Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T03:50:25.689Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Missing in action: Tool use is action based

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Jeffrey J. Lockman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Tulane University, New Orleans, [email protected]://childdevelopment.tulane.edu/
Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, [email protected]://wp.nyu.edu/catherinetamislemonda/
Karen E. Adolph
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY10003. [email protected]://www.psych.nyu.edu/adolph/

Abstract

In this commentary on Osiurak and Reynaud's target article, we argue that action is largely missing in their account of the ascendance of human technological culture. We propose that an action-based developmental account can help to bridge the cognitive-sociocultural divide in explanations of the discovery, production, and cultural transmission of human tool use.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolph, K. E. & Robinson, S. R. (2015) Motor development. In: Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, 7th ed., vol. 2, Cognitive Processes, eds. Liben, L. & Muller, U., pp. 113–57. Wiley.Google Scholar
Chappell, J. & Kacelnik, A. (2002) Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition 5:7178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontenelle, S. A., Kahrs, B. A., Neal, S. A., Newton, A. T. & Lockman, J. J. (2007) Infant manual exploration of composite substrates. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 98:153–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaver, W. W. (1991) Technology affordances. In: Proceedings of the CHI’91 SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems, eds. Robertson, S. P., Olson, G. & Olson, J., pp. 7984. ACM Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J. (1982) The concept of affordances in development: The renascence of functionalism. In: The concept of development: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology, vol. 15, ed. Collins, W. A., pp. 5581. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Inoue-Nakamura, N. & Matsuzawa, T. (1997) Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 111:159–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahrs, B. A., Jung, W. P. & Lockman, J. J. (2014) When does tool use become distinctively human? Hammering in young children. Child Development 85:10501061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenward, B., Schloegl, C., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A. S., Bugnyar, T. & Kacelnik, A. (2011) On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool-oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 102:870–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2017) Raising children: Surprising insights from other cultures. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lederman, S. J. & Klatzky, R. L. (1987) Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology 19:342–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockman, J. J. (2000) A perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child Development 71:137–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachwani, J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Lockman, J. J., Karasik, L. B. & Adolph, K. E. (2020) Learning the designed actions of everyday objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 149:6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resende, B. D., Ottoni, E. B. & Fragaszy, D. M. (2008) Ontogeny of manipulative behavior and nut-cracking in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): A perception-action perspective. Developmental Science 11:828–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visalberghi, E., Addessi, E., Truppa, V., Spagnoletti, N., Ottoni, E., Izar, P. & Fragaszy, D. (2009a) Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded capuchin monkeys. Current Biology 19:213–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar