Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T12:15:03.872Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Traditions in mammalian and avian vocal communication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2009

Vincent M. Janik
Affiliation:
Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Fife KY16 9TS, UK
Peter J. B. Slater
Affiliation:
School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Fife, KY 16 9TS, UK
Dorothy M. Fragaszy
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Susan Perry
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The most basic definition of traditions used by biologists is the one given by Fragaszy and Perry in Ch. 1. It states that traditions are enduring behavior patterns that are shared by at least two individuals and that are acquired in part through social learning. Laland, Richerson, and Boyd (1993) distinguished between two forms of social learning. The first involves primarily horizontal information transmission (i.e., between animals of the same generation) in which information is of only transient value, as in the acquisition of foraging information in a highly variable environment. In the second, information is transmitted vertically (between generations) and results in what Laland et al. (1993) call stable traditions. In this definition, socially learned information has to remain in the population for a certain period of time before it can be called a tradition. These two forms appear not to be exclusive but rather are placed at different points on a continuum. However, it is useful to consider the results of social learning in this theoretical framework to demonstrate how social learning in communication systems differs from that in other domains. We will use these concepts to review vocal traditions in mammals and birds.

By definition, every form of learning about communication has to involve another individual since communication involves at least two individuals. The only exception is learning to change the quality of a signal through practicing. However, this can be recognized by observing the performance of an isolated individual as it changes.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Biology of Traditions
Models and Evidence
, pp. 213 - 235
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adret, P. 1993. Vocal learning induced with operant techniques: an overview. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 125–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrew, R. J. 1962. Evolution of intelligence and vocal mimicking. Science, 137, 585–589CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appleby, B. M. and Redpath, S. M. 1997. Variation in the male territorial hoot of the tawny owl Strix aluco in three English populations. Ibis, 139, 152–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avery, M. and Oring, L. W. 1977. Song dialects in the bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus). Condor, 79, 113–118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Backus, R. H. and Schevill, W. E. 1966. Physeter clicks. In Whales, Dolphins, and Porpoises, ed. K. S. Norris, pp. 510–528. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press
Baird, R. W., Abrams, P. A., and Dill, L. M. 1992. Possible indirect interactions between transient and resident killer whales: implications for the evolution of foraging specializations in the genus Orcinus. Oecologia, 89, 125–132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balaban, E. 1988. Cultural and genetic variation in swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) I. Song variation, genetic variation, and their relationship. Behaviour, 105, 250–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, J. A. and Bailey, E. D. 1987. Sources of phenotypic variation in the separation call of northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 65, 1010–1015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, J. A. and Cunningham, M. A. 1985. The biology of bird-song dialects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 85–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. C. and Boylan, J. T. 1995. A catalog of song syllables of indigo and lazuli buntings. Condor, 97, 1028–1040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, G. W. 1977. Modal action patterns. In How Animals Communicate, ed. T. A. Sebeok, pp. 98–134. Bloomington, IA: Indiana University Press
Bartlett, P. and Slater, P. J. B. 1999. The effect of new recruits on the flock specific call of budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Ethology, Ecology and Evolution, 11, 139–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett-Lennard, L. G. 2000. Population structure and mating patterns of killer whales (Orcinus orca) as revealed by DNA analysis. PhD Thesis, University of British Columbia
Beecher, M. D. 1996. Birdsong learning in the laboratory and field. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ed. D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, pp. 61–78. Ithaca: Comstock Publishing
Bitterbaum, E. and Baptista, L. F. 1979. Geographical variation in songs of California house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus). Auk, 96, 462–474Google Scholar
Boughman, J. W. 1997. Greater spear-nosed bats give group-distinctive calls. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 40, 61–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boughman, J. W. 1998. Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 265, 227–233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, R. A. 1994. Cultural change and geographic variation in the songs of the Belding's savannah sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis belgingi). Bulletin of the South Californian Academy of Science, 93, 91–109Google Scholar
