Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:41:13.091Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When radical uncertainty is too much: Clinical aspects of Conviction Narrative Theory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Omer Linkovski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and The Gonda Interdisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan 5290002, Israel [email protected] https://linkovskilab.com
Renana Eitan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Tel-Aviv Medical Center, Tel-Aviv 6423906, Israel [email protected] https://www.tasmc.org.il/sites/en/Personnel/pages/eitan-renana.aspx Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA

Abstract

We propose extrapolating Conviction Narrative Theory (CNT) to clinical psychology and psychiatry. We demonstrate how CNT principles may benefit assessment, therapy, and possibly even modify public health views of neuropsychiatric disorders. Our commentary focuses on hoarding disorder as a model, elaborates on discrepancies in the scientific literature and suggests how the CNT may resolve them.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Johnson et al. provide a thought-provoking and insightful synthesis of ideas advancing our understanding of decision-making. We suggest that the reach of the Conviction Narrative Theory (CNT) affects far broader psychology fields than Johnson et al. propose. We argue for widening CNT's scope to neuropsychiatric disorders, psychotherapy, and their impact on public health.

Considering the CNT as it applies to generic decision-making under radical uncertainty may limit its impact. Difficulties tolerating uncertainty and resolving it are central in multiple neuropsychiatric disorders (Gillett, Bilek, Hanna, & Fitzgerald, Reference Gillett, Bilek, Hanna and Fitzgerald2018; Lebert, Turkington, Freeston, & Dudley, Reference Lebert, Turkington, Freeston and Dudley2021). Individuals often portray such difficulties using subjective reports, objective measures, or both. There are discrepancies between subjective reports and objective tasks assessing resolution of uncertainty or decision-making under uncertainty. Healthy adults' objective resolution of uncertainty may differ under certain conditions, but their subjective estimate of uncertainty remains constant in these different conditions (Linkovski, Rodriguez, Wheaton, Henik, & Anholt, Reference Linkovski, Rodriguez, Wheaton, Henik and Anholt2021). In other studies, the association between subjective intolerance of uncertainty (IU) and subjective anxiety severity was higher than the association between IU and objective decision-making under uncertainty (Luhmann, Ishida, & Hajcak, Reference Luhmann, Ishida and Hajcak2011). The CNT accounts for these discrepancies as the objective uncertainty measures include a finite set of options and the subjective measures assess IU in real life, where radical uncertainty is abundant.

The discrepancy between objective and subjective uncertainty measures is imperative for neuropsychiatric disorders. Difficulties resolving uncertainty are transdiagnostic and may serve as a treatment target but are often measured with subjective assessments (McEvoy, Hyett, Shihata, Price, & Strachan, Reference McEvoy, Hyett, Shihata, Price and Strachan2019). Multiple clinical cohorts report increased subjective IU (Gillett et al., Reference Gillett, Bilek, Hanna and Fitzgerald2018) and subjective IU predicts response to psychotherapy (Castriotta, Dozier, Taylor, Mayes, & Ayers, Reference Castriotta, Dozier, Taylor, Mayes and Ayers2019). Objective uncertainty measures most often include monetary decision-making or cognitive reasoning (Aranovich, Cavagnaro, Pitt, Myung, & Mathews, Reference Aranovich, Cavagnaro, Pitt, Myung and Mathews2017; Pushkarskaya et al., Reference Pushkarskaya, Tolin, Ruderman, Kirshenbaum, Kelly, Pittenger and Levy2015, Reference Pushkarskaya, Tolin, Ruderman, Henick, Kelly, Pittenger and Levy2017; Ruderman et al., Reference Ruderman, Ehrlich, Roy, Pietrzak, Harpaz-Rotem and Levy2016; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Fradkin, McNally, Linkovski, Anholt and Huppert2020; Zald & Treadway, Reference Zald and Treadway2017) as they are derived from rational approaches and are easily inferred. These tasks informed our understanding of neuropsychiatric disorders and hold some predictive validity to developing psychopathologies (Ruderman et al., Reference Ruderman, Ehrlich, Roy, Pietrzak, Harpaz-Rotem and Levy2016). Yet there are discrepancies between these objective tasks and subjective assessments. For example, hoarding disorder (HD) patients struggle with letting go of items, irrespective of their objective value (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These patients have elevated subjective IU scores and often report struggling with uncertainty in their daily life (Wheaton, Abramowitz, Jacoby, Zwerling, & Rodriguez, Reference Wheaton, Abramowitz, Jacoby, Zwerling and Rodriguez2016). Objective decision-making tasks suggest that HD patients have intact decision-making under uncertainty, although they may be less flexible (Pushkarskaya et al., Reference Pushkarskaya, Tolin, Ruderman, Henick, Kelly, Pittenger and Levy2017) and that these patients are less sensitive to losses than healthy adults (Aranovich et al., Reference Aranovich, Cavagnaro, Pitt, Myung and Mathews2017). In real life however, HD patients may be extremely sensitive to losing items (Orr, Preston-Shoot, & Braye, Reference Orr, Preston-Shoot and Braye2019). This discrepancy may be reduced if we incorporate CNT principles in objective tasks. For example, using emotionally laden tasks yielded significant leaps in our understanding of neuronal mechanisms of HD (Tolin et al., Reference Tolin, Stevens, Villavicencio, Norberg, Calhoun, Frost and Pearlson2012). We hypothesize that experimental tasks using multiple decision options, affect-laden stimuli, and having individuals explain their narrative fragments or engage in affective valuation or simulation of decision outcomes will result in much greater differences between HD and non-HD cohorts.

The CNT framework aligns with psychotherapeutic interventions. Working with clients to expose, expand, and alter their narratives is a common goal of many psychotherapies (Goldblatt, Briggs, Lindner, Schechter, & Ronningstam, Reference Goldblatt, Briggs, Lindner, Schechter and Ronningstam2015; Rhodes, Reference Rhodes2013). In HD, first-line psychotherapies aim to modify thoughts, beliefs, and decision-making processes assigned to items and to the self (Mathews et al., Reference Mathews, Mackin, Chou, Uhm, Bain, Stark and Delucchi2018; Steketee & Frost, Reference Steketee and Frost2013; Tolin, Frost, & Steketee, Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee2007). The CNT may define these thoughts, beliefs, and decision-making processes as narratives or narrative fragments. In line with CNT principles, current treatments have therapists or volunteers aiding HD clients assess their narratives of items and verbally simulate outcomes of their decisions in their homes – such practices may increase affective reasoning and widen the narrative fragments (Crone, Angel, Isemann, & Norberg, Reference Crone, Angel, Isemann and Norberg2020; Linkovski et al., Reference Linkovski, Zwerling, Cordell, Sonnenfeld, Willis, La Lima and Rodriguez2018; Muroff & Otte, Reference Muroff and Otte2019). The CNT suggests that narratives bind processes and perception to guide actions (target article). Therapists may work with clients on explicitly communicating new narrative fragments, which in turn will modify clients' narrative and alter their actions in a more profound way.

CNT discusses shared narratives and their role in economic outcomes. Neuropsychiatric disorders are a leading cause in reducing economic productivity (Chen, Kuhn, Prettner, & Bloom, Reference Chen, Kuhn, Prettner and Bloom2018; James et al., Reference James, Abate, Abate, Abay, Abbafati, Abbasi and Murray2018; Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Jacobi, Rehm, Gustavsson, Svensson, Jönsson and Steinhausen2011). Economic models suggest that major depressive disorder alone costs the US economy 326 billion dollars per year (Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Fournier, Sisitsky, Simes, Berman, Koenigsberg and Kessler2021). These disorders are associated with societal stigma (Chasson, Guy, Bates, & Corrigan, Reference Chasson, Guy, Bates and Corrigan2018; Corrigan, Reference Corrigan2004) which is internalized and affects treatment-seeking and informing mental health status with employers (Clement et al., Reference Clement, Schauman, Graham, Maggioni, Evans-Lacko, Bezborodovs and Thornicroft2015; Schnyder, Panczak, Groth, & Schultze-Lutter, Reference Schnyder, Panczak, Groth and Schultze-Lutter2017). Increasing help-seeking behaviors may lessen the economic burden of neuropsychiatric disorders. Extrapolating the CNT suggests that changing the shared narrative of mental health may reduce the economic costs of neuropsychiatric disorders.

To summarize, we propose that CNT can benefit how we study and treat neuropsychiatric disorders and help decision-makers and health systems lessen the global burden these disorders cause.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

The current work was partially funded by an Israel Science Foundation Grant to Dr. Eitan (#2128/19).

Competing interest

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association (Ed.). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Aranovich, G. J., Cavagnaro, D. R., Pitt, M. A., Myung, J. I., & Mathews, C. A. (2017). A model-based analysis of decision making under risk in obsessive-compulsive and hoarding disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 90, 126132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castriotta, N., Dozier, M. E., Taylor, C. T., Mayes, T., & Ayers, C. R. (2019). Intolerance of uncertainty in hoarding disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 21, 97101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chasson, G. S., Guy, A. A., Bates, S., & Corrigan, P. W. (2018). They aren't like me, they are bad, and they are to blame: A theoretically-informed study of stigma of hoarding disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 16, 5665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S., Kuhn, M., Prettner, K., & Bloom, D. E. (2018). The macroeconomic burden of noncommunicable diseases in the United States: Estimates and projections. PLoS ONE, 13(11), e0206702. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206702CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clement, S., Schauman, O., Graham, T., Maggioni, F., Evans-Lacko, S., Bezborodovs, N., … Thornicroft, G. (2015). What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(1), 1127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714000129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, P. (2004). How stigma interferes with mental health care. American Psychologist, 59(7), 614625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, C., Angel, Z., Isemann, S., & Norberg, M. M. (2020). Clutter-Buddies: A volunteer program to assist clients undergoing group cognitive behavioural therapy. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 27, 100559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2020.100559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillett, C. B., Bilek, E. L., Hanna, G. L., & Fitzgerald, K. D. (2018). Intolerance of uncertainty in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: A transdiagnostic construct with implications for phenomenology and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 60, 100108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.01.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldblatt, M. J., Briggs, S., Lindner, R., Schechter, M., & Ronningstam, E. (2015). Psychodynamic psychotherapy with suicidal adolescents. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 29(1), 2037. https://doi.org/10.1080/02668734.2015.1004188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, P. E., Fournier, A.-A., Sisitsky, T., Simes, M., Berman, R., Koenigsberg, S. H., & Kessler, R. C. (2021). The economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2010 and 2018). PharmacoEconomics, 39(6), 653665. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40273-021-01019-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, S. L., Abate, D., Abate, K. H., Abay, S. M., Abbafati, C., Abbasi, N., … Murray, C. J. L. (2018). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. The Lancet, 392(10159), 17891858. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lebert, L., Turkington, D., Freeston, M., & Dudley, R. (2021). Rumination, intolerance of uncertainty and paranoia in treatment resistant psychosis. Psychosis, 13(1), 6570. https://doi.org/10.1080/17522439.2020.1798489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linkovski, O., Rodriguez, C. I., Wheaton, M. G., Henik, A., & Anholt, G. E. (2021). Momentary induction of inhibitory control and its effects on uncertainty. Journal of Cognition, 4(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linkovski, O., Zwerling, J., Cordell, E., Sonnenfeld, D., Willis, H., La Lima, C. N., … Rodriguez, C. I. (2018). Augmenting buried in treasures with in-home uncluttering practice: Pilot study in hoarding disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 107, 145150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luhmann, C. C., Ishida, K., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Intolerance of uncertainty and decisions about delayed, probabilistic rewards. Behavior Therapy, 42(3), 378386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2010.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, C. A., Mackin, R. S., Chou, C.-Y., Uhm, S. Y., Bain, L. D., Stark, S. J., … Delucchi, K. (2018). Randomised clinical trial of community-based peer-led and psychologist-led group treatment for hoarding disorder. BJPsych Open, 4(4), 285293. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2018.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEvoy, P. M., Hyett, M. P., Shihata, S., Price, J. E., & Strachan, L. (2019). The impact of methodological and measurement factors on transdiagnostic associations with intolerance of uncertainty: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 73, 101778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muroff, J., & Otte, S. (2019). Innovations in CBT treatment for hoarding: Transcending office walls. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 23, 100471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2019.100471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, D. M. R., Preston-Shoot, M., & Braye, S. (2019). Meaning in hoarding: Perspectives of people who hoard on clutter, culture and agency. Anthropology & Medicine, 26(3), 263279. https://doi.org/10.1080/13648470.2017.1391171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pushkarskaya, H., Tolin, D., Ruderman, L., Henick, D., Kelly, J. M., Pittenger, C., & Levy, I. (2017). Value-based decision making under uncertainty in hoarding and obsessive-compulsive disorders. Psychiatry Research, 258, 305315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pushkarskaya, H., Tolin, D., Ruderman, L., Kirshenbaum, A., Kelly, J. M., Pittenger, C., & Levy, I. (2015). Decision-making under uncertainty in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 69, 166173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, J. (2013). Narrative CBT: Distinctive features. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruderman, L., Ehrlich, D. B., Roy, A., Pietrzak, R. H., Harpaz-Rotem, I., & Levy, I. (2016). Posttraumatic stress symptoms and aversion to ambiguous losses in combat veterans. Depression and Anxiety, 33(7), 606613. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnyder, N., Panczak, R., Groth, N., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2017). Association between mental health-related stigma and active help-seeking: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(4), 261268. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.116.189464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2013). Treatment for hoarding disorder: workbook (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Strauss, A. Y., Fradkin, I., McNally, R. J., Linkovski, O., Anholt, G. E., & Huppert, J. D. (2020). Why check? A meta-analysis of checking in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Threat vs. distrust of senses. Clinical Psychology Review, 75, 101807. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101807CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolin, D. F., Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2007). Buried in treasures: Help for compulsive acquiring, saving, and hoarding (1st ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tolin, D. F., Stevens, M. C., Villavicencio, A. L., Norberg, M. M., Calhoun, V. D., Frost, R. O., … Pearlson, G. D. (2012). Neural mechanisms of decision making in hoarding disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(8), 832841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheaton, M. G., Abramowitz, J. S., Jacoby, R. J., Zwerling, J., & Rodriguez, C. I. (2016). An investigation of the role of intolerance of uncertainty in hoarding symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 193, 208214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H. U., Jacobi, F., Rehm, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jönsson, B., … Steinhausen, H.-C. (2011). The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 21(9), 655679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zald, D. H., & Treadway, M. T. (2017). Reward processing, neuroeconomics, and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13(1), 471495. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-044957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed