Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T15:28:25.328Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - The Speech Production of Bilingual Children

from Part III - The Phonetics and Phonology of the Bilingual Child

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2024

Mark Amengual
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Get access

Summary

This chapter reviews research on the phonetic and phonological production patterns of children exposed to two or more languages in early childhood. Much of the research has examined whether bilingual children differ from monolingual children in their production patterns and, when differences have been found, whether such patterns can be explained by language-internal (e.g., the influence of the phonological properties of one language on the other), language-external (e.g., language input or dominance), or other factors (e.g., the developing lexicon). We focus on language-internal influences, also referred to as cross-linguistic interaction, and review models that have been used to account for interaction. Our findings reveal that, while systematic differences between monolingual and bilingual speech exist, the differences are not large and may be explained by multiple factors. The chapter concludes with a discussion of research limitations which include small participant numbers and a predominance of certain language pairs, factors that have hindered attempts to model cross-linguistic interaction.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Almeida, L., Rose, Y., & Freitas, J. (2012). Prosodic influence in bilingual phonological development: Evidence from a Portuguese-French first language learner. In Biller, A., Chung, E., & Kimball, A., eds., Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 4252.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2021). Power considerations in bilingualism research: Time to step up our game. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(5), 813818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunta, F. & Ingram, D. (2007). The acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual Spanish- and English-speaking 4- and 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 9991014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooperson, S., Bedore, L., & Peña, E. (2013). The relationship of phonological skills to language skills in Spanish-English-speaking bilingual children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 371389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodane, C. & Bijeljac-Babic, R. (2017). Cross-language influences in the productions of bilingual children: Separation or interaction. In Yavas, M., Kehoe, M., & Cardoso, W., eds., Romance-Germanic Bilingual Phonology. Sheffield: Equinox, pp. 3855.Google Scholar
Fabiano, L. & Goldstein, B. (2005). Phonological cross-linguistic effects in bilingual Spanish-English children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Barlow, J. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Bunta, F. (2012). Voice onset time of voiceless bilabial and velar stops in 3-year-old bilingual children and their age-matched monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26, 148163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Goldstein, B. (2010a). Early, middle, and late-developing sounds in monolingual and bilingual children: An exploratory study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Goldstein, B. (2010b). Phonological acquisition in bilingual Spanish-English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L., Oglivie, T., Maiefski, O., & Schertz, J. (2015). Acquisition of the stop-spirant alternation in bilingual Mexican Spanish-English speaking children: Theoretical and clinical implications. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 29, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78, 15101525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flege, J. E. (1987). A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages? Applied Linguistics, 8, 162177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W., ed., Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 233273.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. & Bohn, O.-S. (2021). The revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In Wayland, R., ed., Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 6, 161179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., Kester, E., Davis, B., & Peña, E. (2008). English speech sound development in preschool-aged children from bilingual English-Spanish environments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 314328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., Peña, E., Davis, B., & Kester, E. (2009). Effects of L1 during early acquisition of L2: Speech changes in Spanish at first contact. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 12, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. & Wright, K. (2010). English speech acquisition in 3- to 5-year-old children learning Russian and English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 429444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B., Bunta, F., Lange, J., Rodriguez, J., & Burrows, L. (2010). The effects of measures of language experience and language ability on segmental accuracy in bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 238247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B., Fabiano, L., & Washington, P. (2005). Phonological skills in predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish-English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 201218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. & Washington, P. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabe, E. & Low, E. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In Gussenhoven, C. & Warner, N., eds., Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 515546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., Post, B., & Watson, I. (1999). The acquisition of rhythm in English and French. In Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS99), 2, London: International Phonetic Association, pp. 12011204. www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS1999/papers/p14_1201.pdf.Google Scholar
Grech, H. & Dodd, B. (2008). Phonological acquisition in Malta: A bilingual language learning context. International Journal of Bilingualism, 12, 155–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambly, H., Wren, Y., McLeod, S., & Roulstone, S. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on speech production: A systematic review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 48, 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, A., Dodd, B., Stow, C., & Pert, S. (1999). Identification and differential diagnosis of phonological disorder in bilingual children. Language Testing, 16, 271292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, C. & Wilson, I. (2002). Phonetic evidence for early language differentiation: Research issues and some preliminary data. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 271289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kartushina, N. & Frauenfelder, U. (2014). On the effects of L2 perception and of individual differences in L1 production on L2 pronunciation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keffala, B., Barlow, J., & Rose, S. (2018). Interaction in Spanish-English bilinguals’ acquisition of syllable structure. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 1637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keffala, B., Scarpino, S., Hammer, C., et al. (2020). Vocabulary and phonological abilities affect dual-language learners’ consonant production accuracy within and across languages: A large-scale study of 3- to 6-year-old Spanish-English dual language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29, 11961211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. (2015a). Cross-linguistic interaction: A retrospective and prospective view. In Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the International Symposium on Monolingual and Bilingual Speech 2015. Chania, Greece: Institute of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, pp. 141167. http://ismbs.eu/publications.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2015b). Lexical-phonological interactions in bilingual children. First Language, 35, 93125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2018). The development of rhotics: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 710731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2022). The prosody of two-syllable words in French-speaking monolingual and bilingual children: A focus on initial accent and final accent. Language and Speech, 65, 444471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2021). Relations between phonological production, grammar and the lexicon in bilingual French-English children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25, 15761596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Girardier, C. (2020). What factors influence phonological production in French-speaking bilingual children, aged three to six years? Journal of Child Language, 47, 945981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Havy, M. (2019). Bilingual phonological acquisition: The influence of language-internal, language-external and lexical factors. Journal of Child Language, 46, 292333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2004). Voice onset time in bilingual German-Spanish children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 7188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2011). Speech rhythm in the pronunciation of German and Spanish monolingual and German-Spanish bilingual 3-year-olds. Linguistische Berichte, 227, 323351.Google Scholar
LaBelle, C. (2000). A longitudinal study of lexical and prosodic differentiation by a simultaneous French/English bilingual child (1;5–2;3). In Howell, S. C., Fish, S., & Keith-Lucas, T., eds., Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 474485.Google Scholar
Lanza, E. (1992). Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language, 19, 633658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2012a). Stop consonant productions of Korean-English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 275287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2012b). Vowel category formation in Korean-English bilingual children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 55, 14491462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2017). The emergence of phonetic categories in Korean-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 44, 14851515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Kuchenbrandt, I., Kehoe, M., & Trujillo, C. (2003). Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In Müller, N., ed., (Non)Vulnerable Domains in Bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 191220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. & Rakow, M. (2005). Markedness effects in voiced stop spirantization in bilingual German-Spanish children. In Cohen, J., McAlister, K., Rolstad, K., & McSwan, J., eds., Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (ISB4), Vol. 81. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 13531371.Google Scholar
Mack, M. (1990). Phonetic transfer in a French-English bilingual child. In Nelde, P., ed., Language Attitudes and Language Conflict. Bonn: Dümmler, pp. 107124.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980a). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in world-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980b). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: A phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7, 433458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, A., Laukys, K., & Rvachew, S. (2011). The impact of bilingual language learning on whole-word complexity and segmental accuracy among children aged 18 and 36 months. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 490499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. & De Groot, A., eds., Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4967.Google Scholar
Marecka, M., Wrembel, M., Otwinowska, A., et al. (2020). Bilingual children’s phonology shows evidence of transfer, but not deceleration in their L1. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 89114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, R., Howells, G., & Lewis, R. (2015). Asymmetries in phonological development: The case of word-final cluster acquisition in Welsh-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 42, 146179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, R. & Montanari, S. (2015). Cross-linguistic interaction in trilingual phonological development: The role of the input in the acquisition of the voicing contrast. Journal of Child Language, 42, 10061035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, R. & Siddika, A. (2018). Inter-generational transmission in a minority language setting: Stop consonant production by Bangladeshi heritage children and adults. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 255284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, K., Mahon, M., Rosen, S., & Evans, B. (2014). Speech perception and production by sequential bilingual children: A longitudinal study of voice onset time acquisition. Child Development, 85, 19651980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinhold, G. & Stock, E. (1980). Phonologie der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Leipzig : Bibliographisches Institut.Google Scholar
Meziane, R. & MacLeod, A. (2017). L’acquisition de la phonologie en français langue seconde : le profile phonologique d’enfants allophones en maternelle. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 20, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. (2011). The acquisition of rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 458472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. (2013). Speech rhythm of monolingual and bilingual children at age 2;6: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 693703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montanari, S., Mayr, R., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2018). Bilingual speech sound development during the preschool years: The role of language proficiency and cross-linguistic relatedness. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 61, 24672486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, A., Goldstein, B., Gilhool, A., & Paradis, J. (2014). Phonological skills in English language learners. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 45, 2639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, A., Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. (2012). Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55, 203229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73, 265292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlinger, W. & Park, T. (1980). Language mixing in young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 7, 337–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, Y. & Champdoizeau, C. (2007). There is no innate trochaic bias: Acoustic evidence in favour of the neutral start hypothesis. In Gavarro, A. & Freitas, M. J., eds., Proceedings of the Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition Conference 2007. Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars, pp. 357367.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Felter, R., Cooperson, S., Bedore, L., & Peña, E. (2016). Influence of current input-output and age of first exposure on phonological acquisition in early bilingual Spanish-English-speaking kindergarteners. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51, 36383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarpino, S., Hammer, C., Goldstein, B., Rodriguez, B., & Lopez, L. (2019). Effects of home language, oral language skills, and cross-linguistic phonological abilities on whole-word proximity in Spanish-English speaking children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28, 174187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, E. & Post, B. (2015). Language interaction in the development of speech rhythm in simultaneous bilinguals. In Delais-Roussarie, E., Avanzi, M., & Herment, S., eds., Prosody and Language in Contact. New York: Springer, pp. 271291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. & Krasinski, E. (1994). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child. Journal of Child Language, 21, 585622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. & Krasinski, E. (1996). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child: A contrasting second case. Journal of Child Language, 23, 547571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sim, J. & Post, B. (2022). Variation in quality of maternal input and development of coda stops in English-speaking children in Singapore. Journal of Child Language, 49, 11471172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorenson Duncan, T. & Paradis, J. (2016). English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of L2 vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., van Hell, J., & Fikkert, P. (2018). Heritage language exposure impacts voice onset time of Dutch–German simultaneous bilingual preschoolers. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 21, 598617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., van Hell, J., & Fikkert, P. (2019). Bilingual preschoolers’ speech is associated with non-native maternal language input. Language Learning and Development, 15, 75100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamburelli, M., Sanoudaki, E., Jones, G., & Sowinska, M. (2015). Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish-English children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 713725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2002). Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2016). Prosodic structures and templates in bilingual phonological development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 6988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1, 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J. (2021). Vowel development in young Mandarin-English bilingual children. Phonetica, 78, 241272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, J. & Fox, R. (2017). L1–L2 interactions of vowel systems in young bilingual Mandarin-English children. Journal of Phonetics, 65, 6076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J., Fox, R., & Jacewicz, E. (2015). Vowel development in an emergent Mandarin-English bilingual child: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 42, 11251143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×