Byrne, R. 1995. The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K., and Tyack, P. L. 1990. Review of the signature-whistlehypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. In The Bottlenose Dolphin, ed. S. Leatherwood and R. R. Reeves, pp. 199–234. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. 1995. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Chilton, G. and Lein, M. R. 1996. Long-term changes in songs and song dialect boundaries of Puget sound white-crowned sparrows. Condor, 98, 567–580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark Arcadi, A. 1996. Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: patterns of production and interpopulation variability. American Journal of Primatology, 39, 159–1783.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleator, H. J., Stirling, I., and Smith, T. G. 1989. Underwater vocalizations of the bearded seal (Erignathus barbatus). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 67, 1900–1910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deecke, V. B., Ford, J. K. B., and Spong, P. 2000. Dialect change in resident killer whales: implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission. Animal Behaviour, 60, 629–638CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, K. L. 1969. Patterns of singing in a population of the plain titmouse. Condor, 71, 94–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farabaugh, S. M., Linzenbold, A., and Dooling, R. J. 1994. Vocal plasticity in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) – evidence for social factors in the learning of contact calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 81–92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feekes, F. 1982. Song mimesis within colonies of Cacicus c. cela (Icteridae, Aves). A colonial password?Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 58, 119–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, J. K. B. and Fisher, H. D. 1983. Group-specific dialects of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in British Columbia. In Communication and Behavior of Whales, ed. R. Payne, pp. 129–161. Boulder, CO: Westview Press
Freeberg, T. M., King, A. P., and West, M. J. 2001. Cultural transmission of vocal traditions in cowbirds (Molothrus ater) influences courtship patterns and mate preference. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 201–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, H. L. 1990. Cultural evolution of male song types in Darwin's medium ground finches, Geospiza fortis. Animal Behaviour, 39, 253–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, B. R., and Grant, P. R. 1996. Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution, 50, 2471–2487CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, S. 1975. Dialects in japanese monkeys: vocal learning and cultural transmission of locale-specific vocal behavior?Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 38, 304–314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Handford, P. 1981. Vegetational correlates of variation in the song of Zonotrichia capensis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8, 203–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harbison, H., Nelson, D. A., and Hahn, T. P. 1999. Long-term persistence of song dialects in the mountain white-crowned sparrow. Condor, 101, 133–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hile, A. G. and Striedter, G. F. 2000. Call convergence within groups of female budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Ethology, 106, 1105–1114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hile, A. G., Plummer, T. K., and Striedter, G. F. 2000. Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus. Animal Behaviour, 59, 1209–1218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hough, G. E., Nelson, D. A., and Volman, S. F. 2000. Re-expression of songs deleted during vocal development in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys. Animal Behaviour, 60, 279–287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, H. and Todt, D. 1989a. Memorization and reproduction of songs in nightingales: evidence for package formation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 165, 197–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, H. and Todt, D. 1989b. Song acquisition and acquisition constraints in nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos. Naturwissenschaften, 76, 83–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ince, S. A., Slater, P. J. B., and Weismann, C. 1980. Changes with time in the songs of a population of chaffinches. Condor, 82, 285–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, P. C. 1985. Geographical and temporal variation in the calls of the manx shearwater Puffinus puffinus and the British storm petrel Hydrobates pelagicus. Journal of Zoology, 207, 331–344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M. 2000. Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Science, 289, 1355–1357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M. 2001. Is cetacean social learning unique?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 337–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. 1997. Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 26, 59–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. 1998. Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Animal Behaviour, 56, 829–838CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. 2000. The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Animal Behaviour, 60, 1–11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G. 1997. Acoustic signals and speciation: the roles of natural and sexual selection in the evolution of cryptic species. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 26, 317–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, G. and Ransome, R. D. 1993. Echolocation calls of bats are influenced by maternal effects and change over a lifetime. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 252, 125–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, A. P. and West, M. J. 1983. Epigenesis of cowbird song – a joint endeavour of males and females. Nature, 305, 704–706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E. and Canady, R. A. 1985. Differences in repertoire size, singing behavior, and associated neuroanatomy among marsh wren populations have a genetic basis. Auk, 102, 439–446Google Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E.Houlihan, P. W., Fallon, P. A., and Wells, J. A. 1997. Song development in grey catbirds. Animal Behaviour, 54, 457–464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachlan, R. F. and Slater, P. J. B. 1999. The maintenance of vocal learning by gene–culture interaction: the cultural trap hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 701–706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. 1993. Animal social learning: toward a new theoretical approach. Perspectives in Ethology, 10, 249–277Google Scholar
Latruffe, C., McGregor, P. K., Tavares, J. P., and Mota, P. G. 2000. Microgeographic variation in corn bunting (Miliaria calandra) song: quantitative and discrimination aspects. Behaviour, 137, 1241–1255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieblich, A. K., Symmes, D., Newman, J. D., and Shapiro, M. 1980. Development of the isolation peep in laboratory-bred squirrel monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 28, 1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lougheed, S. C. and Handford, P. 1992. Vocal dialects and the structure of geographic variation in morphological and allozymic characters in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis. Evolution, 45, 1443–1456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lougheed, S. C., Handford, P., and Baker, A. J. 1993. Mitochondrial DNA hyperdiversity, vocal dialects and subspecies in the rufous-collared sparrow (Zonotrichia capensis). Condor, 95, 889–895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, A. 1996. The population memetics of birdsong. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ed. D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, pp. 181–197. Ithaca: Comstock Publishing
Lynch, A., Plunkett, G. M., Baker, A. J., and Jenkins, P. F. 1989. A model of cultural evolution of chaffinch song derived with the meme concept. American Naturalist, 133, 634–653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. 1997. Three models of song learning: evidence from behavior. Journal of Neurobiology, 33, 501–5163.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marler, P. and Nelson, D. 1992. Neuroselection and song learning in birds: species universals in culturally transmitted behavior. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 4, 415–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. and Nelson, D. A. 1993. Action-based learning: a new form of developmental plasticity in bird song. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 91–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. and Peters, S. 1982. Developmental overproduction and selective attrition: new processes in the epigenesis of bird song. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 369–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. and Peters, S. 1987. A sensitive period for song acquisition in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia: a case of age-limited learning. Ethology, 76, 89–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. and Pickert, R. 1984. Species-universal microstructure in a learned birdsong: the swamp sparrow (Melospiza georgiana). Animal Behaviour, 32, 673–689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, A. J., Wrangham, R. W., and Arcadi, A. C. 1999. Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees?Animal Behaviour, 58, 825–830CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masataka, N. and Fujita, K. 1989. Vocal learning of Japanese and rhesus monkeys. Behaviour, 109, 191–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, P. K. 1980. Song dialects in the corn bunting (Emberiza calandra). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 54, 285–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, P. K. and Thompson, D. B. A. 1988. Constancy and change in local dialects of the corn bunting. Ornis Scandinavia, 19, 153–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medvin, M. B., Stoddard, P. K., and Beecher, M. D. 1992. Signals for parent–offspring recognition: strong sib–sib call similarity in cliff swallows but not barn swallows. Ethology, 90, 17–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. J. O. and Bain, D. E. 2000. Within-pod variation in the sound production of a pod of killer whales, Orcinus orca. Animal Behaviour, 60, 617–628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. and Brandt, K. 1994. Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology, 96, 233–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E. 1999. Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. American Journal of Primatology, 47, 133–1513.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P., and Byrne, R. 1992. Dialects in wild chimpanzees?American Journal of Primatology, 27, 233–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrice, M. G., Burton, H. R., and Green, K. 1994. Microgeographic variation and songs in the underwater vocalization repertoire of the Weddell seal (Leptonychotes weddelli) from the Vestfold Hills, Antarctica. Polar Biology, 14, 441–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D. A. 1992. Song overproduction and selective attrition lead to song sharing in the field sparrow (Spizella pusilla). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 30, 415–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D. A. 2000. Song overproduction, selective attrition and song dialects in the white-crowned sparrow. Animal Behaviour, 60, 887–898CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolai, J. 1959. Familientradition in der Gesangsentwicklung des Gimpels (Pyrrhula pyrrhula L.). Journal für Ornithologie, 100, 39–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noad, M. J., Cato, D. H., Bryden, M. M., Jenner, M. N., and Jenner, K. C. S. 2001. Cultural revolution in whale song. Nature, 408, 537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nottebohm, F. 1969. The song of the chingolo (Zonotrichia capensis) in Argentina: description and evaluation of a system of dialects. Condor, 71, 299–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. 1992. “Food” calls produced by adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, their normally-raised offspring, and offspring cross-fostered between species. Behaviour, 120, 218–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, K. and Payne, R. 1985. Large scale changes over 19 years in songs of humpback whales in Bermuda. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 68, 89–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, K., Tyack, P., and Payne, R. 1983. Progressive changes in the songs of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): a detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii. In Communication and Behavior of Whales, ed. R. Payne, pp. 9–57. Boulder, CO: Westview Press
Payne, R. B. 1985. Behavioral continuity and change in local song populations of village indigobirds Vidua chalybeata. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 70, 1–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, R. B. 1996. Song traditions in indigo buntings: origin, improvisation, dispersal, and extinction in cultural evolution. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ed. D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, pp. 198–220. Ithaca: Comstock Publishing
Payne, R. B., Thompson, W. L., Fiala, K. L., and Sweany, L. L. 1981. Local song traditions in indigo buntings: cultural transmission of behavior patterns across generations. Behaviour, 77, 199–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., and Doehlert, S. M. 1988. Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings. Ecology, 69, 104–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. 1981. Functional vocalizations by an African grey parrot. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 55, 139–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. 1990. Some cognitive capacities of an African grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Advances in the Study of Behavior, 19, 357–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrinovich, L. 1985. Factors influencing song development in the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 15–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ralls, K., Fiorelli, P., and Gish, S. 1985. Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 63, 1050–1056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riebel, K. 2000. Early exposure leads to repeatable preferences for male song in female zebra finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 267, 2553-2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, M. and Wilczynski, F. 1991. Evolution of intraspecific variation in advertisement calls. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 44, 249–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searcy, W. A., Nowicki, S., and Peters, S. 1999. Song types as fundamental units in vocal repertoires. Animal Behaviour, 58, 37–44CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. and Cheney, D. 1986. Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1640–1665CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slater, P. J. B. and Ince, S. A. 1979. Cultural evolution in chaffinch song. Behaviour, 71, 146–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slater, P. J. B. and Ince, S. A. 1982. Song development in chaffinches: what is learnt and when?Ibis, 124, 21–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slater, P. J. B.Ince, S. A., and Colgan, P. W. 1980. Chaffinch song types: their frequencies in the population and distribution between the repertoires of different individuals. Behaviour, 75, 207–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slater, P. J. B., Lachlan, R. F., and Riebel, K. 2000. The significance of learning in signal development: The curious case of the chaffinch. In Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication, ed. Y. Espmark, T. Amundsen, and G. Rosenqvist, pp. 341–352. Trondheim, Norway: Tapir
Smith, V. A., King, A. P., and West, M. J. 2000. A role of her own: female cowbirds, Molothrus ater, influence the development and outcome of song learning. Animal Behaviour, 60, 599–609CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorjonen, J. 1987. Temporal and spatial differences in traditions and repertoires in the song of the thrush nightingale (Luscinia luscinia). Behaviour, 102, 196–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorjonen, J. 2001. Long-term constancy of two rain-call dialects of the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs in Finnish and Russian Karelia: a consequence of site fidelity?Ornis Fennica, 78, 73–82Google Scholar
Spector, D. A., McKim, L. K., and Kroodsma, D. E. 1989. Yellow warblers are able to learn songs and situations in which to use them. Animal Behaviour, 38, 723–725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. 1967. Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In Social Communication among Primates, ed. S. A. Altmann, pp. 281–324. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press
Struhsaker, T. T. 1970. Phylogenetic implications of some vocalizations of Cercopithecus monkeys. In Old World Monkeys: Evolution, Systematics, and Behavior, ed. J. R. Napier and P. H. Napier, pp. 365–444. New York: Academic Press
Terhune, J. M. 1994. Geographical variation of harp seal underwater vocalizations. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 72, 892–897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terkel, J. 1996. Cultural transmission of feeding behavior in the black rat (Rattus rattus). In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 17–47. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Thielcke, G. 1987. Langjährige Dialektkonstanz beim Gartenbaumläufer (Certhia brachydactyla). Journal für Ornithologie, 128, 171–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, J. A. and Golladay, C. L. 1995. Geographic variation in leopard seal (Hydrurga leptonyx) underwater vocalizations. In Sensory Systems of Aquatic Mammals, ed. R. A. Kastelein, J. A. Thomas and P. E. Nachtigall, pp. 201–221. Woerden: De Spil
Thomas, J. A. and Stirling, I. 1983. Geographic variation in the underwater vocalizations of Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddelli) from Palmer Peninsula and McMurdo Sound, Antarctica. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 61, 2203–2212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. 1958. The learning of song patterns by birds, with especial reference to the song of the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Ibis, 100, 535–570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trainer, J. M. 1989. Cultural evolution in song dialects of yellow-rumped caciques in Panama. Ethology, 80, 190–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyack, P. L. 1997. Development and social functions of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus. Bioacoustics, 8, 21–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parijs, S. M., Hastie, G. D., and Thompson, P. M. 2000. Individual and geographic variation in display behaviour of male harbour seals in Scotland. Animal Behaviour, 59, 559–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, D., Würsig, B., and Evans, W. E. 1995. Whistles of bottlenose dolphins: comparisons among populations. Aquatic Mammals, 21, 65–77Google Scholar
Weilgart, L. and Whitehead, H. 1997. Group-specific dialects and geographical variation in coda repertoire in South Pacific sperm whales. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 40, 277–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, M. J. and King, A. P. 1988. Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird. Nature, 334, 244–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, H., Dillon, M., Dufault, S., Weilgart, L., and Wright, J. 1998. Non-geographically based population structure of South Pacific sperm whales: dialects, fluke-markings and genetics. Journal of Animal Ecology, 67, 253–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiens, J. A. 1982. Song pattern variation in the sage sparrow (Amphispiza belli): dialects or epiphenomena. Auk, 99, 208–229Google Scholar
Winn, H. E., Thompson, T. J., Cummings, W. C., Hain, J., Hudnall, J., Hays, H., and Steiner, W. W. 1981. Song of the humpback whale – population comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8, 41–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×