Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-l4ctd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-06T14:01:24.966Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - A developmental approach to genetic disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Marcia A. Barnes
Affiliation:
University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In recent years, several researchers have attempted to apply the adult neuropsychological model to neurodevelopmental disorders. In the adult model, localized brain damage gives rise to a juxtaposed pattern of impaired and intact abilities. When applied to neurodevelopmental disorders, this approach fails to capture the role of development itself in the formation of the final phenotype. An alternative to this common strategy is a neuroconstructivist approach that considers the contribution of the infant start state, the entire developmental trajectory, and underlying cognitive processes to the phenotypic outcome. Results from studies of language and number in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome, along with a briefer discussion of face processing, will be presented in support of this position. The application of this approach to other developmental disorders such as autism will also be discussed.

The traditional adult neuropsychological approach to disorder is a static one, in contrast to a neuroconstructivist perspective, which emphasizes the importance of development and of studying early development and not just the endstate in older children and adults. A theoretically driven investigation of the cognitive processes underlying behavior as well as the use of converging measures to describe these processes are crucial aspects of understanding neurodevelopmental disorders from a neoconstructivist approach.

Three developmental disorders, i.e., Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and autism, will be used to illustrate the developmental neoconstructive approach. The majority of studies to be discussed concern the first two groups.

Type
Chapter
Information
Genes, Brain and Development
The Neurocognition of Genetic Disorders
, pp. 175 - 198
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

,American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Ansari, D., Donlan, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Typical and atypical development of visual estimation abilities. Cortex, 6, 758–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansari, D., Donlan, C., Thomas, M. S., Ewing, S. A., Peen, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 50–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E. (1997). Plasticity, localization and language development. In Broman, S. H. & Fletcher, J. M. (Eds.), The Changing Nervous System: Neurobehavioral Consequences of Early Brain Disorders (pp. 214–53). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, I., & Volterra, V. (1979). The Emergence of Symbols, Cognition, and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 6, 115–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Bertrand, J., Mervis, C. B., Rice, C. E., & Adamson, L. (1993). Development of joint attention by a toddler with Williams syndrome. Paper presented at the Gatlinburg Conference on Research and Theory in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, Gatlinburg, TN.
Brock, J. (2007). Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 97–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, J. H., Johnson, M. H., Paterson, S. J., Gilmore, R., Longhi, E., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Spatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1037–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge of number: Its evolution and ontogeny. Science, 282, 641–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caycho, L., Gunn, P., & Siegal, M. (1991). Counting by children with Down's syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 575–83.Google Scholar
,CDC. (2007). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 Sites, United States, 2002. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 56, 12–28.Google Scholar
Cipolotti, L., Butterworth, B., & Denes, G. (1991). A specific deficit for numbers in a case of dense acalculia. Brain, 114, 2619–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clearfield, M. W. & Mix, K. S. (1999). Number versus contour length in infants' discrimination of small visual sets. Psychological Science, 10, 408–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1989). The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data. Perception and Psychophysics, 45(6), 557–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1994). Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosis patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 958–75.Google ScholarPubMed
Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Livet, J. O., Casse-Perrot, C. & Schonen, S. (1999). Configural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 41, 276–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 199–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Mancini, J., & Livet, M. (2003). Exploring face processing in Williams syndrome. Cognitie, Creier, Comportanent, 7, 157–71.Google Scholar
Donnai, D. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2000). Williams syndrome: From genotype through to the cognitive phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 97(2), 164–71.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Franco, F. & Wishart, J. G. (1995). Use of pointing and other gestures by young children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100(2), 160–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Frith, U.Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Oxford: Blackwell; 1989.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21(1), 125–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, V., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Funnell, E., & Tassabehji, M. (2006). In-depth analysis of spatial cognition in Williams syndrome: A critical assessment of the role of the LIMK1 gene. Neuropyschologia, 44(5), 679–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 213–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, S. J., Spratling, M. W., Karmiloff-Smith, A., et al. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport. 12(12), 2697–700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Jordan, N. C., & Levine, S. C. (1994). A mental model for early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(3), 284–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Järvinen-Pasley, A., Bellugi, U., Reilly, J., et al. (2008). Defining the social phenotype in Williams syndrome: A model for linking gene, brain, and cognition. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (1997). Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introduction. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. C. (2000). The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 22–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English- learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Crucial differences between developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 513–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Brown, J. H., Grice, S., & Paterson, S. J. (2002). Dethroning the myth: Cognitive dissociations and innate modularity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 227–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P., & Udwin, O. (1997). Language and Williams Syndrome: How intact is “intact”?Child Development, 68, 246–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Scerif, G., & Ansari, D. (2003). Double dissociations in developmental disorders? Theoretically misconceived, empirically dubious. Cortex, 39, 161–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Scerif, G., & Thomas, M. (2002). Different approaches to relating genotype to phenotype in developmental disorders. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 311–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., et al. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1258–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Tyler, L. K., Voice, K., et al. (1998). Linguistic dissociations in Williams syndrome: Evaluating receptive syntax in on-line and off-line tasks. Neuropsychologia, 36(4), 342–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. C., & Shatz, C. J. (1996). Synaptic activity and the construction of cortical circuits. Science, 274, 1133–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 483–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, B. P., & Mervis, C. B. (1999). Contrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 177–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Science, 358(1430), 345–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. J. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., et al. (2002). Atypical linguistic and socio-communicative development in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5(2), 233–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1997). Developmental relations between cognition and language: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In: Adamson, L. B. & Romski, M. A. (Eds.), Research on Communication and Language Disorders: Contributions to Theories of Language Development (pp. 75–106). New York: Brookes.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Morris, J., Bertrand, J., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental Disorders (pp. 65–110). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mills, D. L., Alvarez, T. D., St. George, M., Appelbaum, L. G., Bellugi, U., & Neville, H. (2000). Electrophysiological studies of face processing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.), 47–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyer, R. S. & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 325–38.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P. & Hogan, A. (1996). A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Available through the University of Miami, Department of Psychology, Coral Gables, Florida: www.psy.miami.edu/
Nazzi, T. & Gopnik, A. (2001). Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: When does language become a tool for categorization?Cognition, 80, B11–B20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Visual- and name-based object categorization by children with Williams syndrome. Neuroreport, 13, 1259–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Paterson, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Early word segmentation by infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Infancy, 4(2), 251–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, H., et al. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nye, J., Clibbens, J., & Bird, G. (1995). Numerical ability, general ability and language in children with Down's Syndrome. Down's Syndrome: Research and Practice, 3(3), 92–103.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, W. T., & Warren, S. T. (2002). A decade of molecular studies of fragile X syndrome. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 315–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Roak, B. & State, M. (2008). Autism genetics: Strategies, challenges, and opportunities. Autism Research, 1, 4–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, S. (2000). The Development of Language and Number Understanding in Williams Syndrome and Down Syndrome: Evidence from the Infant and Mature Phenotypes. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University College London.Google Scholar
Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsodl, M. K., Johnson, M. H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. D. (1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science, 286(5448), 2355–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, S., Girelli, L., Butterworth, B., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Are numerical difficulties syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 47(2), 190–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York, NY: Harper Collins Publishers, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1996). Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 606–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scerif, G. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). The dawn of cognitive genetics? Crucial developmental caveats. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 126–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scerif, G., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Campos, R., Elsabbagh, M., Driver, J., & Cornish, K. (2005). To look or not to look? Typical and atypical development of oculomotor control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 591–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, T. J. (1997). Reconceptualizing the origins of number knowledge: A non-numerical account. Cognitive Development, 3, 349–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, W. & Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18, 555–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 345–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, T. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Word learning in a special population: Do individuals with Williams syndrome obey lexical constraints?Journal of Child Language, 24, 737–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J. (1998). The effects of early focal brain injury on lateralization of cognitive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(1), 21–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 269–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals about the understanding of mind in children with autism. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Autism (pp. 138–57). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tamanini, F., Willemsen, R., Unen, L., et al. (1997). Differential expression of FMR1, FXR1 and FXR2 proteins in human and brain testis. Human Molecular Genetics, 6, 1315–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, C. M. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and its applications to children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 27–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Grant, J., Barham, Z., et al. (2001). Past tense formation in Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2(16), 143–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Can developmental disorders reveal the component parts of the human language faculty?Language Learning and Development, 1(1), 65–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udwin, O., Davies, M., & Howlin, P. (1996). A longitudinal study of cognitive abilities and educational attainment in Williams syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 1020–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Udwin, O. & Yule, W. (1991). A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 232–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uller, C., Carey, S., Huntley-Fenner, G., & Klatt, L. (1999). What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge?Cognitive Development, 14(1), 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkmar, F., Chawarska, K., & Klin, A. (2005). Autism in infancy and early childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 12.1–12.22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Pezzini, G., Sabbadini, L., & Vicari, S. (1996). Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 32, 663–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., & Rogers, T. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×