Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T13:38:52.741Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

25 - Cognitive Aging and Culture: Older Brain Predictions about Different Environments

from Part IV - Cognitive, Social, and Biological Factors across the Lifespan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

Ayanna K. Thomas
Affiliation:
Tufts University, Massachusetts
Angela Gutchess
Affiliation:
Brandeis University, Massachusetts
Get access

Summary

With world population senescence and globalization, more present-day older adults will evince cognitive aging that is influenced over a longer life span by a wide range of social practices and motivational beliefs from cultural groups across the world. Although there is no dispute that brain structure and function aggregate biological and experiential influences, a useful framework is still needed regarding the specific neural mechanisms underlying the exchange between biology and experience with age, and the effect on cognition. We introduce a predictive coding framework of the aging cognitive brain that views the older brain as making predictions about the environment based on a lifetime of experience in it. The influence of cultural experiences in shaping the aging predictive brain then reflects individual differences in processing social signals about appropriate or inappropriate behaviors and cognitive styles amid neural resources changes. We briefly annotate relevant findings on age effects and cultural differences in neurocognitive processing. We further review findings showing that cultural cognitive differences are present in children, persist in young adulthood, and are either maintained or accentuated in older adulthood. Finally, we consider that the predictive aging brain is an enculturated one, reflecting the accumulation of a lifetime of experiences that have fortified culture-specific modes of thought and neural processing in older adults.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging
A Life Course Perspective
, pp. 457 - 479
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akechi, H., Senju, A., Uibo, H., et al. (2013). Attention to eye contact in the West and East: Autonomic responses and evaluative ratings. PLoS One, 8(3), e59312. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, L. A., & McConnell, S. R. (2007). The healthy brain and our aging population: Translating science to public health practice. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 3(Suppl. 2), 12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M. (2011). Predictions in the brain: Using our past to generate a future. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bayes, M., & Price, M. (1763). An essay towards solving a problem in the doctrine of chances. By the Late Rev. Mr. Bayes, F.R.S. communicated by Mr. Price, in a letter to John Canton, A.M.F.R.S. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 53, 370418. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstl.1763.0053Google Scholar
Blais, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers, C., Fiset, D., & Caldara, R. (2008). Culture shapes how we look at faces. PLoS One, 3(8), e3022. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003022Google Scholar
Caldara, R., Zhou, X., & Miellet, S. (2010). Putting culture under the “spotlight” reveals universal information use for face recognition. PLoS One, 5(3), e9708. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009708CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2007). Adding up the effects of cultural experience on the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.008Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312(5782), 19131915. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127488Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165181. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.54.3.165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., et al. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: Evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 2133. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021285Google Scholar
Chan, J. S., Wibral, M., Wollstadt, P., et al. (2017). Predictive coding over the lifespan: Increased reliance on perceptual priors in older adults – a magnetoencephalography and dynamic causal modelling study. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/178095Google Scholar
Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 310324. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310Google Scholar
Chee, M. W. L., Goh, J. O. S., Venkatraman, V., et al. (2006). Age-related changes in object processing and contextual binding revealed using fMR adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(4), 495507. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.495Google Scholar
Chee, M. W. L., Zheng, H., Goh, J. O. S., Park, D. C., & Sutton, B. P. (2011). Brain structure in young and old East Asians and Westerners: Comparisons of structural volume and cortical thickness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 10651079. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21513Google Scholar
Cheon, B. K., Im, D.-M., Harada, T., et al. (2013). Cultural modulation of the neural correlates of emotional pain perception: The role of other-focusedness. Neuropsychologia, 51(7), 11771186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.018Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., et al. (2010). Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20151Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., et al. (2008a). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(12), 21672174. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, J. Y., Li, S.-C., Seligman, R., & Turner, R. (2016). The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Li, Z., & Harada, T. (2008b). Cultural neuroscience of consciousness: From visual perception to self-awareness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15, 5869.Google Scholar
Chiu, L. H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7(4), 235242. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207597208246604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(35), 1262912633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102Google Scholar
Chua, H. F., Chen, W., & Park, D. C. (2006). Source memory, aging and culture. Gerontology, 52(5), 306313. https://doi.org/10.1159/000094612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477Google Scholar
Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Malcolm-Smith, S., et al. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12403. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12403Google Scholar
Dahle, C. L., Jacobs, B. S., & Raz, N. (2009). Aging, vascular risk, and cognition: Blood glucose, pulse pressure, and cognitive performance in healthy adults. Psychology and Aging, 24(1), 154162. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014283CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. (Londres, J. M., Ed.). London: William Clowes and Sons.Google Scholar
Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 12011209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm155Google Scholar
Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In Craik, F. I. M. and Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition, 3rd ed. (pp. 154). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124129. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eppinger, B., Schuck, N. W., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). Reduced striatal responses to reward prediction errors in older compared with younger adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(24), 99059912. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2942-12.2013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N., & Schultz, W. (2003). Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science, 299(5614), 18981902. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1077349Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622Google Scholar
Friston, K., & Kiebel, S. (2009). Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 12111221. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0300CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fung, H. H., Lu, A. Y., Goren, D., et al. (2008). Age-related positivity enhancement is not universal: Older Chinese look away from positive stimuli. Psychology and Aging, 23(2), 440446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, J. R., & Moran, R. J. (2016). Inputs to prefrontal cortex support visual recognition in the aging brain. Scientific Reports, 6, 31943. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep31943Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S. (2011). Functional dedifferentiation and altered connectivity in older adults: Neural accounts of cognitive aging. Aging and Disease, 2(1), 3048.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Chee, M. W. L., Tan, J. C., & Park, D. C. (2007a). Aging and cultural differences in eye-movements during complex picture viewing. Presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society Annual Meeting, New York, May 6.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Chee, M. W. L., Tan, J. C., et al. (2007b). Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(1), 4452. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.1.44CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goh, J. O. S., Hebrank, A. C., Sutton, B. P., et al. (2013). Culture-related differences in default network activity during visuo-spatial judgments. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(2), 134142. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr077Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., & Huang, C.-M. (2012). Images of the cognitive brain across age and culture. In Bright, P. (Ed.), Neuroimaging – Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience (pp. 1746). Rijeka, Croatia. InTech.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Hung, H.-Y., & Su, Y.-S. (2018). A conceptual consideration of the free energy principle in cognitive maps: How cognitive maps help reduce surprise. In Federmeier, K. D. & Watson, D. G. (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 69 (pp. 205240). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Leshikar, E., Hebrank, A., et al. (2008). Age and culture modulate neural selectivity in the ventral visual area during face and place viewing. Presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, Washington, November 16.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Leshikar, E. D., Sutton, B. P., et al. (2010). Culture differences in neural processing of faces and houses in the ventral visual cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 227235. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq060Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Li, C.-Y., Tu, Y.-Z., & Dallaire-Théroux, C. (2020). Visual cognition and culture. In Pedraza, O. (Ed.), Clinical Cultural Neuroscience: Foundations and Neuropsychological Assessment (pp. 124150). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., & Park, D. C. (2009a). Neuroplasticity and cognitive aging: The scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27(5), 391403. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2009-0493Google Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., & Park, D. C. (2009b). Culture sculpts the perceptual brain. Progress in Brain Research, 178, 95111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17807-XGoogle Scholar
Goh, J. O. S., Tan, J. C., & Park, D. C. (2009). Culture modulates eye-movements to visual novelty. PLoS One, 4(12), e8238. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutchess, A. H., & Goh, J. O. S. (2013). Refining concepts and uncovering biological mechanisms for cultural neuroscience. Psychological Inquiry, 24(2), 3136. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.765338Google Scholar
Gutchess, A. H., & Huff, S. (2016). Cross-cultural differences in memory. The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience, Vol. 155. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gutchess, A. H., Yoon, C., Luo, T., et al. (2006). Categorical organization in free recall across culture and age. Gerontology, 52(5), 314323. https://doi.org/10.1159/000094613Google Scholar
Han, S., Northoff, G., Vogeley, K., et al. (2013). A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 335359. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-071112-054629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 1217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.xGoogle Scholar
Hedden, T., & Park, D. C. (2001). Culture, aging, and cognitive aspects of communication. In Charness, N., Parks, D. C., & Sabel, B. A. (Eds.), Communication, Technology, and Aging (pp. 81107). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XGoogle Scholar
Hsin, A., & Xie, Y. (2014). Explaining Asian Americans’ academic advantage over whites. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111(23), 84168421. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1406402111Google Scholar
Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., & Itakura, S. (2013). East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science, 16(2), 198208. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imbo, I., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2009). Cultural differences in complex addition: Efficient Chinese versus adaptive Belgians and Canadians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 14651476. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017022Google Scholar
Imbo, I., & Lefevre, J.-A. (2011). Cultural differences in strategic behavior: A study in computational estimation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 12941301. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024070Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Blais, C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G., & Caldara, R. (2009). Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal. Current Biology, 19(18), 15431548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.051Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 109(19), 72417244. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200155109Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Sun, W., Delis, I., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2016). Four not six: Revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 708730. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, L. J., Yang, Y.-J., Goh, J. O. S., et al. (2010). Cultural differences in the lateral occipital complex while viewing incongruent scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 236241. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp056Google Scholar
Ji, L.-J., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 5765. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.) (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201206. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02432Google Scholar
Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(12), 712719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007Google Scholar
Knox, P. C., & Wolohan, F. D. A. (2014). Cultural diversity and saccade similarities: Culture does not explain saccade latency differences between Chinese and Caucasian participants. PLoS One, 9(4), e94424. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094424Google Scholar
Ko, S.-G., Lee, T.-H., Yoon, H.-Y., Kwon, J.-H., & Mather, M. (2011). How does context affect assessments of facial emotion? The role of culture and age. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 4859. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020222Google Scholar
Kuwabara, M., & Smith, L. B. (2016). Cultural differences in visual object recognition in 3-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 2238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.006Google Scholar
Kwon, Y., Scheibe, S., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Tsai, J. L., & Carstensen, L. L. (2009). Replicating the positivity effect in picture memory in Koreans: Evidence for cross-cultural generalizability. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), 748754. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016054Google Scholar
Leidner, D. E., & Kayworth, T. (2006). A review of culture in information systems research: Toward a theory of information technology culture conflict. MIS Quarterly, 30(2), 357399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, Y., Hendler, T., Ben-Bashat, D., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2001). A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11(4), 287297. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.4.287Google Scholar
Li, S.-C., & Sikström, S. (2002). Integrative neurocomputational perspectives on cognitive aging, neuromodulation, and representation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 795808. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00066-0Google Scholar
Luo, Z., Jose, P. E., Huntsinger, C. S., & Pigott, T. D. (2007). Fine motor skills and mathematics achievement in East Asian American and European American kindergartners and first graders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 595614. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X185329Google Scholar
Ma, Y., Bang, D., Wang, C., et al. (2014). Sociocultural patterning of neural activity during self-reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 7380. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss103Google Scholar
Mandal, M. K., Harizuka, S., Bhushan, B., & Mishra, R. C. (2001). Cultural variation in hemifacial asymmetry of emotion expressions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 385398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466601164885Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., et al. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 365381. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922934. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.922Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science, 30(2), 381399. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., et al. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15(4), 259263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00662.xGoogle Scholar
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005Google Scholar
Mattson, M. P. (2010). The impact of dietary energy intake on cognitive aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.24.005.2010Google Scholar
Miellet, S., Zhou, X., He, L., Rodger, H., & Caldara, R. (2010). Investigating cultural diversity for extrafoveal information use in visual scenes. Journal of Vision, 10(6), 21. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.6.21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millar, P. R., Serbun, S. J., Vadalia, A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in memory specificity. Culture and Brain, 1(2–4), 138157. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-013-0011-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6(1), 5660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00305.xGoogle Scholar
Moran, R. J., Symmonds, M., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (2014). The brain ages optimally to model its environment: Evidence from sensory learning over the adult lifespan. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(1), e1003422. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003422Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Kitayama, S., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2015). How culture gets embrained: Cultural differences in event-related potentials of social norm violations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 112(50), 1534815353. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509839112Google Scholar
Ng, S. S. N., & Rao, N. (2010). Chinese number words, culture, and mathematics learning. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 180206. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310364764Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently – and why. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100(19), 1116311170. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1934527100Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 467473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.004Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.291Google Scholar
Oeppen, J., & Vaupel, J. W. (2002). Broken limits to life expectancy. Science, 296(5570), 10291031. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069675Google Scholar
Paige, L. E., Ksander, J. C., Johndro, H. A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2017). Cross-cultural differences in the neural correlates of specific and general recognition. Cortex, 91, 250261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, D. C., & Goh, J. O. S. (2009). Successful aging. In Bernston, G. G. & Cacioppo, J. T. (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience for the behavioral sciences (pp. 12031219). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Park, D. C., & Gutchess, A. H. (2002). Aging, cognition, and culture: A neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 859867. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00072-6Google Scholar
Park, D. C., & Gutchess, A. H. (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of aging and culture. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 105108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00416.xGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C., & Huang, C.-M. (2010). Culture wires the brain: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 391400. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610374591Google Scholar
Rao, R. P., & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(1), 7987. https://doi.org/10.1038/4580Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Castelhano, M. S., & Yang, J. (2009). Eye movements when looking at unusual/weird scenes: Are there cultural differences? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 254259. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013508Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Li, X., Williams, C. C., Cave, K. R., & Well, A. D. (2007). Eye movements during information processing tasks: Individual differences and cultural effects. Vision Research, 47(21), 27142726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.007Google Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychology Review, 24(3), 355370. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9Google Scholar
Rodic, M., Zhou, X., Tikhomirova, T., et al. (2015). Cross-cultural investigation into cognitive underpinnings of individual differences in early arithmetic. Developmental Science, 18(1), 165174. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12204Google Scholar
Rohan, M. J. (2000). A rose by any name? The values construct. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(3), 255277. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0403_4Google Scholar
Ross, M., & Wang, Q. (2010). Why we remember and what we remember: Culture and autobiographical memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 401409. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375555Google Scholar
Rossion, B., Dricot, L., Devolder, A., et al. (2000). Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 793802. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892900562606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotshtein, P., Geng, J. J., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). Role of features and second-order spatial relations in face discrimination, face recognition, and individual face skills: Behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 14351452. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1435Google Scholar
Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Gibbs, S. E. B., Khanna, K., et al. (2007). Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nature Neuroscience, 10(6), 787791. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1894Google Scholar
Schlagman, S., Schulz, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2006). A content analysis of involuntary autobiographical memories: Examining the positivity effect in old age. Memory, 14(2), 161175. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210544000024Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2007). Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(5), 203210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007Google Scholar
Schwartz, A. J., Boduroglu, A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2014). Cross-cultural differences in categorical memory errors. Cognitive Science, 38(5), 9971007. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12109Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 162. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6Google Scholar
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 580591. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014Google Scholar
Su, Y.-S., Chen, J.-T., Tang, Y.-J., et al. (2018). Age-related differences in striatal, medial temporal, and frontal involvement during value-based decision processing. Neurobiology of Aging, 69, 185198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.05.019Google Scholar
Tang, Y., Zhang, W., Chen, K., et al. (2006). Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103(28), 1077510780. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0604416103Google Scholar
Tobler, P. N., Fiorillo, C. D., & Schultz, W. (2005). Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science, 307(5715), 16421645. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1105370Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Tu, Y.-Z., Lin, D.-W., Suzuki, A., & Goh, J. O. S. (2018). East Asian young and older adult perceptions of emotional faces from an age- and sex-fair East Asian facial expression database. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2358. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02358Google Scholar
Vaupel, J. W. (2010). Biodemography of human ageing. Nature, 464(7288), 536542. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08984Google Scholar
Wagar, B. M., & Cohen, D. (2003). Culture, memory, and the self: An analysis of the personal and collective self in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(5), 468475. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00021-0Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2001). Culture effects on adults’ earliest childhood recollection and self-description: Implications for the relation between memory and the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2), 220233. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.2.220Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.3Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2006). Earliest recollections of self and others in European American and Taiwanese young adults. Psychological Science, 17(8), 708714. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01770.xGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2009). Are Asians forgetful? Perception, retention, and recall in episodic remembering. Cognition, 111(1), 123131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.004Google Scholar
Wang, Q., & Conway, M. A. (2004). The stories we keep: Autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-aged adults. Journal of Personality, 72(5), 911938. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00285.xGoogle Scholar
Yang, L., Chen, W., Ng, A. H., & Fu, X. (2013). Aging, culture, and memory for categorically processed information. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68(6), 872881. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt006Google Scholar
Yoon, C., Feinberg, F., & Gutchess, A. H. (2006). Pictorial naming specificity across ages and cultures: A latent class analysis of picture norms for younger and older Americans and Chinese. Gerontology, 52(5), 295305. https://doi.org/10.1159/000094611Google Scholar
You, J., Fung, H. H. L., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2009). Age differences in dispositional optimism: A cross-cultural study. European Journal of Ageing, 6(4), 247252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-009-0130-zGoogle Scholar
Yovel, G., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Face perception: Domain specific, not process specific. Neuron, 44(5), 889898. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-009-0130-zGoogle Scholar
Yovel, G., Tambini, A., & Brandman, T. (2008). The asymmetry of the fusiform face area is a stable individual characteristic that underlies the left-visual-field superiority for faces. Neuropsychologia, 46(13), 30613068. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.017Google Scholar
Zhu, C. W., & Sano, M. (2006). Economic considerations in the management of Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 1(2), 143154. https://doi.org/10.2147/ciia.2006.1.2.143Google Scholar
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), 13101316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×