S., Simon, G., Delcroix, N., Lamberton, F., Vigneau, M., Wisniewski, G., Vicet, J.-R., & Mazoyer, B. (2011). Functional MRI study of Piaget’s conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children: A neo-Piagetian approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 332346.Google Scholar
Houdé, O., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003). Neural foundations of logical and mathematical cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houdé, O., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Moutier, S., Pineau, A., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2000). Shifting from the perceptual brain to the logical brain: The neural impact of cognitive inhibition training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 721728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (2010). Interactive specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2000). Choices, Values, and Frames. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipton, J., & Spelke, E. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science, 14, 396401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loosbroek, E., & Smitsman, A. (1990). Visual perception of numerosity in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 26, 916922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J.,& Bever, T. (1967). Cognitive capacity of very young children. Science, 158, 141142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Reilly, R. (1998). Six principles for biologically based computational models of cortical cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 455462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parpart, P., Jones, M., & Love, B. (2018). Heuristics as Bayesian inference under extreme priors. Cognitive Development, 102, 127144.Google ScholarPubMed
Perret, P., Blaye, A., & Paour, J.-L. (2003). Respective contributions of inhibition and knowledge levels in class inclusion development: A negative priming study. Developmental Science, 6, 283286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 103128). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Poirel, N., Borst, G., Simon, G., Rossi, S., Cassotti, M., Pineau, A., & Houdé, O. (2012). Number conservation is related to children’s prefrontal inhibitory control: An fMRI study of a Piagetian task. PLoS ONE, 7, e40802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. (1996). Emerging Minds: The Process of Change in Children’s Thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. (1999). Strategic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 430435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 12331243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Téglás, E., Vul, E., Girotto, V., Gonzales, M., Tenenbaum, J.-B., & Bonatti, L. (2011). Pure reasoning in 12-month-old infants as probabilistic inference. Science, 332, 10541059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: Typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44, 561575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1998). Psychological foundations of number: Numerical competence in human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 296303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×