Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T13:20:54.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Temporal Organization and Procedure in Ascribing Action

from Part I - Constituents of Action Ascription

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2022

Arnulf Deppermann
Affiliation:
Universität Mannheim, Germany
Michael Haugh
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Get access

Summary

Human interaction is organized in time, but that organization is neither automatic nor externally imposed. It is an effortful creation within everyday interaction as participants produce next utterances and place them adjacent to their own or other’s prior utterances. Participants produce utterances in sequence using their knowledge of the normative sequential organizations of a wide range of elements of interaction, key among those elements being turns, actions, and repair initiations. These sequential organizations are distinct, although inseparable from, the triadic temporal organization of adjacency, nextness, and progressivity of elements in sequence: an organization that has been widely acknowledged, but less closely examined than sequential organizations. Examining the temporal organization of interaction reveals the procedure by which participants assess the nextness of another participant’s next adjacent utterance as they interpret prior utterances, which has direct implications for understanding how participants ascribe actions to those prior utterances. The triadic temporal organization of everyday interacting points to third position utterances as essential for the recipient in ascribing action to any given first position utterance, and as a consequence, essential for the speaker and for the recipient in together establishing the state of the talk and conduct between them.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arundale, R. B. (2010). Constituting face in conversation: Face, facework, and interactional achievement. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 2078–105.Google Scholar
Arundale, R. B. (2020). Communicating and Relating: Constituting Face in Everyday Interaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arundale, R. B. & Good, D. A. (2002). Boundaries and sequences in studying conversation. In Fetzer, A. & Meierkord, C., eds., Rethinking Sequentiality: Linguistics Meets Conversational Interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 120–50.Google Scholar
Clayman, S. & Heritage, J. (2014). Benefactors and beneficiaries: Benefactive status and stance in the management of offers and requests. In Drew, P. & Couper-Kuhlen, E., eds., Requesting in Social Interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 5586.Google Scholar
Drew, P. (2011). Reflections on the micro-politics of social action in interaction. Paper presented the 12th International Pragmatics Association Conference, University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Drew, P. (2013). Turn design. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 131–49.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. (2013). Conversation analysis in the classroom. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 593611.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, H. (1961). Aspects of the problem of common-sense knowledge of social structures. In Wolff, K. H., ed., Transactions of the Fourth World Conference of Sociology (1959). Louvain: International Sociological Association, pp. 5165.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, H. ([1948] 2006). Seeing Sociologically: The Routine Grounds of Social Action. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers.Google Scholar
Haugh, M. (2017). Prompting offers of assistance in interaction. Pragmatics and Society, 8(2), 183207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugh, M. & Pillet-Shore, D. M. (2018). Getting to know you: Teasing as an invitation to intimacy in initial interactions. Discourse Studies, 20(2), 246–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayashi, M. & Yoon, K. (2009). Negotiating boundaries in talk. In Sidnell, J., ed., Conversation Analysis: Comparative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 250–78.Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (2012a). Epistemics in action: Action formation and territories of knowledge. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(1), 129.Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (2012b). The epistemic engine: Sequence organization and territories of knowledge. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(1), 3052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heritage, J. (2016). On the diversity of “changes of state” and their indices. Journal of Pragmatics, 104, 207–10.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. & Schenkein, J. (1978). Some sequential negotiations in conversation: Unexpanded and expanded versions of projected action sequences. In Schenkein, J., ed., Studies in the Organization of Conversational Interaction. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 155–72.Google Scholar
Kevoe-Feldman, H. & Robinson, J. D. (2012). Exploring essentially three-turn courses of action: An institutional case study with implications for ordinary talkDiscourse Studies14(2), 217–41.Google Scholar
Kim, H. R. S. (2013). Retroactive indexing of relevance: The use of “well” in third position. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 46(2), 125–43.Google Scholar
Lerner, G. H. (2019). When someone other than the addressed recipient speaks next: Three kinds of intervening action after the selection of next speaker. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 52(4), 388405.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2013). Action formation and ascription. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 103–30.Google Scholar
Maynard, D. W. (1989). Perspective-display sequences in conversation. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 53(2), 91113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazeland, H. (2013). Grammar in conversation. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 475–91.Google Scholar
Merritt, M. (1976). On questions following questions in service encounters. Language in Society, 5(3), 315–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pillet-Shore, D. (2018). How to begin. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 51(3), 213–31.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, A. & Heritage, J. (2013). Preference. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 210–28.Google Scholar
Rawls, A. W. (2005). Garfinkel’s conception of time. Time and Society, 14, 163–90.Google Scholar
Rawls, A. W. (2006). Respecifying the study of social order: Garfinkel’s transition from theoretical conceptualization to practices in details. In Rawls, A. W., ed., Seeing Sociologically: The Routine Grounds of Social Action by Harold Garfinkel. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers, pp. 197.Google Scholar
Raymond, G. (2003). Grammar and social organization: Yes/no interrogatives and the structure of responding. American Sociological Review, 68(6), 939–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. D. (2013). Overall structural organization. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds., Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 257–80.Google Scholar
Robinson, J. D. (2016). Accountability in social interaction. In Robinson, J. D., ed., Accountability in Social Interaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H. (1987). On the preferences for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation. In Button, G. & Lee, J., eds., Talk and Social Organisation. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, pp. 5469.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on Conversation, Volume 2. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696735.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1981). Discourse as interactional achievement: Some uses of “uh huh” and other things that come between sentences. In Tannen, D., ed., Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 7193.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1988). Discourse as interactional achievement II: An exercise in conversation analysis. In Tannen, D., ed., Linguistics in Context. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing, pp. 135–58.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1991). Reflections on talk and social structure. In Boden, D. & Zimmerman, D. H., eds., Talk and Social Structure. Cambridge: Polity, pp. 4470.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97(5), 1295–345.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1995). Discourse as interactional achievement III: The omnirelevance of action. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28(3), 185211.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closingsSemiotica8(4), 289327.Google Scholar
Seuren, L. M. (2018). Assessing answers: Action ascription in third position. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 51(1), 3351.Google Scholar
Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T., eds. (2013). Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stevanovic, M. & Peräkylä, A. (2014). Three orders in the organization of human action: On the interface between knowledge, power, and emotion in interaction and social relations. Language in Society, 43(2), 185207.Google Scholar
Stivers, T. & Robinson, J. D. (2006). A preference for progressivity in interaction. Language in Society, 35(3), 367–92.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×