Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T19:02:13.188Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A theory limited in scope and evidence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Elena Miu
Affiliation:
School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]
Robert Boyd
Affiliation:
School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]
Peter J. Richerson
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California Davis, CA95616. [email protected]
Thomas J. H. Morgan
Affiliation:
School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]

Abstract

What promised to be a refreshing addition to cumulative cultural evolution, by moving the focus from cultural transmission to technological innovation, falls flat through a lack of thoroughness, explanatory power, and data. A comprehensive theory of cumulative cultural change must carefully integrate all existing evidence in a cohesive multi-level account. We argue that the manuscript fails to do so convincingly.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. (2011) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108(Suppl. 2):10918–25. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R. T. (2017) A different kind of animal: How culture transformed our species, vol. 46. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C. & Sperber, D. (2014a) How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369:20130368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earl, J. W. & McCleary, B. V. (1994) Mystery of the poisoned expedition. Nature 368(6473):683–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. (2016) The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. (2012) What's social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology 126(2):193202. doi: 10.1037/a0025180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, S., Cornish, H. & Smith, K. (2008) Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105(31):10681–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. (2017a) Darwin's unfinished symphony: How culture made the human mind. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2016) Cultural evolution: A review of theory, findings and controversies. Evolutionary Biology 43(4):481–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Dall, S. R. X. & Thornton, A. (2016) The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 31(3):215–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, T. J. H. (2016) Testing the cognitive and cultural niche theories of human evolution. Current Anthropology 57(3):370–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthukrishna, M. & Henrich, J. (2016) Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1690):20150192. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed