Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-s9k8s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-01T07:46:24.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sleep deprivation in development of obesity, effects on appetite regulation, energy metabolism, and dietary choices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2023

Masoumeh Akhlaghi*
Affiliation:
Department of Community Nutrition, School of Nutrition and Food Sciences, Shiraz University of Medical Sciences, Shiraz, Iran
Ali Kohanmoo
Affiliation:
Department of Community Nutrition, School of Nutrition and Food Sciences, Shiraz University of Medical Sciences, Shiraz, Iran
*
*Corresponding author: Masoumeh Akhlaghi, emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Sleep deprivation, which is a decrease in duration and quality of sleep, is a common problem in today’s life. Epidemiological and interventional investigations have suggested a link between sleep deprivation and overweight/obesity. Sleep deprivation affects homeostatic and non-homoeostatic regulation of appetite, with the food reward system playing a dominant role. Factors such as sex and weight status affect this regulation; men and individuals with excess weight seem to be more sensitive to reward-driven and hedonistic regulation of food intake. Sleep deprivation may also affect weight through affecting physical activity and energy expenditure. In addition, sleep deprivation influences food selection and eating behaviours, which are mainly managed by the food reward system. Sleep-deprived individuals mostly crave for palatable energy-dense foods and have low desire for fruit and vegetables. Consumption of meals may not change but energy intake from snacks increases. The individuals have more desire for snacks with high sugar and saturated fat content. The relationship between sleep and the diet is mutual, implying that diet and eating behaviours also affect sleep duration and quality. Consuming healthy diets containing fruit and vegetables and food sources of protein and unsaturated fats and low quantities of saturated fat and sugar may be used as a diet strategy to improve sleep. Since the effects of sleep deficiency differ between animals and humans, only evidence from human subject studies has been included, controversies are discussed and the need for future investigations is highlighted.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

Sleep physiology

Sleep is a basic need for humans(Reference Stich, Huwiler, D’Hulst and Lustenberger1). Humans spend almost a third of their life in sleep. Sleep is needed for removing toxic metabolites, synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation. It is also required for normal metabolic activity, hormone regulation, tissue repair and optimal function of cardiovascular and immune system(Reference Luyster, Strollo, Zee and Walsh2).

Sleep cycle has two main phases: non-rapid eye movement (NREM) and rapid eye movement (REM)(Reference Le Bon3,Reference Patel, Reddy, Shumway and Araujo4) . NREM has three stages: N1, N2 and N3. N1 is the lightest stage of sleep, and is the transition state between wakefulness and deep sleep. It is associated with muscle relaxation, and lasts for a few minutes. N2 is deeper and is associated with decreased body temperature and heart rate. It constitutes 50% of total sleep time. N3, which is also called the slow-wave sleep (SWS), is the deepest stage of sleep. At this stage, the body is in complete relaxation and the person hardly wakes up. NREM sleep is essential for physical recovery and repair. A reduction in NREM results in physical and metal fatigue, reduced performance and impaired cognitive function(Reference Armitage, Landis, Hoffmann, Lentz, Watson, Goldberg and Buchwald5,Reference Wilckens, Ferrarelli, Walker and Buysse6) .

REM sleep is the stage where most dreams occur(Reference Patel, Reddy, Shumway and Araujo4). It is characterised by quick eye movement behind the eyelids, muscle relaxation, irregular breathing, elevated heart rate and increased brain activity. NREM stage accounts for 75% of the total sleep time, leaving the remaining 25% for REM stage. Stages of sleep are repeated regularly throughout the night. Sleep cycle starts with N1, deepens through N2 and N3, and ends in REM. After the REM sleep, the cycle starts again. The duration of each sleep cycle is approximately 90 min. Each night has four to six sleep cycles. As the night progresses and the morning approaches, the length of NREM sleep decreases and that of REM increases(Reference Stich, Huwiler, D’Hulst and Lustenberger1,Reference Patel, Reddy, Shumway and Araujo4,Reference Porkka-Heiskanen, Zitting and Wigren7) .

It is postulated that sleep is regulated by interaction of two processes: process S and process C(Reference Borbély, Daan, Wirz-Justice and Deboer8,Reference Deboer9) . Process S or homoeostasis process controls the amount of sleep required for restoration of physical and mental functions. It keeps the balance between the need for sleep and wakefulness, and determines the amount of sleep according to the duration of wakefulness and the amount of sleepiness. During waking hours, adenosine levels gradually increase in the brain, and lead to a feeling of sleepiness and the desire to sleep(Reference Porkka-Heiskanen, Zitting and Wigren7).

Process C or circadian process regulates the timing of sleep according to body’s internal clock, also called the circadian clock(Reference Porkka-Heiskanen, Zitting and Wigren7,Reference Duhart, Inami and Koh10) . The circadian clock is located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) that are a group of neurons situated above the optic chiasm in the brain(Reference Pandi-Perumal, Cardinali, Zaki, Karthikeyan, Spence, Reiter and Brown11). The clock is influenced by external cues such as light and temperature and internal cues such as hormones. In turn, the activation of SCN targets hypothalamus, thereby regulating the rhythmic secretion of many hormones and body temperature. In dark, the SCN stimulates pineal gland to produce melatonin, which promotes sleep, whereas in light, SCN stimulates production of cortisol to promote wakefulness(Reference von Gall12). Thus, melatonin and cortisol have opposite alterations during a 24 h period: melatonin levels are lowest during day and are highest at night, while cortisol levels are lowest at midnight, gradually increase during night sleep, peak at approxmately 9 am, and then gradually decline to reach its minimum again at midnight(Reference Hirotsu, Tufik and Andersen13). An overview of the proposed mechanisms for sleep physiology and diurnal fluctuations of serum melatonin and cortisol is demonstrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Proposed schematic of sleep and wake regulation by two homoeostatic and circadian processes as well as a simplistic view of diurnal changes in melatonin and cortisol levels. Homoeostatic process (blue curve) is hypothesised to be regulated by adenosine levels in the brain. In homoeostatic cycle, neuronal activity gradually increases adenosine levels in the brain, reaching its highest level concomitant with sleepiness and desire to sleep at the time of sleep. In circadian process (red curve), sunlight inhibits the circadian clock located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), thus inhibiting the SCN signals. In dark, SCN signals stimulate the pineal gland to produce melatonin, a molecule synthesised from tryptophan that has an important role in regulation of sleep–wake cycle. SCN also sends signals to the superior cervical ganglion, which regulates circadian rhythm in other parts of the body such as heart and circulatory system. In sleep-deprivation, the regular sleep–wake cycle is disrupted and this leads to misalignment of homoeostatic (demonstrated with brown colour) and circadian processes, leading to a number of brain and systemic dysregulations. Melatonin levels (dashed green curve), which are positively associated with drowsiness and sleep, are lowest during the day and highest during the night. Conversely, cortisol levels (dotted pink curve) are lowest at midnight, gradually increase during night sleep, peak at approximately 09.00, and then gradually decline to reach a minimum again at midnight.

Circadian rhythm plays an important role in maintaining sufficient sleep, and disruption of this rhythm by any factor, such as shift work, can result in sleep problems. Apart from circadian rhythm, the time of sleep is regulated by chronotype. Chronotype is a natural propensity in individuals to sleep or be active and alert at certain times during a 24 h period(Reference Zou, Zhou, Yan, Yao and Lu14). There are two main chronotypes: morning and evening. Individuals with morning chronotype sleep and wake up early and prefer to be active in the morning while those with evening chronotype prefer to sleep and wake up late. Chronotype is influenced by genetics but factors such as age and sex affect it(Reference Randler and Engelke15). In general, infants are morning chronotype, young adults are evening chronotype, and in middle and old ages people become morning chronotype again(Reference Zou, Zhou, Yan, Yao and Lu14,Reference Randler, Faßl and Kalb16) . Morning chronotype is more frequently seen among women while men are generally evening chronotype(Reference Randler and Engelke15).

Sleep deprivation

Sleep deprivation, also known as sleep deficiency or sleep insufficiency, occurs when the amount and/or quality of sleep is reduced(Reference Abrams17). This may occur when a person does not get sufficient sleep that he/she needs, sleeps at wrong time (e.g. sleeps a part of day instead of night), or has sleep disorders. In low sleep efficiency, the duration of deep sleep, i.e. stage 3 of NREM (N3), is usually reduced(Reference Shrivastava, Jung, Saadat, Sirohi and Crewson18). Sleep deprivation also occurs when the total time spent in bed does not change but the actual time spent asleep reduces(Reference Gruber, Somerville, Enros, Paquin, Kestler and Gillies-Poitras19). US National Sleep Foundation has recommended sleep durations for different age groups as follows: 14–17 h for newborns, 12–15 h for infants, 11–14 h for toddlers, 10–13 h for preschoolers, 9–11 h for school-aged children, 8–10 h for teenagers, 7–9 h for young adults and adults and 7–8 h for older adults(Reference Hirshkowitz, Whiton and Albert20).

Surveys performed in different parts of the world indicate that sleep deprivation is more or less prevalent even among healthy individuals. According to the statistics released on World Sleep Day 2019, adults in 12 countries throughout the world (Australia, Brazil, Canada, China, France, Germany, India, Japan, Netherlands, Singapore, South Korea and the USA) had on average 6·8 h sleep in weeknights and 7·8 h in weekends(21). Overall, 62% of these adults did not feel they were getting enough sleep. In the USA, 35·3% of adults reported <7 h sleep during a typical 24 h period(Reference Liu, Wheaton, Chapman, Cunningham, Lu and Croft22) and 7·1% were diagnosed at least once with a sleep disorder(Reference Huyett, Siegel and Bhattacharyya23). A meta-analysis of surveys conducted in low- and middle-income countries (Iran, Turkey, China, Russia, Georgia, Pakistan, India, Bangladesh, Haiti, Brazil, Mexico, Venezuela, Peru, Ecuador, South Africa, Argentina and Ghana) demonstrated a sleep duration of 7·6 h and 32·8% prevalence of poor sleep quality in working age adults(Reference Simonelli, Marshall, Grillakis, Miller, Hoyos and Glozier24). Another meta-analysis estimated a prevalence of 36% for poor sleep quality, 37% for sleep duration of <7 h, 22% for insomnia and 10% for sleepiness in industrial workers(Reference Amiri25).

Sleep deprivation may result from a variety of medical conditions. These include but not limited to psychiatric diseases (e.g. depression and anxiety)(Reference Geiker, Astrup, Hjorth, Sjödin, Pijls and Markus26), illnesses that bring along physical pain(Reference Sun, Laksono and Selvanathan27), asthma(Reference Kavanagh, Jackson and Kent28), sleep apnea(Reference Kavanagh, Jackson and Kent28), insomnia, parasomnias, Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease, and restless legs syndrome(Reference Colten and Altevogt29). Healthy individuals may experience sleep deficiency as a result of interference of work, school and social activities with sleep time, psychological pressures of modern life, environmental factors such as noise, light, temperature, extensive use of electronic devices, and application of recreational substances such as alcohol, caffeine and nicotine(Reference Zimberg, Dâmaso and Del Re30,Reference McHill and Wright31) . Night shift work(Reference Sanches, Teixeira, dos Santos and Ferreira32), ageing(Reference Pappas and Miner33) and food insecurity(Reference Lee, Deason, Rancourt and Gray34) are also factors that have reduced sleep duration/quality over the last decades(Reference Zimberg, Dâmaso and Del Re30,Reference McHill and Wright31) .

Apart from feeling less refreshed during the day, sleep deprivation has negative effects on agility, productivity, efficiency to perform daily activities and health status(Reference Abrams17,Reference Herzog, Friedrich and Fujita35) . Sleep deprivation leaves adverse effects on cognitive performance and brain function, cardiovascular health, immune system, endocrine homoeostasis and mood(Reference Roenneberg, Foster and Klerman36). It predisposes individuals to obesity, and as a consequence to insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes, dyslipidaemia and other metabolic diseases(Reference Crispim, Zalcman and Dáttilo37,Reference Hatamizargaran, Sasani and Akhlaghi38) . Accordingly, sleep disorder is suggested as a predictor of increased mortality(Reference Huyett, Siegel and Bhattacharyya23,Reference Patel, Ayas and Malhotra39) , and sleep extension has been introduced as a strategy to attenuate obesity risk and cardiometabolic dysfunction(Reference Hoddy, Potts, Bazzano and Kirwan40).

Objective

The current review focuses on two main topics: mechanistic pathways for the role of sleep deprivation in development of obesity and the current knowledge on the relationship between sleep deprivation and food and dietary choices. In more details: first of all, an overview on the description, causes, consequence and prevalence of sleep deprivation is presented. Then, scientific evidence for the link between sleep deprivation and obesity is reviewed, and mechanistic pathways for the effect of sleep deprivation in promoting obesity are discussed. Possible effects of sleep deprivation on energy intake, appetite regulation, physical activity and energy expenditure will be described. Also, the role of sex and weight status in the effect of sleep deprivation on appetite control and food selection will be stated. Impact of sleep deprivation on food selection, diet quality, consumption of snacks and desire for consumption of sweets are among issues that will be discussed in more details. At the end, the relationships are reversed and evidence for possible effects of eating habits, diet quality and dietary components on sleep health will be presented. Applying these diet considerations may help in reducing sleep problems in affected individuals.

Role of sleep deprivation in development of obesity

The relationship of sleep deprivation and weight gain has been highlighted in previous investigations(Reference Cooper, Neufeld, Dolezal and Martin41). Longitudinal cohort studies have shown the fastest weight gain in people who get the least amount of sleep(Reference Patel, Malhotra, White, Gottlieb and Hu42). Also, meta-analyses of cohort studies in both children(Reference Miller, Bates, Ji and Cappuccio43) and adults(Reference Bacaro, Ballesio and Cerolini44) have emphasised short sleep duration as a predisposing factor for obesity. For instance, a meta-analysis of 22 cohort studies on adults showed that <7 h sleep compared with 7–8 h sleep was associated with an odds ratio of 1·26 (95% confidence interval (CI): 1·17, 1·36) for obesity(Reference Guimarães, Silva, Latorraca, Oliveira and Crispim45). Also, a meta-analysis of 33 cohort studies on children and adolescents revealed an obesity risk of 1·57 (95% CI: 1·36, 1·81) for short sleep duration(Reference Deng, He, He, Zhu, Zhang and Niu46). Results of meta-analyses on cohort studies have been confirmed by meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials (RCT), in which strategies that improve duration and/or quality of sleep reduced body weight(Reference Capers, Fobian, Kaiser, Borah and Allison47).

Each hour sleep deprivation has been associated with 0·16 and 0·35 kg/m2 increase in body mass index (BMI) in adolescents(Reference Wells, Hallal, Reichert, Menezes, Araújo and Victora48) and adults(Reference Cappuccio, Taggart and Kandala49), respectively. Accumulation of fat occurs especially in central body areas even when total body fat does not change. As an example, in an RCT on healthy non-obese individuals, alterations in total body fat did not differ between sleep restriction (14 d sleep restriction with 4 h sleep opportunity) and control conditions, but total abdominal fat increased during sleep restriction, with significant increases evident in both subcutaneous and visceral abdominal fat masses(Reference Covassin, Singh and McCrady-Spitzer50). Also, in a study on resident physicians, those with poor sleep quality reported greater weight gain and higher frequency of abnormal waist circumference after beginning of residency compared with physicians with better sleep quality(Reference Mota, Waterhouse and De-Souza51).

In weight loss programmes, sleep disturbance during weight loss has been associated with an elevated risk of failure to lose weight(Reference Li, Li and Zhou52). Moreover, higher sleep fragmentation, which is determined by measuring the number of wake-ups, led to less weight reduction in those who participated in weight loss programmes(Reference Sawamoto, Nozaki and Furukawa53). Energy restriction programmes may have more adverse effects on sleep-restricted individuals. Nedeltcheva et al. reported that energy-restricted individuals under insufficient sleep had a higher loss of fat-free mass whereas those with adequate sleep showed higher loss of fat mass(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Schoeller and Penev54). Sleep curtailment decreased the proportion of weight lost as fat by 55% and increased loss of fat-free mass by 60%.

The association of short sleep duration and obesity seems to be stronger in younger ages. This has been clearly shown in a meta-analysis of observational data collected from 634,511 participants with ages ranging from 2 to 102 years(Reference Cappuccio, Taggart and Kandala49). The relationship of short sleep duration and obesity had odds ratios of 1·89 (95% CI: 1·46, 2·43) and 1·55 (95% CI: 1·43, 1·68) in children and adults, respectively. Short sleep duration in childhood may predict adulthood obesity. A 32 year prospective birth cohort study showed that shorter childhood sleep times were associated with higher adult BMI values, and the association remained after adjustment for adult sleep time and potential confounders such as early childhood BMI, parental BMI and adult physical activity(Reference Landhuis, Poulton, Welch and Hancox55).

It is worthwhile noting that it is not only sleep deprivation that acts as a risk factor for obesity but oversleeping may also function as a risk factor, suggesting a U-shaped relationship between sleep duration and BMI. However, there may be a sex-specific effect in this proposed U-shaped relationship(Reference Ren, Chang, Kang, Zhao, Chen and Pei56). In this regard, a 10 year cohort study in women showed that both habitual short and long sleep durations were risk factors for general obesity in women aged <40 years(Reference Theorell-Haglöw, Berglund, Berne and Lindberg57). Habitual long sleep duration was also a risk factor for central obesity in young women. But, this kind of association was not observed in a meta-analysis which included prospective studies of both sexes(Reference Wu, Zhai and Zhang58). Hence, long sleep duration may be the risk factor of obesity only in women. Since there is still controversy for the existence of such curvilinear association between sleep duration and BMI especially in men(Reference Ren, Chang, Kang, Zhao, Chen and Pei56) more studies examining consequences of long sleep duration are needed. Consistent with what was mentioned for short sleep duration, long sleep duration is also associated with increased mortality risk(Reference Patel, Ayas and Malhotra39).

In the following sections, we will present some of the mechanisms by which sleep deprivation may contribute to obesity.

Energy intake in sleep deprivation

Most obesity cases result from positive energy balance due to extra energy intake(Reference Brondel, Quilliot, Mouillot, Khan, Bastable, Boggio, Leloup and Pénicaud59). Meta-analyses of RCT indicate that partial sleep deprivation increases total energy intake compared with habitual sleep(Reference González-Ortiz, López-Bautista, Valencia-Flores and Espinosa Cuevas60,Reference Soltanieh, Solgi, Ansari, Santos and Abbasi61) . Cross-sectional studies have also shown the association of sleep deprivation and increased energy intake(Reference Zuraikat, Makarem, Liao, St-Onge and Aggarwal62Reference Stelmach-Mardas, Iqbal and Mardas65). Most of this extra energy is consumed in the form of snacks especially after dinner(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66) as discussed later in this article. This increased energy intake is assumed to be a physiologic adaptation to compensate the augmented energy expenditure due to additional wake hours to prevent weight loss(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66). However, easy access to snacks, increased desire for energy-dense snacks after sleep deprivation, and in some cases increased hunger, leads to overeating and weight gain(Reference García-García, Juárez-Aguilar, Santiago-García and Cardinali67), as will be discussed in the following sections.

Despite the above justifications, sleep deprivation has not increased energy intake in some trials with short but severe restriction, for example, after two nights of 4 h sleep(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68) or one night of <5 h sleep(Reference Romney, Larson, Clark, Tucker, Bailey and LeCheminant69). It is likely that psychological stress caused by severe sleep deprivation reduces food intake while in prolonged sleep deprivations a sort of adaption occurs. The lack of the relationship of sleep deprivation with energy intake in some cross-sectional studies can also be justified. In an analysis of NHANES data, short sleepers had lower mean percentage of energy from main meals but higher energy intake from snacks(Reference Kant and Graubard70). Although energy intake did not associate with sleep duration, eating main meals and snacks at or after 20.00 in short-duration sleepers was suggestive of eating behaviours that may, in the long-term, lead to positive energy balance. Also, in a cross-sectional study on Japanese women, energy intake was lower in women with low sleep efficiency compared to participants with the middle and high sleep efficiency(Reference Hashimoto, Inoue and Kuwano71). Since the participants were young Japanese women, the authors justified this unexpected finding to participants’ desire to control their energy intake to prevent obesity, and thus attributed the observed low sleep efficiency a consequence of this eating control.

Appetite regulation in sleep deprivation

Increased food intake is driven by homoeostatic or hormonal and non-homoeostatic mechanisms. In the following sections, we describe these mechanisms in detail.

Homoeostatic regulation of appetite

There is compelling evidence in the literature indicating increased hunger and reduced fullness and satiety following lack or insufficient sleep. A meta-analysis of 15 RCTs showed that both partial and total sleep deprivation increase hunger and daily energy intake(Reference Zhu, Shi, Park, Zhao and Reutrakul72). The increased hunger has been observed in all fasted, pre-prandial(Reference Brondel, Romer, Nougues, Touyarou and Davenne73,Reference Santa Cecília Silva, Lopes and Teixeira74) and sated states(Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Chapman75). In agreement, in a study on adults with overweight and habitual short sleep duration, extending sleep from ∼ 5·6 h to ∼ 7·1 h led to 14% decrease in overall appetite and 62% decrease in desire for sweet and salty foods(Reference Tasali, Chapotot, Wroblewski and Schoeller76). However, not all investigations have reported the change in hunger in sleep restriction(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68,Reference McHill, Hull, McMullan and Klerman77) . Contradictions could be due to differences in the protocol of investigations. For instance, in ad libitum eating condition, participants were less likely to feel hunger than in calculated and fixed amounts of food. The time of waking in the morning, the time interval between awaking and hunger assessment and the type of activities that participants performed after sleep deprivation (e.g. watching television compared with playing games) may have also caused such contradictions in the reports of hunger induction by sleep deprivation(Reference Chaput and St-Onge78).

The role of gut hormones

Leptin and a number of gut hormones are involved in the regulation of appetite and food intake(Reference Yu, Fernandez, Meng, Zhao and Groth79). Leptin and ghrelin have been speculated to play a role in development of obesity in sleep deprivation conditions(Reference Caroleo, Carbone, Primerano, Foti, Brunetti and Segura-Garcia80). Leptin is primarily produced by adipose tissue in amounts proportional to body fat masses(Reference Frago and Chowen81). It is an important anorexigenic hormone which induces satiety and reduces food intake. Ghrelin is mainly secreted by endocrine cells of the stomach and is the chief orexigenic hormone that stimulates hunger. The effect of sleep on leptin and ghrelin levels has not been completely elucidated. Some cross-sectional(Reference Mota, Waterhouse and De-Souza51,Reference Stern, Grant and Thomson64,Reference Taheri, Lin, Austin, Young and Mignot82,Reference Chaput, Després, Bouchard and Tremblay83) and interventional studies(Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Chapman75,Reference Spiegel, Tasali, Penev and Van Cauter84Reference Chapman, Nilsson and Nilsson86) have shown alterations in blood ghrelin and/or leptin concentrations in directions to increase hunger or decrease satiety (i.e. augmented ghrelin and reduced leptin). The same direction of alteration in leptin has been reported for cholecystokinin in a cross-sectional study of adults with obesity, in which low sleep efficiency was associated with lower postprandial cholecystokinin levels(Reference Nymo, Kleppe, Coutinho, Rehfeld, Kulseng and Martins87). As expected, poor sleep quality has the same relationship as short sleep duration with the mentioned appetite-regulating hormones(Reference Mota, Waterhouse and De-Souza51,Reference Nymo, Kleppe, Coutinho, Rehfeld, Kulseng and Martins87,Reference Olson, Hamilton and Somers88) .

Leptin

The decreased leptin concentration after sleep deprivation may play a role in stimulation of obesity in insufficient sleep conditions. In this regard, in a cross-sectional study, after adjustment for plasma leptin levels, adiposity indices were no longer associated with short habitual sleep(Reference Chaput, Després, Bouchard and Tremblay83). The decrease in leptin may be due to increased activity of sympathetic nervous system which occurs following sleep deprivation and has an inhibitory effect on leptin secretion from adipose tissue(Reference Spiegel, Leproult, L’hermite-Balériaux, Copinschi, Penev and Van Cauter89). The inverse relationship between sleep deprivation and leptin levels may be reciprocal, meaning that it is not only sleep deprivation which prevent leptin release, but leptin may in turn contribute to deep sleep(Reference Hirota, Morioka and Yoda90). Hirota et al. reported that plasma leptin levels were independently associated with sleep quality in patients with obesity and type 2 diabetes, although no association was observed in non-obese patients(Reference Hirota, Morioka and Yoda90), suggesting that the association between sleep deprivation and leptin concentrations may be stronger in individuals with obesity, as also evidenced in female children in a population-based study(Reference Boeke, Storfer-Isser, Redline and Taveras91). Since individuals with obesity generally have augmented levels of leptin and considering that due to adiposity they are at increased risk of poor sleep quality(Reference Salzano, Maglitto and Bisogno92), the higher levels of leptin in individuals with obesity may suggest leptin’s potential to improve sleep quality of these patients to compensate, at least partly, for the adverse effects of obesity on their sleep(Reference Hirota, Morioka and Yoda90).

Negative reports and controversies

There are also interventions in which the decreased leptin was not accompanied with an increase in ghrelin(Reference Hart, Carskadon and Considine93) or vice versa(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid, Jauch-Chara, Born and Schultes94,Reference Broussard, Kilkus and Delebecque95) . Literature also contains reports denying the effect of sleep deprivation on appetite-regulating hormones(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66,Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68,Reference Gonnissen, Hursel, Rutters, Martens and Westerterp-Plantenga96Reference Simpson, Banks and Dinges101) , and a meta-analysis of RCT did not show significant effect of sleep restriction on leptin and ghrelin levels(Reference Zhu, Shi, Park, Zhao and Reutrakul72). In contrast, a meta-analysis of both cross-sectional and experimental studies indicated that short sleep duration was associated with increased ghrelin level in cross-sectional studies, but sleep deprivation increased leptin levels in experimental investigations(Reference Lin, Jiang and Wang102). Reasons of controversies may be incompatibility between studies regarding the age, sex, and weight of participants(Reference Isidori, Strollo and Morè103), the energy balance and diet composition during experiments, the extent of sleep deprivation and the type of exercise and activities that participants were allowed during waking hours. For instance, the increased leptin levels in studies where participants had obesity seems reasonable due to the leptin resistance that obesity induces(Reference Izquierdo, Crujeiras, Casanueva and Carreira104). More details on the effects of sex and diet composition on sleep are given in the following sections.

Non-homeostatic regulation of appetite, the role of food reward system

Brain reward system

The paradox of increased food and energy intake in the lack of change in appetite-regulating hormones, e.g.(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97) may be explained by the brain reward system, which in addition to homoeostatic factors regulate the eating. Brain reward system is responsible for stimulating behaviours that increase the likelihood of survival and reproduction(Reference Schultz105). Rewarding stimulus for such a system is any stimulus, object, activity or situation that elicits pleasure and is thus attractive and motivating. Food is one of these stimuli(Reference Jager and Witkamp106). Non-homoeostatic appetite regulation operates independently of homoeostatic regulation. Thus, the increased activity of the reward system and subjective food valuation could occur in the absence of hunger and alteration in appetite-related hormones (ghrelin, leptin, insulin and cortisol)(Reference Rihm, Menz and Schultz99). Food reward consists of two components: appetitive that refers to “wanting” or motivation to consume a food and consummatory which returns to the hedonic aspects of a food and leads to “liking” a food(Reference Jager and Witkamp106).

Neuroanatomy of the food reward system

Parts of cortical and subcortical areas of brain are known to evaluate food stimuli(Reference Greer, Goldstein and Walker107). At the cortical region, the anterior insula cortex, lateral orbital frontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex have roles in governing appetitive choices and food preferences. Thus, the activity of cortical areas relates to the “liking” component of food reward. At subcortical regions, the amygdala and ventral striatum are engaged in managing motivation to eat, what is called “wanting”(Reference Greer, Goldstein and Walker107). In fact, the amygdala is involved in modulation of emotional feeding and reward processing(Reference Zhang, Li and Guo108,Reference Izadi and Radahmadi109) . Reward information from the motivational and hedonic structures is sent to the nucleus accumbens and then to the lateral hypothalamic area to control feeding behaviours(Reference Uribe-Cerda, Morselli and Perez-Leighton110Reference Morales and Berridge112). In abnormal conditions, the function of the reward system is impaired and leads to over- or under-eating. For instance, patients with anorexia or frail elderly lose weight because they suffer from loss of pleasure in eating and appetite(Reference Jager and Witkamp106). In contrast, in some obesity cases, hyper-sensitisation of motivational structures in brain leads to excessive food “wanting”(Reference Morales and Berridge112). Such condition also occurs in cases of drug addiction.

Sleep deprivation and food reward system

A large body of evidence indicates that sleep deprivation alters reward-driven eating behaviour(Reference Chaput113). Animal and human subject studies have shown that sleep deprivation increases motivation for reward(Reference Liu, Wang and Cai114). There are two hypotheses by which the reward system reacts to inadequate sleep. In the first hypothesis, the sensitivity of the reward system to food cues increases, thus individuals have augmented responsiveness to food rewards(Reference Demos, Sweet and Hart115,Reference St-Onge, McReynolds, Trivedi, Roberts, Sy and Hirsch116) . In this regard, Demos et al. showed that compared to sufficient sleep (9 h/night), short sleep (6 h/night) caused greater activity to food images in brain regions of reward processing (e.g nucleus accumbens and putamen) and sensory/motor signalling (i.e. right paracentral lobule) effects that are mostly seen in individuals with obesity(Reference Demos, Sweet and Hart115). Also, St-Onge and colleagues reported an increased neuronal activity in response to food stimuli after restricted sleep (4 h/night) compared with habitual sleep (9 h/night)(Reference St-Onge, McReynolds, Trivedi, Roberts, Sy and Hirsch116). In the second hypothesis, insufficient or low-quality sleep causes weaker neuronal reactivity in reward-related regions in response to food intake, thus more inspiring food rewards (i.e. delicious high-energy foods) may need to induce neurons in the reward system or alternatively more foods need to be consumed to compensate for the reduced reward response and induce good pleasure(Reference Chaput113). Mechanisms involved in both of these hypotheses result in overeating.

Areas of the brain that are affected by sleep deprivation and the manner of changes are subject to controversy in neuroimaging investigations. For instance, Greer et al. reported that one night sleep deprivation suppressed activity in the cortical regions involved in hedonic evaluation and augmented that in the amygdala, leading to increased desire for consumption of high-energy foods(Reference Greer, Goldstein and Walker107). In contrast, Katsunuma et al. found that one night sleep deprivation (39 h wakefulness) activated the right anterior insula in response to food images but the activity of the amygdala remained unchanged although in optimal sleep condition both amygdala and anterior insula had low activity(Reference Katsunuma, Oba and Kitamura117). Likewise, six nights of sleep restriction increased neural activity in response to food stimuli in brain regions associated with reward(Reference St-Onge, McReynolds, Trivedi, Roberts, Sy and Hirsch116). Also, Benedict et al. reported that one night sleep deprivation increased neural activation in the right anterior cingulate cortex in response to food images(Reference Benedict, Brooks and O’Daly118). Although these studies seem contradictory at the first glance, an overview of their results suggests that sleep loss may modulate neural activity in at least one of cortical or subcortical regions. In the majority of the above examples, it was the cortical region (involved in liking) that increased its activity following sleep loss(Reference St-Onge, McReynolds, Trivedi, Roberts, Sy and Hirsch116Reference Benedict, Brooks and O’Daly118), but suppression of cortical neurons when accompanied with heightened subcortical neuron activity also associated with augmented food desirability(Reference Greer, Goldstein and Walker107).

Inhibitory control

In addition to homoeostatic and reward-driven regulation, as a third mechanism, sleep deprivation impairs inhibitory control over eating. Inhibitory control or dietary restraint is defined as the ability to resist a potent stimulus, such as a palatable food(Reference de Klerk, Smeets and la Fleur119). The interaction between food reward system and inhibitory control predicts food intake(Reference Appelhans, Woolf, Pagoto, Schneider, Whited and Liebman120). Hence, a strong food reward perception along with low levels of inhibitory control may lead to overeating. Acute sleep deprivation reduces the ability to exert cognitive inhibition toward food stimuli(Reference Cedernaes, Brandell and Ros121). This low inhibitory control along with hormonal signals for hunger from the gut and/or increased motivation towards food stimuli from the reward system intensifies the risk of obesity. An overview of the possible mechanisms of sleep deprivation in the development of obesity has been given in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2. Possible mechanisms by which sleep deprivation lead to overweight and obesity. CCK, cholecystokinin; GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide-1.

Interaction of different systems

The hormonal, cognitive, emotional and behavioural systems that control feeding behaviour influence and interact with one another(Reference Lundahl and Nelson122,Reference Liu and Kanoski123) . For instance, specific areas of the brain reward system, such as the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus, have receptors for ghrelin and leptin. Moreover, ghrelin levels have been positively correlated with increased sensitivity to dairy odour and higher intensity ratings to food odors including dairy and vanilla(Reference Ginieis, Abeywickrema, Oey and Peng124). In addition, the homoeostatic regulation has a strong control overeating under normal circumstances, but in abnormal conditions like adiposity, depression or sleep deprivation, particularly when the person lives in an obesogenic environment with easy access to highly energetic and palatable foods, the homoeostatic control may be weakened(Reference Berthoud125). Thus, the outcome of the interaction of these systems will finally determine the eating behaviour.

Physical activity in sleep deprivation

Weight gain in sleep deprivation seems to be partly due to decreased exercise and physical activity as a result of fatigue and tiredness(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68). In contrast, good sleep quality may predict increased physical activity through its positive effect on emotion regulation(Reference Semplonius and Willoughby126). On the opposite side of exercise, sedentary activities may be increased during sleep deprivation(Reference Romney, Larson, Clark, Tucker, Bailey and LeCheminant69,Reference Bromley, Booth, Kilkus, Imperial and Penev127Reference Jindal, Puyau, Adolph, Butte, Musaad and Bacha129) . Sedentary activities not only have low energy requirements but they may also promote consumption of high-energy snacks, further predisposing individuals to overweight. A hypothetical scheme for the possible effect of sleep deprivation on physical activity has been depicted in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3. Hypothetical scheme for the possible effect of poor sleep quality on physical activity.

Nonetheless, controlled trials for the effect of sleep deprivation on physical activity are inconsistent and all cases of increased(Reference Brondel, Romer, Nougues, Touyarou and Davenne73,Reference Hursel, Rutters, Gonnissen, Martens and Westerterp-Plantenga130,Reference McNeil, Doucet and Brunet131) , decreased(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68,Reference Bromley, Booth, Kilkus, Imperial and Penev127,Reference Lee132) or of no effect(Reference Romney, Larson, Clark, Tucker, Bailey and LeCheminant69,Reference Porter, Pettit-Mee and Emerson133,Reference Bosy-Westphal, Hinrichs and Jauch-Chara134) in the amount and/or intensity of physical activity have been reported. These controversies may have resulted from diversity in study protocols. For instance, the observation of increased physical activity has been mostly in trials with one night of sleep deprivation(Reference Brondel, Romer, Nougues, Touyarou and Davenne73,Reference McNeil, Doucet and Brunet131) or two nights of fragmented sleep(Reference Hursel, Rutters, Gonnissen, Martens and Westerterp-Plantenga130) where the severity of sleep deprivation was mild. Decreased physical activity has been reported during prolonged (14 d) sleep deprivation(Reference Bromley, Booth, Kilkus, Imperial and Penev127) or in studies where participants were not allowed to exercise and perform brisk physical activities(Reference Schmid, Hallschmid and Jauch-Chara68). Under normal living conditions (not laboratory) and ad libitum eating and free exercise, a moderated sleep curtailment may not largely affect physical activity(Reference Porter, Pettit-Mee and Emerson133,Reference Bosy-Westphal, Hinrichs and Jauch-Chara134) . The time of sleep restriction also seems to matter. For instance, in a trial on normal-weight men sleep restriction during the second half of night reduced physical activity more than sleep restriction during the first half(Reference Wilms, Kuhr and Chamorro135), indicating that sleep during the second half of the night has a more important effect on feeling freshness during the next day. In agreement, McNeil et al. reported that participants with delayed sleep had more moderate-intensity physical activity than those with advanced wakeup time, although vigorous-intensity physical activity was greater following advanced wakeup(Reference McNeil, Doucet and Brunet131).

Energy expenditure

Like physical activity, the effect of sleep deprivation on daily energy expenditure has not been completely elucidated and all modes of increased(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66,Reference Shechter, Rising, Albu and St-Onge136Reference Jung, Melanson, Frydendall, Perreault, Eckel and Wright138) , decreased(Reference Benedict, Hallschmid and Lassen139) and no effect(Reference Covassin, Singh and McCrady-Spitzer50,Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference Hibi, Kubota and Mizuno140Reference Calvin, Carter and Adachi142) have been reported. There are many reasons for the controversies such as the difference in sleep deprivation protocol, very low sample size of some trials, the type of activities that were allowed during waking hours, and age, sex and BMI of participants. Studies largely differ in sleep deprivation protocol, such as total v. partial, moderate v. severe, and acute v. chronic sleep deprivation as well as the time of sleep deprivation. For the last item, depriving from deep or slow-wave sleep may have a different impact on energy expenditure than depriving from REM(Reference Kayaba, Park and Iwayama143).

A positive effect of sleep deprivation on energy expenditure was mostly seen in investigations where subjects had to spend at least 3 d inside calorimetric units. Staying for several days inside these units may cause a sort of psychological stress in participants because of limitations that such units impose, for instance, in eating, exercise, daily routine activities and communication with people as desired(Reference Vitale and Smith144). Such kind of stress may have also occurred by prolonged (40 h) sleep deprivation(Reference Jung, Melanson, Frydendall, Perreault, Eckel and Wright138). Stressful conditions may increase cortisol and/or norepinephrine levels, augment heart rate(Reference Benedict, Hallschmid and Lassen139) and subsequently increase metabolic rate(Reference Haase, Long and Gillooly145). However, investigations that give participants some sort of freedom for eating, physical activity, and doing daily activities did not find a significant increase in energy expenditure(Reference Covassin, Singh and McCrady-Spitzer50,Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference St-Onge, Roberts and Chen141,Reference Calvin, Carter and Adachi142) . The extent of the increase is estimated to be ∼ 4–5% equal to ∼ 377–460 kJ/day(Reference St-Onge and Shechter146). This amount is not large enough to compensate the increased energy intake during waking hours. For this increased energy expenditure, carbohydrates have shown to be oxidised more than fats, thereby promoting fat accumulation and weight gain(Reference Hursel, Rutters, Gonnissen, Martens and Westerterp-Plantenga130,Reference de Jonge, Zhao and Mattingly147) .

Basal metabolic rate

Few studies have examined the association of sleep deprivation with metabolic rate, and again findings are with controversy. In a cross-sectional study on obese individuals with short sleep duration, poor sleep quality was associated with increased resting energy expenditure(Reference de Jonge, Zhao and Mattingly147). The authors reasoned that secretion of cortisol and epinephrine during short sleep stress may be the cause of this increase. In contrast, in a cross-sectional study on children and adolescents, suboptimal sleep was associated with lower basal metabolic rate and physical activity and higher sedentary behaviours(Reference Jindal, Puyau, Adolph, Butte, Musaad and Bacha129). There are also cross-sectional studies that did not find a relationship between sleep deprivation and basal metabolic rate(Reference Jurado-Fasoli, Mochon-Benguigui, Castillo and Amaro-Gahete148,Reference Shechter, Rising, Wolfe, Albu and St-Onge149) . On the other hand, two RCT reported decreased metabolic rate following sleep deprivation. In one study, Nedeltcheva et al. reported that energy restriction with 5·5 h sleep/night for 14 d decreased resting metabolic rate and 24 h plasma epinephrine concentrations compared with energetic restriction with 8·5 h sleep/night(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Schoeller and Penev54). In agreement, Spaeth et al. reported that 4 h sleep/night for five nights reduced resting metabolic rate and 12 h recovery sleep returned it to baseline levels(Reference Spaeth, Dinges and Goel150).

According to what was stated in this and the previous two sections, there is still much work to be done to complete our understanding of the effect of sleep deprivation on physical activity, total and resting energy expenditure and thermic effect of food; for the latter, evidence is scarce.

Sex-specific effects

There is a large difference in the sleep and metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation between men and women. A global sleep dataset of 11·14 million nights from 69 650 non-shift workers aged 19–67 years from forty-seven countries showed that men tended to sleep less than women across the lifespan, but nighttime awakening was more prevalent in women, which was coincided with early to middle adulthood, i.e. a life stage associated with child-rearing(Reference Jonasdottir, Minor and Lehmann151). The prevalence of evening chronotype is higher in men than women, meaning that men generally wake up later in the morning and go to sleep later at night than women(Reference Adan and Natale152). This could be because the timing of the circadian rhythm of core body temperature and pineal melatonin secretion occurs earlier in women than men(Reference Cain, Dennison and Zeitzer153). A study of intrinsic circadian rhythm in a group of men and women aged 18–74 years revealed that the intrinsic circadian period is shorter in women (24 h 5 min in women v. 24 h 11 min in men) and that a greater proportion of women have intrinsic circadian periods shorter than 24·0 h (35% v. 14%)(Reference Duffy, Cain and Chang154).

Hormonal regulation of appetite in sleep restriction may also differ between sexes. Nymo et al. reported that while in men shorter habitual sleep duration and worse sleep quality was associated with increased basal and postprandial ghrelin, in women lower sleep quality was associated with decreased postprandial ghrelin(Reference Nymo, Kleppe, Coutinho, Rehfeld, Kulseng and Martins87). Also, in a study by St-Onge et al., sleep deprivation led to an increase in ghrelin in men and a decrease in glucagon-like peptide-1 in women without significant change in leptin levels(Reference St-Onge, O’Keeffe, Roberts, RoyChoudhury and Laferrère155). In the same line, a cohort study on 655 children showed an association between chronic curtailed sleep and lower leptin levels at age 7 years in girls, but not boys(Reference Boeke, Storfer-Isser, Redline and Taveras91). In contrast, in another cohort of 502 adolescents shorter sleep was associated with lower leptin in males(Reference Boeke, Storfer-Isser, Redline and Taveras91). Although somehow contradictory, the current findings mostly support the notion that the increased food consumption after sleep restriction may be caused by increased feeling of hunger in men and weakened satiety in women. Circadian misalignment has proved to reduce fullness in women and increase cravings for energy-dense and savory foods in men, suggesting that men may be more sensitive to hedonic regulation of food intake(Reference Qian, Morris, Caputo, Wang, Garaulet and Scheer156). This diverse and sex-specific hormonal response to sleep restriction may partly justify the lack of sleep deprivation effect on appetite hormones in studies that have not separated their results by sex.

Men and women also show a different weight response to sleep deprivation; i.e. men appear more susceptibility to weight gain than women. For instance, in RCT with sleep restriction for five nights men gained more weight than females(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66,Reference Spaeth, Dinges and Goel157) . Also, a cross-sectional study in elderly aged 67–96 years showed that the risk of increased BMI in sleep durations <5 h was greater in men than women(Reference Patel, Blackwell and Redline158). Cross-sectional investigations on children and adolescents have also shown an inverse correlation between sleep duration and BMI in boys but not girls(Reference Larsen, Sleddens, Vink, van den Broek and Kremers159,Reference Skidmore, Howe and Polak160) . The reason of this sex-specific effect of sleep deprivation on weight is not clear but hormonal fluctuations in women(Reference Herzog, Jauch-Chara, Oltmanns and Benedict161) and the dominant hedonic aspects of food selection in men(Reference Qian, Morris, Caputo, Wang, Garaulet and Scheer156) may partially explain it. For the latter, a large cross-sectional study on school-aged children in China showed that short sleep duration was associated with increased risk of more sugar-sweetened beverage intake among younger children and boys and less vegetable and fruit intake among older children and girls(Reference Cao, Zhu, Sun, Luo and Jing162). Also, in a trial after chronic sleep restriction, men showed a trend towards decreased implicit positive attitude for low-energy foods whereas women did not show a significant change(Reference Alkozei, Killgore and Smith163). A summary of the sex-specific effect of sleep deprivation is depicted in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4. The effect of sleep deprivation on males and females. GLP-1, glucagon like peptide-1.

Influence of weight status

Neural response to sleep deprivation depends on weight status. Jensen et al. found that in sleep restriction, both normal-weight and overweight/obese adolescents showed higher activity in reward regions irrespective of their weight status(Reference Jensen, Duraccio and Barnett164). However, while adolescents with normal-weight demonstrated concomitant increases in neural activity in brain regions associated with food reward and inhibitory control, adolescents who were overweight/obese showed only increases in brain areas related to food reward without a change in inhibition-related brain processing(Reference Jensen, Duraccio and Barnett164). This suggests that in sleep restriction, individuals who are overweight/obese may be at a higher risk of food overconsumption compared with their normal-weight counterparts. Interestingly, in sufficient sleep conditions, adolescents with overweight/obesity had higher level of activity in inhibition-related regions than their normal-weight counterparts but in sleep restriction they showed a considerable decrease in neural activity of such regions(Reference Jensen, Duraccio and Barnett164). Contrary to the results observed by Jensen et al. (Reference Jensen, Duraccio and Barnett164), Duraccio and colleagues(Reference Duraccio, Zaugg and Jensen165) found that adolescents with normal weight demonstrated increased perception of food appeal following sleep restriction but adolescents with overweight/obesity consistently indicated high scores of food reward, which did not differ between habitual and restricted sleep conditions(Reference Duraccio, Zaugg and Jensen165). However, both groups showed poor scores of food-related inhibitory control task following sleep restriction(Reference Duraccio, Zaugg and Jensen165). These results suggest that sleep restriction increases the risk of weight gain in both normal-weight and overweight/obese adolescents but adolescents who were overweight/obese are at greater risk. A summary of the possible influence of weight status in homoeostatic regulation of appetite has been depicted in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5. Differential response to sleep restriction according to body weight. Both normal-weight individuals and individuals who are overweight/obese are assumed to stimulate the food reward system more strongly during sleep deprivation, but individuals who are overweight/obese indicate a weaker inhibitory control over eating compared with normal-weight individuals.

Impact of sleep deprivation on food selection

As stated above, a part of eating behaviours following sleep restriction is driven by reward-driven food intake, which results in “eating in the absence of hunger”. Available evidence indicates that contribution of this reward-driven eating in the increased food intake is larger than hormonal factors even in adequate sleep but more importantly in sleep deprivation(Reference Rihm, Menz and Schultz99,Reference Benedict, Brooks and O’Daly118) , reviewed in Alonso-Alonso et al.(Reference Alonso-Alonso, Woods and Pelchat166). In this regard, a systematic review showed that insufficient sleep leads to increments in energy and fat intake, body weight, appetite, hunger, eating occasions and portion size without affecting leptin, ghrelin and cortisol concentrations, suggesting the dominance of non-homoeostatic mechanisms over the hormonal or homeostatic factors(Reference Soltanieh, Solgi, Ansari, Santos and Abbasi61). Also, in a cross-sectional study on normal-weight young women, scores of emotional eating (i.e. eating in response to aroused emotional states) and external eating (i.e. eating in the presence of food irrespective of hunger) were higher in women who had poor sleep quality (but not short sleep duration)(Reference Dweck, Jenkins and Nolan167). Upon administration of snacks, participants who scored high on emotional eating and reported short sleep consumed higher amounts of snacks. Similarly, an RCT on normal-weight young men showed that in sated state (i.e. following breakfast), men who experienced one night total sleep deprivation chose larger snack portions, further indicating the predominance of reward-driven eating over homoeostatic signals(Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Nilsson168). Also, five nights of sleep restriction (6·5 h sleep/night) in adolescents did not affect their self-reported hunger or desire for non-sweet foods but pictures of sweet/dessert foods were more appealing for them(Reference Simon, Field, Miller, DiFrancesco and Beebe169).

Sleep deprived individuals usually look for and consume palatable foods, foods that are mostly unhealthy, i.e. energy-dense with high fat and/or high sugar content(Reference Kant and Graubard70,Reference Khan and Uddin170Reference Cerolini, Rodgers and Lombardo173) . Craving for unhealthy foods may be due to alteration in taste function including sweet, salt and even umami or sour tastes(Reference Lv, Finlayson and Dando174). Extended use of such unhealthy foods may disrupt appetite regulation by inducing addiction-like deficits in brain regions involved in the reward system, including the lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, ventral tegmental area, prefrontal cortex and amygdala(Reference Alonso-Alonso, Woods and Pelchat166,Reference de Macedo, de Freitas and da Silva Torres175) . In an RCT in normal-weight individuals, after five nights sleep restriction with 4 h sleep/night, greater activation in parts of the brain reward system was seen upon viewing unhealthy foods (pepperoni pizza, doughnuts, chocolate bars and candy) compared with healthy foods (carrots, yogurt, oatmeal and grapes) whereas such increased activity did not occur after normal sleep(Reference St-Onge, Wolfe, Sy, Shechter and Hirsch176). In another RCT, the increased brain connectivity from the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex to the bilateral anterior insula after one night total sleep deprivation correlated with increased fat and decreased carbohydrate intake in healthy adults(Reference Fang, Spaeth and Ma177). A summary of the effect of sleep deprivation on food selection and eating behaviours is presented in Fig. 6.

Fig. 6. Effect of sleep deprivation on eating behaviours and food selection.

Effect of sleep deprivation on diet quality

Compelling evidence from cross-sectional investigations indicates an inverse relationship between sleep deprivation and diet quality. This inverse association has been reported, for instance, in adolescents aged 10–16 years(Reference Kracht, Chaput, Martin, Champagne, Katzmarzyk and Staiano178), postmenopausal women(Reference Stern, Grant and Thomson64) and women with diverse age, race, and ethnicity(Reference Zuraikat, Makarem, Liao, St-Onge and Aggarwal62) although postmenopausal women with ≥8 h sleep have also shown poor diet quality(Reference Stern, Grant and Thomson64). Furthermore, social jetlag, which is the difference in the average sleep duration between weekdays and weekends, has been associated with negative food choices even in cases of adequate weekly sleep duration(Reference Al Khatib, Dikariyanto, Bermingham, Gibson and Hall179). Interestingly, weekend catch-up sleep did not prevent the adverse effect of short weekday sleep on unhealthy dietary choices(Reference Al Khatib, Dikariyanto, Bermingham, Gibson and Hall179,Reference LeMay-Russell, Tanofsky-Kraff and Schvey180) .

Sleep deprivation usually induces cravings for consumption of carbohydrates/starches, fast foods and sweets(Reference Li, Li and Zhou52). A cross-sectional study on resident physicians indicated that additional work was associated with higher intake of cereals, bread and pasta, and lower intake of fruits and beans, along with lower score of adapted healthy eating index(Reference Mota, Waterhouse and De-Souza51). Consistently, in motor freight workers, adequate sleep was associated with more healthful food choices(Reference Buxton, Quintiliani and Yang181). In agreement, a systematic review of interventions that used sleep extension as a strategy to improve cardiometabolic health suggested that sleep extension may be associated with reductions in overall appetite, desire for sweets and salty foods, and increased percentage of daily energy intake from protein(Reference Henst, Pienaar, Roden and Rae182). Although there are RCT denying the selection of high v. low energy foods under sleep deprivation states(Reference Chapman, Nilsson and Nilsson86,Reference Duraccio, Krietsch and Zhang183) , most of trials agree with the effect of insufficient sleep on activation of the reward system and selection of high energy foods (Reference Li, Li and Zhou52,Reference Spaeth, Dinges and Goel157, Reference Dweck, Jenkins and Nolan167,Reference Simon, Field, Miller, DiFrancesco and Beebe169,Reference St-Onge, Wolfe, Sy, Shechter and Hirsch176,Reference Fang, Spaeth and Ma177,Reference Brunet, McNeil, Jaeger Hintze, Doucet and Forest184) . Unhealthy habits such as skipping breakfast(Reference Tambalis, Panagiotakos, Psarra and Sidossis171), skipping main meals(Reference Kant and Graubard70), and higher ingestion of snacks(Reference Kant and Graubard70) have been reported in individuals with insufficient sleep.

Sleep insufficiency has also been associated with low diet quality in children and adolescents. Lower rate of fruit and vegetable consumption has been reported in sleep-deprived school-aged children in China(Reference Cao, Zhu, Sun, Luo and Jing162), children and adolescents of Australia(Reference Golley, Maher, Matricciani and Olds185), Spanish children(Reference Pérez-Farinós, Villar-Villalba and López Sobaler186) and US adolescents(Reference Kruger, Reither, Peppard, Krueger and Hale172). In the same line, an analysis of data from 10 453 children from five European countries revealed that one additional hour of sleep was associated with increased consumption of fruits and vegetables(Reference Börnhorst, Wijnhoven and Kunešová187). Also, in a group of Danish 2–6-year-old children, those with more variable sleep duration consumed less fruit and vegetables and a higher amounts of added sugars and sugary beverages(Reference Rangan, Zheng, Olsen, Rohde and Heitmann63). Also, in a cross-sectional study on 118 462 Korean adolescents, short sleep duration of <6 h was associated with higher intake of soft drinks and confectionaries, and poor sleep quality with 7–8 h sleep was associated with lower intake of fruits, vegetables and milk, and higher intake of soda, soft drinks, fast-food, instant noodle and confectionaries(Reference Min, Kim and Park188). Given the importance of childhood habits in the formation of adulthood eating habits, results of these investigations remind the importance of childhood sleep in adulthood health status. Sleeping at earlier bedtimes may result in healthier eating behaviours such as eating low glycemic index foods, fruits and dairy in adolescents(Reference Asarnow, Greer, Walker and Harvey189).

Desire for consumption of snacks

Both epidemiological and interventional investigations have shown increased consumption of snacks in sleep deprivation conditions. A large cross-sectional study showed that adults with habitual short sleep duration reported a lower mean percentage of energy intake from main meals but higher energy from snacks(Reference Kant and Graubard70). Likewise, a survey on high school students revealed an association between shorter sleep and more frequent cracker consumption(Reference Lee132). In another cross-sectional study, university students with habitual short sleep ate more often, i.e. more than three meals per day and more frequent snacking, than those with long and sufficient sleep(Reference Hicks, McTighe and Juarez190). RCT have indicated the same line of evidence. For instance, two groups of investigators reported that during sleep curtailment in healthy volunteers, meal intake did not change but the amount or energy from snacks increased(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Nilsson168) . These findings have been confirmed by other investigators(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference Cerolini, Rodgers and Lombardo173,Reference Weiss, Xu, Storfer-Isser, Thomas, Ievers-Landis and Redline191) .

Craving for sweetness

Increased consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages and snacks after sleep deprivation may result from the preference for sweet taste under such conditions. Cross-sectional investigations have shown inverse associations between sugar intake and sleep duration(Reference Kant and Graubard70,Reference Kjeldsen, Hjorth and Andersen192) , and an inverse association between sleep efficiency and sweet cravings(Reference Kracht, Chaput, Martin, Champagne, Katzmarzyk and Staiano178). RCT have also reported sweet craving in normal-weight young adults after three nights of sleep restriction (5 h sleep/night)(Reference Tajiri, Yoshimura, Hatamoto, Shiratsuchi, Tanaka and Shimoda193) or non-obese adults after one night of sleep curtailment with 33% reduction in habitual sleep duration(Reference Szczygiel, Cho and Tucker194). But, such alteration in sweet taste pleasantness or intensity was not observed after one night total sleep deprivation although feeling of hunger and ghrelin concentration increased(Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Chapman75). It is likely that stress caused by total sleep deprivation decreases sensitivity to sweet taste(Reference Hogenkamp, Nilsson and Chapman75) as animals with mild stress have shown attenuated sensitivity to food rewards(Reference Papp, Willner and Muscat195).

Sweet liking leads to increased consumption of energy from sugar-sweetened snacks and beverages by sleep-deprived individuals(Reference Broussard, Kilkus and Delebecque95,Reference Yang, Schnepp and Tucker196) , augments glycaemic load of the consumed diets(Reference Beebe, Simon, Summer, Hemmer, Strotman and Dolan197,Reference Duraccio, Whitacre and Krietsch198) , and enhances risk of obesity(Reference Hajhashemy, Mirzaei, Asadi, Akhlaghi and Saneei199). Moreover, sweetness sensitivity is associated with higher preference for sweets and other carbohydrate-dominated foods but lower preference for foods with high content of protein(Reference Han, Mohebbi, Seo and Hummel200). The low preference for protein is particularly important because proteins alter appetite regulating hormones in a direction that leads to increase satiety and decreased hunger(Reference Kohanmoo, Faghih and Akhlaghi201). Sweet sensitivity may also bring along a higher interest in high-fat food odours such as chocolate and peanuts(Reference Han, Mohebbi, Seo and Hummel200), consumption of which may further intensify risk of weight gain.

Appealing macronutrients

The extra energy consumed during sleep deprivation is provided from carbohydrate and fat rather than protein(Reference Tasali, Chapotot, Wroblewski and Schoeller76). Numerous cross-sectional and interventional investigations have shown increased fat(Reference St-Onge, Roberts and Chen141,Reference Fang, Spaeth and Ma177,Reference Weiss, Xu, Storfer-Isser, Thomas, Ievers-Landis and Redline191,Reference Saleh-Ghadimi, Dehghan, Abbasalizad Farhangi, Asghari-Jafarabadi and Jafari-Vayghan202Reference McNeil, Forest and Hintze204) and carbohydrate(Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference Mi, Kelly and Brychta205) intake in sleep-deprived individuals. Consistently, increased sleep duration and quality following an exercise regimen in adolescent girls with obesity led to significant decreases in food energy density, fat, and sugar consumption(Reference Saidi, Rochette, Bovet, Merlin and Duché206). Likewise, transitioning from an insufficient to adequate/recovery sleep schedule decreased energy intake, especially that from fats and carbohydrates(Reference Markwald, Melanson and Smith66). However, there are also documents indicating the increase in fat intake and a decrease in carbohydrate consumption(Reference Fang, Spaeth and Ma177,Reference Weiss, Xu, Storfer-Isser, Thomas, Ievers-Landis and Redline191) , suggesting that sleep deprivation may cause a greater desire for consumption of fats compared with non-sweet carbohydrates. In this regard, a cross-sectional study on adults with obesity showed associations between sleep apnea, as a sleep disorder, and a shift from carbohydrate to fat intake(Reference Galli, Piaggi and Mattingly207). Accordingly, a systematic review of RCT showed increased consumption of fat without a change in protein and carbohydrate intake in sleep deprived conditions(Reference Soltanieh, Solgi, Ansari, Santos and Abbasi61). It seems that the type of fat is also important. A meta-analysis of nine cohort studies with 14 906 participants of European descent showed associations between sleep duration and lower saturated fatty acid intake in adults aged 20–64 years old and higher total fat and polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA), as well as lower carbohydrate intake in women aged 65–80 years(Reference Dashti, Follis and Smith208). Similarly, a cross-sectional study on Mexican American children showed associations between longer sleep duration and higher percentage of energy obtained from fats, especially from PUFA(Reference Martinez, Tschann and Butte209). On the contrary, in an RCT on adults, sleep deprivation (4 h sleep for five nights) caused increases in the consumption of fats, notably saturated fats(Reference St-Onge, Roberts and Chen141). Based on these reports, in sleep deprivation, individuals crave foods high in fat, particularly saturated fats, while in sufficient sleep consumption of food sources of saturated fats seems less appealing.

Time of eating

Although, short-duration sleepers are reported to eat from the earliest time in the morning to the latest time of night(Reference Kant and Graubard70), extra energy is mostly consumed during evening hours(Reference McHill and Wright31,Reference Herzog, Friedrich and Fujita35,Reference Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Kasza, Schoeller and Penev97,Reference Spaeth, Dinges and Goel157,Reference Duraccio, Whitacre and Krietsch198,Reference Spaeth, Goel and Dinges210) . For instance, adolescents who experienced five nights with 6·5 h sleep indicated increases in the intake of energy, fat and carbohydrates after 9 pm but not earlier in the afternoon or morning(Reference Duraccio, Whitacre and Krietsch198). Also, participants who experienced 4 h sleep/night for 5 d consumed additional energy between 10 pm and 4 am and consumed less energy the next morning(Reference Spaeth, Dinges and Goel157). Similarly, participants who had 5 h sleep/night for 5 d consumed a smaller breakfast but 42% more energy after dinner as snacks(Reference McHill and Wright31). Interestingly, the energy consumed after dinner have been more than that consumed at individual meals(Reference McHill and Wright31,Reference Spaeth, Goel and Dinges210) . The cause of tendency to late-night food intake is not known, but it is likely that changes in circadian sleep and awakening time affect eating patterns. For instance, the reduced desire to consume breakfast may be due to short sleep and waking up at a time when the internal circadian clock is still promoting sleep. Also, staying awake during the night may stimulate food consumption because melatonin levels are high during the night and promote sleep, and the pleasure of eating helps to better tolerate a stressful insomnia condition(Reference McHill and Wright31). The additional energy consumed during late-night waking hours compensates for the extra energy needed to stay awake for the extra hours(Reference Spaeth, Goel and Dinges210). However, this additional energy may exceed energy needs and thus contribute to weight gain and obesity. In this regard, night eating syndrome is more frequently seen in individuals with obesity(Reference McCuen-Wurst, Ruggieri and Allison211,Reference Sutcu, Pamuk and Ongel212) . Animal studies have shown that when mice eat food at times when they normally sleep, they gain weight even though they have their regular energy intake(Reference Fonken, Workman, Walton, Weil, Morris, Haim and Nelson213).

The effect of diet on sleep

In previous sections the effect of sleep on dietary intakes was discussed but the literature also contains documents claiming the influence of diet and eating behaviours on sleep health.

Eating habits

A number of investigations have claimed adverse effect of eating habits on sleep. A cross-sectional study on healthy adults indicated that food intake near the sleeping time was associated with poor sleep quality(Reference Crispim, Zimberg, dos Reis, Diniz, Tufik and de Mello214). Also, Gwin and Leidy reported that when compared with skipping breakfast, consuming breakfast resulted in shorter total sleep time but tended to improve perceived sleep quality and sleep onset(Reference Gwin and Leidy215). Likewise, a survey on grade 5 children in Canada, unhealthy eating habits (e.g. eating fried foods, eating fast food, eating supper alone, eating supper in front of the TV and eating ready-made dinners) were inversely associated with sleep duration and quality(Reference Khan, Faught, Chu, Ekwaru, Storey and Veugelers216). Snacking between meals and after supper was also negatively associated with sleep quality.

Diet quality

Epidemiological investigations have shown that consumption of unhealthy foods may predispose individuals to poor sleep quality. A population-based survey on Brazilian adults during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic showed that higher consumption of ultra-processed foods and lower consumption of fresh and minimally processed foods was associated with a higher chance of poor sleep quality(Reference Menezes-Júnior, Andrade and Coletro217). Another survey on adolescents of the same country revealed an association between consumption of ultra-processed foods and anxiety-induced sleep disturbance(Reference Werneck, Vancampfort, Oyeyemi, Stubbs and Silva218,Reference Werneck, Hoare and Silva219) . Also diets with low fibre and high saturated fats and sugar intake was associated with lighter and less restorative sleep with more arousals in normal-weight adults with habitual sleep(Reference St-Onge, Roberts, Shechter and Choudhury220). In contrast, in a sample of college students in the USA, higher frequency of healthy protein and healthy dairy food consumption reduced the odds of poor sleep quality, while higher consumption of empty calories predicted poor sleep quality(Reference Knowlden, Burns, Harcrow and Shewmake221). Similarly, in a sample of Japanese college students, the likelihood of poor sleep quality for individuals in the middle and highest tertile of the Food Guide (i.e. well-balanced dietary intakes of meats, fish, eggs, soy products, dairy and fruits) was half of that in the lowest tertile(Reference Yamamoto, Ota and Minematsu222). Fish, eggs, nuts, seeds, and to a lesser extent cereals, legumes, seeds and a number of fruits such as kiwi fruit and tart cherries are food sources of melatonin and on consumption can increase serum melatonin levels(Reference Meng, Li and Li223,Reference Castellucci, Barrea and Laudisio224) .

Consistent with the positive effect of diet quality on sleep, a large body of evidence indicates adverse effect of food insecurity on sleep outcomes, e.g.(Reference Wang225Reference Liu, Njai, Greenlund, Chapman and Croft229). For instance, a population-based sample of 10 901 US adults, marginally food secure, low food secure and very low food secure participants, were more likely to report sleep complaints than their fully food secure counterparts(Reference Ding, Keiley, Garza, Duffy and Zizza230). Also, a meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies showed associations between food insecurity and increased risk of sleep disorders(Reference Arenas, Thomas, Wang and DeLisser231). Food insecurity has also been associated with trouble falling and staying asleep(Reference Nagata, Palar and Gooding232). Sleep quality and quantity may mediate the relationship between food insecurity and obesity(Reference Narcisse, Long, Felix, Rowland, Bursac and McElfish233).

Dietary carbohydrates

Investigations claiming the effect of carbohydrates on diet are contradictory and some are with cross-sectional design and thus cannot conclusively indicate the cause and effect relationship. A cross-sectional study on 9239 Chinese adults showed associations between higher intake of carbohydrate and longer sleep duration(Reference Liu, Wu and Qi234). In agreement, in a study on 4435 Japanese adults, low carbohydrate intake (<50% v. ≥50% of total energy) was associated with difficulty in maintaining sleep(Reference Tanaka, Yatsuya and Uemura235). Also, consumption of a high-glycaemic index meals shortened sleep onset latency in healthy sleepers compared with a low-glycaemic index meal although it was less effective when consumed 1 h before sleep(Reference Afaghi, O’Connor and Chow236).

Nonetheless, there are investigations from both RCT and cross-sectional type that have reported benefits of a low carbohydrate diet on sleep. For instance, two cross-sectional investigations demonstrated that low carbohydrate diet was associated with better sleep quality in Iranian women(Reference Tavakoli, Mirzababaei and Mirzaei237,Reference Daneshzad, Keshavarz, Qorbani, Larijani and Azadbakht238) . Also, a systematic review of RCTs showed that lower carbohydrate intakes lengthen the proportion and duration of N3 stage sleep (referred to as slow wave sleep or deep sleep), while higher carbohydrate ingestions prolong REM stage sleep(Reference Vlahoyiannis, Giannaki, Sakkas, Aphamis and Andreou239). Surveys also point to an adverse effect of simple carbohydrates on sleep quality. A survey on Korean adolescents showed a direct relationship of energy drink intake with sleep dissatisfaction and feeling of fatigue(Reference Park, Lee and Lee240). Likewise, a survey on 100 648 Brazilian adolescents suggested that ingestion of sweets and other confectionaries and soda drinks increases the risk of anxiety-induced sleep disturbance(Reference Werneck, Vancampfort, Oyeyemi, Stubbs and Silva218).

The controversies may be resolved by considering the type of carbohydrates; benefits of carbohydrates are seen with complex and low-glycaemic index carbohydrates while adverse effects are more related to simple and high-glyacemic index carbohydrates. In this context, cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses of a prospective cohort study with 77 860 post-menopausal women showed that higher dietary glycaemic index (dietary added sugars, starch and refined grains) was associated with increasing odds of prevalent and incident insomnia. By contrast, higher intakes of dietary fiber, whole grains, non-juice fruit and vegetables were associated with lower odds of prevalent insomnia(Reference Gangwisch, Hale and St-Onge241). High dietary glycaemic load triggers hyperinsulinaemia and subsequently hypoglycaemia, resulting in secretion of counter-regulatory hormones such as adrenaline and cortisol which cause symptoms like heart palpitations, tremors and irritability that lead to awakening from sleep(Reference Gangwisch, Hale and St-Onge241).

Dietary fats

In agreement with studies that have shown benefits of low carbohydrate diet on sleep quality, Bennett et al. reported that higher intake of mono-unsaturated fats if substituted for carbohydrate may protect against poor sleep quality in pregnant women(Reference Bennett, Cain and Blumfield242). However, Cao et al. in a cohort study with five follow-ups on Chinese adults reported that association between fat intake and sleep depends on the time of fat intake; high-fat breakfast was associated with less daytime sleep, which may mean feeling more refreshed during the day, but a high-fat dinner was associated with persistent short sleep(Reference Cao, Taylor, Pan, Adams, Appleton and Shi243).

Dietary protein

There is evidence of a protective effect of dietary protein on sleep. A cross-sectional study in Japanese adults indicated that regular exercise combined with high protein intake contributes to good sleep quality(Reference Suzuki, Morita and Miyagi244). Likewise, in an RCT on adults who are overweight and obese, a greater proportion of protein in an energy-restricted diet improved sleep quality(Reference Zhou, Kim, Armstrong, Chen and Campbell245). In agreement, a cross-sectional analysis of 4435 non-shift workers in Japan showed that low-protein intake (<16% v. ≥16% of total energy) was associated with difficulty in initiating sleep and poor sleep quality, although high-protein intake (≥19% v. <19% of total energy) was associated with difficulty in maintaining sleep(Reference Tanaka, Yatsuya and Uemura235).

Other effective components

Numerous components in healthy diets may render sleep-promoting properties. An RCT on adults with obesity demonstrated that adults who are obese and poor sleepers may become good sleepers when they consume either the recommended or a higher protein energy-restricted healthy eating pattern(Reference Hudson, Zhou and Campbell246). Animal and plant protein in healthy diets provides tryptophan which is an essential amino acid and the precursor of serotonin and melatonin, both are known to play roles in sleep-wake cycle(Reference Bhat, Pires, Tan, Babu Chidambaram and Guillemin247). Evening consumption of tryptophan caused a 130% increase in plasma tryptophan to large neutral amino acids ratio and reduced sleepiness and improved brain-sustained attention processes the following morning(Reference Markus, Jonkman, Lammers, Deutz, Messer and Rigtering248). In agreement, a recent meta-analysis of 18 RCT showed that tryptophan supplementation, especially at ≥1 g, can help improve sleep quality(Reference Sutanto, Loh, Toh, Lee and Kim249).

Food sources of protein are good sources of zinc. Numerous studies in both cross-sectional and interventional designs have indicated the effect of zinc on improving the duration and quality of sleep and possible mechanisms of action have been suggested(Reference Cherasse and Urade250). Fruit and vegetables provide great quantities of magnesium. Observational studies have suggested an association between higher magnesium intake and sleep quality and RCT have shown a positive effect of magnesium supplementation on sleep quality, sleep efficiency and sleep time(Reference Arab, Rafie, Amani and Shirani251). Fibre also reduces stage 1 (the first 1–5 min of sleep) and increases slow wave sleep (deep sleep) in individuals with habitually normal sleep(Reference St-Onge, Roberts, Shechter and Choudhury220). Trials have also documented benefits of pro-, pre- and post-biotics on sleep length and quality(Reference Haarhuis, Kardinaal and Kortman252). Animal studies have shown that short-chain fatty acids, particularly butyrate, promote sleep(Reference Szentirmai, Millican, Massie and Kapás253).

In national surveys in the USA, short (<6 h) and long (>9 h) sleepers indicated lower variety in their diet compared to normal sleepers(Reference Grandner, Jackson, Gerstner and Knutson254). Also, short sleepers consumed less vitamin C, lutein, zeaxanthin and selenium(Reference Grandner, Jackson, Gerstner and Knutson254). Whether low consumption of antioxidants impairs sleep is not known but sleep deprivation has been associated with increased oxidative stress and reduced serum antioxidant capacity(Reference Atrooz and Salim255,Reference Jówko, Różański and Tomczak256) and sleep is supposed to restore antioxidant potential of the body(Reference Jówko, Różański and Tomczak256,Reference Reimund257) . Cross-sectional investigations have also shown inverse associations between serum folate, vitamin D and sleep disturbance and a positive association between total carotenoids concentrations and short sleep duration, although an inverse association was also observed between serum vitamin B12 levels and sleep duration(Reference Beydoun, Gamaldo and Canas258). Regarding vitamin B12, a cross-sectional study on Chinese patients with type 2 diabetes showed associations between elevated serum vitamin B12 level and incidence of insomnia(Reference Xiong, Liu, Yao, Zhang and Ge259). However, evidence also suggests improvement in daytime alertness, concentration and in shortening and improving sleep following vitamin B12 supplementation, likely through its effect on melatonin(Reference Mayer, Kröger and Meier-Ewert260). The literature for the effects of nutrients on sleep is largely incomplete and somehow contradictory, and many more investigations aimed at exploring the potential of food components on sleep duration and quality should be conducted in the future. A brief overview of the current findings is given in Fig. 7.

Fig. 7. Nutrients with possible influence on sleep health. GI, glycemic index; PUFA, polyunsaturated fatty acids; SFA, saturated fatty acids; Trp, tryptophan.

Conclusions

Sleep deprivation, which is caused by reduction in the quantity or quality of sleep, disrupts hormonal and neural regulation of appetite, leading to increased sensation of hunger, decreased satiety and intensification of the desire to eat food even in the absence of hunger. Feeling less refreshed during the day may end in lowering physical activity and a willingness to perform sedentary activities, the latter is generally accompanied with consuming high-energy snacks, further increasing obesity risk. Living in an obesogenic environment with plenty of palatable energy-dense foods available, having less time and motivation for doing regular exercise, and being exposed to mental pressures such as stress and depression all act in a consortium with sleep deprivation to push individuals towards obesity. Therefore, sufficient and good quality sleep is necessary for obesity prevention and should be a part of any weight-loss programme. Little data on weekend catch-up sleep have not yielded promising results, but more research should focus on this topic. Whether daytime sleep can compensate nocturnal short sleep also remains to be determined in future. As discussed during the previous sections, much still needs to be done to complete our knowledge of the consequences of different intensities and duration of sleep deprivation on appetite and desire to eat, energy intake and expenditure and physical and sedentary activities, and about the involved mechanisms. The current literature shows an interaction between sleep deprivation and diet quality, which is also largely unknown and is the subject of ongoing research. Moreover, current data have mainly been obtained from cross-sectional investigations and cannot suggest causal relationships. Well-designed RCT with longer periods of sleep deprivation while considering age, sex and weight status effects are needed.

Financial support

None.

Competing interests

None.

Authorship

The idea of the work was from M.A., but A.A. contributed with his valuable search to find relevant citations. Both the authors collaborated in the preparation of the manuscript.

References

Stich, FM, Huwiler, S, D’Hulst, G & Lustenberger, C (2022) The potential role of sleep in promoting a healthy body composition: underlying mechanisms determining muscle, fat, and bone mass and their association with sleep. Neuroendocrinology 112, 673701. doi: 10.1159/000518691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luyster, FS, Strollo, PJ Jr, Zee, PC & Walsh, JK (2012) Boards of directors of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and the sleep research society. Sleep: a health imperative. Sleep 35, 727734. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Bon, O (2020) Relationships between REM and NREM in the NREM-REM sleep cycle: a review on competing concepts. Sleep Med 70, 616. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, AK, Reddy, V, Shumway, KR & Araujo, JF (2023) Physiology, Sleep Stages. 2022 Sep 7. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing; PMID: 30252388.Google Scholar
Armitage, R, Landis, C, Hoffmann, R, Lentz, M, Watson, NF, Goldberg, J & Buchwald, D (2007) The impact of a 4-hour sleep delay on slow wave activity in twins discordant for chronic fatigue syndrome. Sleep 30, 657662. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.5.657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilckens, KA, Ferrarelli, F, Walker, MP & Buysse, DJ (2018) Slow-wave activity enhancement to improve cognition. Trends Neurosci 41, 470482. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.03.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porkka-Heiskanen, T, Zitting, KM & Wigren, HK (2013) Sleep, its regulation and possible mechanisms of sleep disturbances. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 208, 311328. doi: 10.1111/apha.12134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borbély, AA, Daan, S, Wirz-Justice, A & Deboer, T (2016) The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res 25, 131143. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deboer, T (2018) Sleep homeostasis and the circadian clock: do the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat influence each other’s functioning? Neurobiol Sleep Circadian Rhythms 5, 6877. doi: 10.1016/j.nbscr.2018.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duhart, JM & Inami, S, Koh, K (2023) Many faces of sleep regulation: beyond the time of day and prior wake time. FEBS J 290, 931950. doi: 10.1111/febs.16320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pandi-Perumal, SR, Cardinali, DP, Zaki, NFW, Karthikeyan, R, Spence, DW, Reiter, RJ & Brown, GM (2022) Timing is everything: circadian rhythms and their role in the control of sleep. Front Neuroendocrinol 66, 100978. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.100978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Gall, C (2022) The effects of light and the circadian system on rhythmic brain function. Int J Mol Sci 23, 2778. doi: 10.3390/ijms23052778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirotsu, C, Tufik, S & Andersen, ML (2015) Interactions between sleep, stress, and metabolism: from physiological to pathological conditions. Sleep Sci 8, 143152. doi: 10.1016/j.slsci.2015.09.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zou, H, Zhou, H, Yan, R, Yao, Z & Lu, Q (2022) Chronotype, circadian rhythm, and psychiatric disorders: recent evidence and potential mechanisms. Front Neurosci 16, 811771. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.811771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randler, C & Engelke, J (2019) Gender differences in chronotype diminish with age: a meta-analysis based on morningness/chronotype questionnaires. Chronobiol Int 36, 888905. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2019.1585867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randler, C, Faßl, C & Kalb, N (2017) From lark to owl: developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from new-borns to early adulthood. Sci Rep 7, 45874. doi: 10.1038/srep45874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrams, RM (2015) Sleep deprivation. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am 42, 493506. doi: 10.1016/j.ogc.2015.05.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrivastava, D, Jung, S, Saadat, M, Sirohi, R & Crewson, K (2014) How to interpret the results of a sleep study. J Community Hosp Intern Med Perspect 4, 24983. doi: 10.3402/jchimp.v4.24983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, R, Somerville, G, Enros, P, Paquin, S, Kestler, M & Gillies-Poitras, E (2014) Sleep efficiency (but not sleep duration) of healthy school-age children is associated with grades in math and languages. Sleep Med 15, 15171525. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.08.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirshkowitz, M, Whiton, K, Albert, SM, et al. (2015) National sleep foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: final report. Sleep Health 1, 233243. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y, Wheaton, AG, Chapman, DP, Cunningham, TJ, Lu, H & Croft, JB (2016) Prevalence of healthy sleep duration among adults—United States, 2014. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 65,137141. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6506a1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huyett, P, Siegel, N & Bhattacharyya, N (2021) Prevalence of sleep disorders and association with mortality: results from the NHANES 2009–2010. Laryngoscope 131, 686689. doi: 10.1002/lary.28900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonelli, G, Marshall, NS, Grillakis, A, Miller, CB, Hoyos, CM & Glozier, N (2018) Sleep health epidemiology in low and middle-income countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence of poor sleep quality and sleep duration. Sleep Health 4, 239250. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2018.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amiri, S (2022) Sleep quality and sleep-related issues in industrial workers: a global meta-analysis. Int J Occup Saf Ergon 114. doi: 10.1080/10803548.2021.2024376.Google Scholar
Geiker, NRW, Astrup, A, Hjorth, MF, Sjödin, A, Pijls, L & Markus, CR (2018) Does stress influence sleep patterns, food intake, weight gain, abdominal obesity and weight loss interventions and vice versa? Obes Rev 19, 8197. doi: 10.1111/obr.12603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, Y, Laksono, I, Selvanathan, J, et al. (2021) Prevalence of sleep disturbances in patients with chronic non-cancer pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev 57, 101467. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavanagh, J, Jackson, DJ & Kent, BD (2018) Sleep and asthma. Curr Opin Pulm Med 24, 569573. doi: 10.1097/MCP.0000000000000526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Sleep Medicine and Research (2006) Sleep Disorders and Sleep Deprivation: An Unmet Public Health Problem. Colten, HR, Altevogt, BM, editors. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (US); PMID: 20669438.Google Scholar
Zimberg, IZ, Dâmaso, A, Del Re, M, et al. (2012) Short sleep duration and obesity: mechanisms and future perspectives. Cell Biochem Funct 30, 524529. doi: 10.1002/cbf.2832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHill, AW & Wright, KP Jr (2017) Role of sleep and circadian disruption on energy expenditure and in metabolic predisposition to human obesity and metabolic disease. Obes Rev 18(Suppl 1), 1524. doi: 10.1111/obr.12503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanches, I, Teixeira, F, dos Santos, JM & Ferreira, AJ (2015) Effects of acute sleep deprivation resulting from night shift work on young doctors. Acta Med Port 28, 457462. doi: 10.20344/amp.5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pappas, JA & Miner, B (2022) Sleep deficiency in the elderly. Clin Chest Med 43, 273286. doi: 10.1016/j.ccm.2022.02.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S, Deason, K, Rancourt, D & Gray, HL (2021) Disentangling the relationship between food insecurity and poor sleep health. Ecol Food Nutr 60, 580595. doi: 10.1080/03670244.2021.1926245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, N, Friedrich, A, Fujita, N, et al. (2012) Effects of daytime food intake on memory consolidation during sleep or sleep deprivation. PLoS One 7, e40298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roenneberg, T, Foster, RG & Klerman, EB (2022) The circadian system, sleep, and the health/disease balance: a conceptual review. J Sleep Res 31, e13621. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crispim, CA, Zalcman, I, Dáttilo, M, et al. (2007) The influence of sleep and sleep loss upon food intake and metabolism. Nutr Res Rev 20, 195212. doi: 10.1017/S0954422407810651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatamizargaran, Z, Sasani, M & Akhlaghi, M (2021) Association of non-alcoholic fatty liver disease with body fat percentage in normal-weight individuals. Shiraz E-Med J 22, e108643. doi: 10.5812/semj.108643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, SR, Ayas, NT, Malhotra, MR, et al. (2004) A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women. Sleep 27, 440444. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.3.440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoddy, KK, Potts, KS, Bazzano, LA & Kirwan, JP (2020) Sleep extension: a potential target for obesity treatment. Curr Diab Rep 20, 81. doi: 10.1007/s11892-020-01360-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, CB, Neufeld, EV, Dolezal, BA & Martin, JL (2018) Sleep deprivation and obesity in adults: a brief narrative review. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med 4, e000392. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2018-000392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, SR, Malhotra, A, White, DP, Gottlieb, DJ & Hu, FB (2006) Association between reduced sleep and weight gain in women. Am J Epidemiol 164, 947954. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, MA, Bates, S, Ji, C & Cappuccio, FP (2021) Systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between short sleep and incidence of obesity and effectiveness of sleep interventions on weight gain in preschool children. Obes Rev 22, e13113. doi: 10.1111/obr.13113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bacaro, V, Ballesio, A, Cerolini, S, et al. (2020) Sleep duration and obesity in adulthood: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Res Clin Pract 14, 301309. doi: 10.1016/j.orcp.2020.03.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guimarães, KC, Silva, CM, Latorraca, COC, Oliveira, & Crispim, CA (2022) Is self-reported short sleep duration associated with obesity? A systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Nutr Rev 80, 9831000. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuab064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deng, X, He, M, He, D, Zhu, Y, Zhang, Z & Niu, W (2021) Sleep duration and obesity in children and adolescents: evidence from an updated and dose-response meta-analysis. Sleep Med 78, 169181. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.12.027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capers, PL, Fobian, AD, Kaiser, KA, Borah, R & Allison, DB (2015) A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of the impact of sleep duration on adiposity and components of energy balance. Obes Rev 16, 771782. doi: 10.1111/obr.12296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, JC, Hallal, PC, Reichert, FF, Menezes, AM, Araújo, CL & Victora, CG (2008) Sleep patterns and television viewing in relation to obesity and blood pressure: evidence from an adolescent Brazilian birth cohort. Int J Obes (Lond) 32, 10421049. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappuccio, FP, Taggart, FM, Kandala, NB, et al. (2008) Meta-analysis of short sleep duration and obesity in children and adults. Sleep 31, 619626. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.5.619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covassin, N, Singh, P, McCrady-Spitzer, SK, et al. (2022) Effects of experimental sleep restriction on energy intake, energy expenditure, and visceral obesity. J Am Coll Cardiol 79, 12541265. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2022.01.038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mota, MC, Waterhouse, J, De-Souza, DA, et al. (2014) Sleep pattern is associated with adipokine levels and nutritional markers in resident physicians. Chronobiol Int 31, 11301138. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.957300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, A, Li, X, Zhou, T, et al. (2022) Sleep disturbance and changes in energy intake and body composition during weight loss in the POUNDS Lost Trial. Diabetes 71, 934944. doi: 10.2337/db21-0699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawamoto, R, Nozaki, T, Furukawa, T, et al. (2014) Higher sleep fragmentation predicts a lower magnitude of weight loss in overweight and obese women participating in a weight-loss intervention. Nutr Diabetes 4, e144. doi: 10.1038/nutd.2014.41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nedeltcheva, AV, Kilkus, JM, Imperial, J, Schoeller, DA & Penev, PD (2010) Insufficient sleep undermines dietary efforts to reduce adiposity. Ann Intern Med 153, 435441. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-153-7-201010050-00006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landhuis, CE, Poulton, R, Welch, D & Hancox, RJ (2008) Childhood sleep time and long-term risk for obesity: a 32-year prospective birth cohort study. Pediatrics 122, 955960. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ren, L, Chang, L, Kang, Y, Zhao, Y, Chen, F & Pei, L (2022) Gender-specific association between sleep duration and body mass index in rural China. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 13, 877100. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2022.877100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theorell-Haglöw, J, Berglund, L, Berne, C & Lindberg, E (2014) Both habitual short sleepers and long sleepers are at greater risk of obesity: a population-based 10-year follow-up in women. Sleep Med 15, 12041211. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.02.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y, Zhai, L & Zhang, D (2014) Sleep duration and obesity among adults: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep Med 15, 14561462. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.07.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brondel, L, Quilliot, D, Mouillot, T, Khan, NA, Bastable, P, Boggio, V, Leloup, C & Pénicaud, L (2022) Taste of fat and obesity: different hypotheses and our point of view. Nutrients 14, 555. doi: 10.3390/nu14030555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González-Ortiz, A, López-Bautista, F, Valencia-Flores, M & Espinosa Cuevas, Á (2020) Partial sleep deprivation on dietary energy intake in healthy population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Hosp 37, 10521060. English. doi: 10.20960/nh.03108.Google Scholar
Soltanieh, S, Solgi, S, Ansari, M, Santos, HO & Abbasi, B (2021) Effect of sleep duration on dietary intake, desire to eat, measures of food intake and metabolic hormones: a systematic review of clinical trials. Clin Nutr ESPEN 45, 5565. doi: 10.1016/j.clnesp.2021.07.029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuraikat, FM, Makarem, N, Liao, M, St-Onge, MP & Aggarwal, B (2020) Measures of poor sleep quality are associated with higher energy intake and poor diet quality in a diverse sample of women from the go red for women strategically focused research network. J Am Heart Assoc 9, e014587. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.014587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rangan, A, Zheng, M, Olsen, NJ, Rohde, JF & Heitmann, BL (2018) Shorter sleep duration is associated with higher energy intake and an increase in BMI z-score in young children predisposed to overweight. Int J Obes (Lond) 42, 5964. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2017.216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, JH, Grant, AS, Thomson, CA, et al. (2014) Short sleep duration is associated with decreased serum leptin, increased energy intake and decreased diet quality in postmenopausal women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, E55E61. doi: 10.1002/oby.20683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stelmach-Mardas, M, Iqbal, K, Mardas, M, et al. (2017) Synchronic inverse seasonal rhythmus of energy density of food intake and sleep quality: a contribution to chrono-nutrition from a Polish adult population. Eur J Clin Nutr 71, 718722. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2016.229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markwald, RR, Melanson, EL, Smith, MR, et al. (2013) Impact of insufficient sleep on total daily energy expenditure, food intake, and weight gain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110, 56955700. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216951110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-García, F, Juárez-Aguilar, E, Santiago-García, J & Cardinali, DP (2014) Ghrelin and its interactions with growth hormone, leptin and orexins: implications for the sleep-wake cycle and metabolism. Sleep Med Rev 18, 8997. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2013.04.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, SM, Hallschmid, M, Jauch-Chara, K, et al. (2009) Short-term sleep loss decreases physical activity under free-living conditions but does not increase food intake under time-deprived laboratory conditions in healthy men. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 14761482. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romney, L, Larson, MJ, Clark, T, Tucker, LA, Bailey, BW & LeCheminant, JD (2016) Reduced sleep acutely influences sedentary behavior and mood but not total energy intake in normal-weight and obese women. Behav Sleep Med 14, 528538. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2015.1036272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, AK & Graubard, BI (2014) Association of self-reported sleep duration with eating behaviors of American adults: NHANES 2005–2010. Am J Clin Nutr 100, 938947. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.085191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hashimoto, A, Inoue, H & Kuwano, T (2020) Low energy intake and dietary quality are associated with low objective sleep quality in young Japanese women. Nutr Res 80, 4454. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2020.06.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, B, Shi, C, Park, CG, Zhao, X & Reutrakul, S (2019) Effects of sleep restriction on metabolism-related parameters in healthy adults: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev 45, 1830. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.02.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brondel, L, Romer, MA, Nougues, PM, Touyarou, P & Davenne, D (2010) Acute partial sleep deprivation increases food intake in healthy men. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 15501559. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santa Cecília Silva, AA, Lopes, TDVC, Teixeira, KR, et al. (2017) The association between anxiety, hunger, the enjoyment of eating foods and the satiety after food intake in individuals working a night shift compared with after taking a nocturnal sleep: a prospective and observational study. Appetite 108, 255262. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogenkamp, PS, Nilsson, E, Chapman, CD, et al. (2013) Sweet taste perception not altered after acute sleep deprivation in healthy young men. Somnologie (Berl) 17, 111114. doi: 10.1007/s11818-013-0606-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tasali, E, Chapotot, F, Wroblewski, K & Schoeller, D (2014) The effects of extended bedtimes on sleep duration and food desire in overweight young adults: a home-based intervention. Appetite 80, 220224. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHill, AW, Hull, JT, McMullan, CJ & Klerman, EB (2018) Chronic insufficient sleep has a limited impact on circadian rhythmicity of subjective hunger and awakening fasted metabolic hormones. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 9, 319. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaput, JP & St-Onge, MP (2014) Increased food intake by insufficient sleep in humans: are we jumping the gun on the hormonal explanation? Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 5, 116. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, Y, Fernandez, ID, Meng, Y, Zhao, W & Groth, SW (2021) Gut hormones, adipokines, and pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines/markers in loss of control eating: a scoping review. Appetite 166, 105442. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caroleo, M, Carbone, EA, Primerano, A, Foti, D, Brunetti, A & Segura-Garcia, C (2019) The role of hormonal, metabolic and inflammatory biomarkers on sleep and appetite in drug free patients with major depression: a systematic review. J Affect Disord 250, 249259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frago, LM & Chowen, JA (2015) Hypothalamic leptin and ghrelin signaling as targets for improvement in metabolic control. Curr Pharm Des 21, 35963605. doi: 10.2174/1381612821666150710145428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taheri, S, Lin, L, Austin, D, Young, T & Mignot, E (2004) Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Med 1, e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0010062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaput, JP, Després, JP, Bouchard, C & Tremblay, A (2007) Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin levels and increased adiposity: results from the Quebec family study. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 253261. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spiegel, K, Tasali, E, Penev, P & Van Cauter, E (2004) Brief communication: sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Ann Intern Med 141, 846850. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-141-11-200412070-00008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullington, JM, Chan, JL, Van Dongen, HP, et al. (2003) Sleep loss reduces diurnal rhythm amplitude of leptin in healthy men. J Neuroendocrinol 15, 851854. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.01069.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, CD, Nilsson, EK, Nilsson, VC, et al. (2013) Acute sleep deprivation increases food purchasing in men. Obesity (Silver Spring) 21, E555E560. doi: 10.1002/oby.20579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nymo, S, Kleppe, MM, Coutinho, SR, Rehfeld, JF, Kulseng, B & Martins, C (2021) Association between habitual sleep duration/quality and appetite markers in individuals with obesity. Physiol Behav 232, 113345. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, CA, Hamilton, NA & Somers, VK (2016) Percentage of REM sleep is associated with overnight change in leptin. J Sleep Res 25, 419425. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spiegel, K, Leproult, R, L’hermite-Balériaux, M, Copinschi, G, Penev, PD & Van Cauter, E (2004) Leptin levels are dependent on sleep duration: relationships with sympathovagal balance, carbohydrate regulation, cortisol, and thyrotropin. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 89, 57625771. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirota, T, Morioka, T, Yoda, K, et al. (2018) Positive association of plasma leptin with sleep quality in obese type 2 diabetes patients. J Diabetes Investig 9, 11001105. doi: 10.1111/jdi.12826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boeke, CE, Storfer-Isser, A, Redline, S & Taveras, EM (2014) Childhood sleep duration and quality in relation to leptin concentration in two cohort studies. Sleep 37, 613620. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salzano, G, Maglitto, F, Bisogno, A, et al. (2021) Obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome: relationship with obesity and management in obese patients. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Ital 41, 120130. doi: 10.14639/0392-100X-N1100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, CN, Carskadon, MA, Considine, RV, et al. (2013) Changes in children’s sleep duration on food intake, weight, and leptin. Pediatrics 132, e1473e1480. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, SM, Hallschmid, M, Jauch-Chara, K, Born, J & Schultes, B (2008) A single night of sleep deprivation increases ghrelin levels and feelings of hunger in normal-weight healthy men. J Sleep Res 17, 331334. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00662.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broussard, JL, Kilkus, JM, Delebecque, F, et al. (2016) Elevated ghrelin predicts food intake during experimental sleep restriction. Obesity (Silver Spring) 24, 132138. doi: 10.1002/oby.21321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonnissen, HK, Hursel, R, Rutters, F, Martens, EA & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2013) Effects of sleep fragmentation on appetite and related hormone concentrations over 24 h in healthy men. Br J Nutr 109, 748756. doi: 10.1017/S0007114512001894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nedeltcheva, AV, Kilkus, JM, Imperial, J, Kasza, K, Schoeller, DA & Penev, PD (2009) Sleep curtailment is accompanied by increased intake of calories from snacks. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 126133. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radcliffe, PN, Whitney, CC, Fagnant, HS, et al. (2021) Severe sleep restriction suppresses appetite independent of effects on appetite regulating hormones in healthy young men without obesity. Physiol Behav 237, 113438. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rihm, JS, Menz, MM, Schultz, H, et al. (2019) Sleep deprivation selectively upregulates an amygdala-hypothalamic circuit involved in food reward. J Neurosci 39, 888899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0250-18.2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omisade, A, Buxton, OM & Rusak, B (2010) Impact of acute sleep restriction on cortisol and leptin levels in young women. Physiol Behav 99, 651656. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, NS, Banks, S & Dinges, DF (2010) Sleep restriction is associated with increased morning plasma leptin concentrations, especially in women. Biol Res Nurs 12, 4753. doi: 10.1177/1099800410366301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, J, Jiang, Y, Wang, G, et al. (2020) Associations of short sleep duration with appetite-regulating hormones and adipokines: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 21, e13051. doi: 10.1111/obr.13051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isidori, AM, Strollo, F, Morè, M, et al. (2000) Leptin and aging: correlation with endocrine changes in male and female healthy adult populations of different body weights. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85, 19541962. doi: 10.1210/jcem.85.5.6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izquierdo, AG, Crujeiras, AB, Casanueva, FF & Carreira, MC (2019) Leptin, obesity, and leptin resistance: where are we 25 years later? Nutrients 11, 2704. doi: 10.3390/nu11112704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, W (2015) Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiol Rev 95, 853951. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00023.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jager, G & Witkamp, RF (2014) The endocannabinoid system and appetite: relevance for food reward. Nutr Res Rev 27, 172185. doi: 10.1017/S0954422414000080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greer, SM, Goldstein, AN & Walker, MP (2013) The impact of sleep deprivation on food desire in the human brain. Nat Commun. 4:2259. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Q, Li, H & Guo, F (2011) Amygdala, an important regulator for food intake. Front Biol 6, 8285. doi 10.1007/s11515-011-0950-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izadi, MS & Radahmadi, M (2022) Overview of the central amygdala role in feeding behaviour. Br J Nutr 127, 953960. doi: 10.1017/S0007114521002312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uribe-Cerda, S, Morselli, E & Perez-Leighton, C (2018) Updates on the neurobiology of food reward and their relation to the obesogenic environment. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes 25, 292297. doi: 10.1097/MED.0000000000000427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishijo, H & Takatoshi, O. (2021) Neural mechanisms of feeding behavior and its disorders. In: Takada, A. New Insights Into Metabolic Syndrome. London: IntechOpen. Available from: https://www.intechopen.com/books/7841. doi: 10.5772/intechopen.77659.Google Scholar
Morales, I & Berridge, KC (2020) ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ in eating and food reward: brain mechanisms and clinical implications. Physiol Behav 227, 113152. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaput, JP (2010) Short sleep duration promoting overconsumption of food: a reward-driven eating behavior? Sleep 33, 11351136. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.9.1135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Z, Wang, Y, Cai, L, et al. (2016) Prefrontal cortex to Accumbens projections in sleep regulation of reward. J Neurosci 36, 78977910. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0347-16.2016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demos, KE, Sweet, LH, Hart, CN, et al. (2017) The effects of experimental manipulation of sleep duration on neural response to food cues. Sleep 40, zsx125. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St-Onge, MP, McReynolds, A, Trivedi, ZB, Roberts, AL, Sy, M & Hirsch, J (2012) Sleep restriction leads to increased activation of brain regions sensitive to food stimuli. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 818824. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.027383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katsunuma, R, Oba, K, Kitamura, S, et al. (2017) Unrecognized sleep loss accumulated in daily life can promote brain hyperreactivity to food cue. Sleep 40. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, C, Brooks, SJ, O’Daly, OG, et al. (2012) Acute sleep deprivation enhances the brain’s response to hedonic food stimuli: an fMRI study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 97, E443E447. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Klerk, MT, Smeets, PAM & la Fleur, SE (2022) Inhibitory control as a potential treatment target for obesity. Nutr Neurosci, 116. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2022.2053406.Google Scholar
Appelhans, BM, Woolf, K, Pagoto, SL, Schneider, KL, Whited, MC & Liebman, R (2011) Inhibiting food reward: delay discounting, food reward sensitivity, and palatable food intake in overweight and obese women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19, 21752182. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cedernaes, J, Brandell, J, Ros, O, et al. (2014) Increased impulsivity in response to food cues after sleep loss in healthy young men. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, 17861791. doi: 10.1002/oby.20786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundahl, A & Nelson, TD. (2015) Sleep and food intake: a multisystem review of mechanisms in children and adults. J Health Psychol 20, 794805. doi: 10.1177/1359105315573427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, CM & Kanoski, SE (2018) Homeostatic and non-homeostatic controls of feeding behavior: distinct vs. common neural systems. Physiol Behav 193, 223231. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginieis, R, Abeywickrema, S, Oey, I & Peng, M (2022) Testing links of food-related olfactory perception to peripheral ghrelin and leptin concentrations. Front Nutr 9:888608. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.888608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthoud, HR (2006) Homeostatic and non-homeostatic pathways involved in the control of food intake and energy balance. Obesity (Silver Spring) 14(Suppl 5), 197S200S. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semplonius, T & Willoughby, T (2018) Long-term links between physical activity and sleep quality. Med Sci Sports Exerc 50, 24182424. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromley, LE, Booth, JN 3rd, Kilkus, JM, Imperial, JG & Penev, PD (2012) Sleep restriction decreases the physical activity of adults at risk for type 2 diabetes. Sleep 35, 977984. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, JN, Bromley, LE, Darukhanavala, AP, Whitmore, HR, Imperial, JG & Penev, PD (2012) Reduced physical activity in adults at risk for type 2 diabetes who curtail their sleep. Obesity (Silver Spring) 20, 278284. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jindal, I, Puyau, M, Adolph, A, Butte, N, Musaad, S & Bacha, F (2021) The relationship of sleep duration and quality to energy expenditure and physical activity in children. Pediatr Obes 16, e12751. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hursel, R, Rutters, F, Gonnissen, HK, Martens, EA & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2011) Effects of sleep fragmentation in healthy men on energy expenditure, substrate oxidation, physical activity, and exhaustion measured over 48 h in a respiratory chamber. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 804808. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.017632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, J, Doucet, É, Brunet, JF, et al. (2016) The effects of sleep restriction and altered sleep timing on energy intake and energy expenditure. Physiol Behav 164, 157163. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J (2017) Sleep duration’s association with diet, physical activity, mental status, and weight among Korean high school students. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 26, 906913. doi: 10.6133/apjcn.082016.04.Google Scholar
Porter, JW, Pettit-Mee, RJ, Emerson, TS, et al. (2021) Modest sleep restriction does not influence steps, physical activity intensity or glucose tolerance in obese adults. J Sleep Res 30, e13381. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosy-Westphal, A, Hinrichs, S, Jauch-Chara, K, et al. (2008) Influence of partial sleep deprivation on energy balance and insulin sensitivity in healthy women. Obes Facts 1, 266273. doi: 10.1159/000158874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilms, B, Kuhr, M, Chamorro, R, et al. (2020) Chronobiological aspects of sleep restriction modulate subsequent spontaneous physical activity. Physiol Behav 215, 112795. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shechter, A, Rising, R, Albu, JB & St-Onge, MP (2013) Experimental sleep curtailment causes wake-dependent increases in 24-h energy expenditure as measured by whole-room indirect calorimetry. Am J Clin Nutr 98, 14331439. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.069427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingenberg, L, Chaput, JP, Holmbäck, U, Jennum, P, Astrup, A & Sjödin, A (2012) Sleep restriction is not associated with a positive energy balance in adolescent boys. Am J Clin Nutr 96, 240248. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.038638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, CM, Melanson, EL, Frydendall, EJ, Perreault, L, Eckel, RH & Wright, KP (2011) Energy expenditure during sleep, sleep deprivation and sleep following sleep deprivation in adult humans. J Physiol 589, 235244. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.197517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, C, Hallschmid, M, Lassen, A, et al. (2011) Acute sleep deprivation reduces energy expenditure in healthy men. Am J Clin Nutr 93, 12291236. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.006460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibi, M, Kubota, C, Mizuno, T, et al. (2017) Effect of shortened sleep on energy expenditure, core body temperature, and appetite: a human randomised crossover trial. Sci Rep 7, 39640. doi: 10.1038/srep39640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St-Onge, MP, Roberts, AL, Chen, J, et al. (2011) Short sleep duration increases energy intakes but does not change energy expenditure in normal-weight individuals. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 410416. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.013904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvin, AD, Carter, RE, Adachi, T, et al. (2013) Effects of experimental sleep restriction on caloric intake and activity energy expenditure. Chest 144, 7986. doi: 10.1378/chest.12-2829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kayaba, M, Park, I, Iwayama, K, et al. (2017) Energy metabolism differs between sleep stages and begins to increase prior to awakening. Metabolism 69, 1423. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2016.12.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitale, EM & Smith, AS (2022) Neurobiology of loneliness, isolation, and loss: integrating human and animal perspectives. Front Behav Neurosci 16, 846315. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.846315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haase, CG, Long, AK & Gillooly, JF (2016) Energetics of stress: linking plasma cortisol levels to metabolic rate in mammals. Biol Lett 12, 20150867. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St-Onge, MP & Shechter, A (2014) Sleep disturbances, body fat distribution, food intake and/or energy expenditure: pathophysiological aspects. Horm Mol Biol Clin Investig 17, 2937. doi: 10.1515/hmbci-2013-0066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jonge, L, Zhao, X, Mattingly, MS, et al. (2012) Poor sleep quality and sleep apnea are associated with higher resting energy expenditure in obese individuals with short sleep duration. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 97:28812889. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurado-Fasoli, L, Mochon-Benguigui, S, Castillo, MJ & Amaro-Gahete, FJ (2020) Association between sleep quality and time with energy metabolism in sedentary adults. Sci Rep 10, 4598. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61493-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shechter, A, Rising, R, Wolfe, S, Albu, JB & St-Onge, MP (2014) Postprandial thermogenesis and substrate oxidation are unaffected by sleep restriction. Int J Obes (Lond) 38, 11531158. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spaeth, AM, Dinges, DF & Goel, N (2015) Resting metabolic rate varies by race and by sleep duration. Obesity (Silver Spring) 23, 23492356. doi: 10.1002/oby.21198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonasdottir, SS, Minor, K & Lehmann, S (2021) Gender differences in nighttime sleep patterns and variability across the adult lifespan: a global-scale wearables study. Sleep 44. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adan, A & Natale, V (2002) Gender differences in morningness-eveningness preference. Chronobiol Int 19, 709720. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120005390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, SW, Dennison, CF, Zeitzer, JM, et al. (2010) Sex differences in phase angle of entrainment and melatonin amplitude in humans. J Biol Rhythms 25, 288296. doi: 10.1177/0748730410374943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, JF, Cain, SW, Chang, AM, et al. (2011) Sex difference in the near-24-hour intrinsic period of the human circadian timing system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108(Suppl 3), 1560215608. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010666108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St-Onge, MP, O’Keeffe, M, Roberts, AL, RoyChoudhury, A & Laferrère, B (2012) Short sleep duration, glucose dysregulation and hormonal regulation of appetite in men and women. Sleep 35, 15031510. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qian, J, Morris, CJ, Caputo, R, Wang, W, Garaulet, M & Scheer, FAJL (2019) Sex differences in the circadian misalignment effects on energy regulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 2380623812. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1914003116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spaeth, AM, Dinges, DF & Goel, N (2013) Effects of experimental sleep restriction on weight gain, caloric intake, and meal timing in healthy adults. Sleep 36, 981990. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, SR, Blackwell, T, Redline, S, et al. (2008) The association between sleep duration and obesity in older adults. Int J Obes (Lond) 32, 18251834. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, JK, Sleddens, EFC, Vink, JM, van den Broek, N & Kremers, SPJ (2017) The sex-specific interaction between food responsiveness and sleep duration explaining body mass index among children. Sleep Med 40, 106109. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.09.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skidmore, PM, Howe, AS, Polak, MA, et al. (2013) Sleep duration and adiposity in older adolescents from Otago, New Zealand: relationships differ between boys and girls and are independent of food choice. Nutr J 12, 128. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-12-128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, N, Jauch-Chara, K, Oltmanns, KM & Benedict, C (2012) Compromised sleep increases food intake in humans: two sexes, same response? Am J Clin Nutr 95, 531; author reply 531-2. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.028647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cao, M, Zhu, Y, Sun, F, Luo, J & Jing, J (2019) Short sleep duration is associated with specific food intake increase among school-aged children in China: a national cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 19, 558. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6739-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alkozei, A, Killgore, WDS, Smith, R, et al. (2018) Chronic sleep restriction differentially affects implicit biases toward food among men and women: preliminary evidence. J Sleep Res 27, e12629. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, CD, Duraccio, KM, Barnett, KA, et al. (2019) Sleep duration differentially affects brain activation in response to food images in adolescents with overweight/obesity compared to adolescents with normal weight. Sleep 42. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duraccio, KM, Zaugg, K & Jensen, CD (2019) Effects of sleep restriction on food-related inhibitory control and reward in adolescents. J Pediatr Psychol 44, 692702. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsz008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alonso-Alonso, M, Woods, SC, Pelchat, M, et al. (2015) Food reward system: current perspectives and future research needs. Nutr Rev 73, 296307. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, JS, Jenkins, SM & Nolan, LJ (2014) The role of emotional eating and stress in the influence of short sleep on food consumption. Appetite 72, 106113. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogenkamp, PS, Nilsson, E, Nilsson, VC, et al. (2013) Acute sleep deprivation increases portion size and affects food choice in young men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 16681674. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, SL, Field, J, Miller, LE, DiFrancesco, M & Beebe, DW (2015) Sweet/dessert foods are more appealing to adolescents after sleep restriction. PLoS One 10, e0115434. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khan, A & Uddin, R (2020) Is consumption of fast-food and carbonated soft drink associated with anxiety-induced sleep disturbance among adolescents? A population-based study. Clin Nutr ESPEN 36, 162165. doi: 10.1016/j.clnesp.2020.01.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tambalis, KD, Panagiotakos, DB, Psarra, G & Sidossis, LS (2018) Insufficient sleep duration is associated with dietary habits, screen time, and obesity in children. J Clin Sleep Med 14, 16891696. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.7374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruger, AK, Reither, EN, Peppard, PE, Krueger, PM & Hale, L (2014) Do sleep-deprived adolescents make less-healthy food choices? Br J Nutr 111, 18981904. doi: 10.1017/S0007114514000130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cerolini, S, Rodgers, RF & Lombardo, C (2018) Partial sleep deprivation and food intake in participants reporting binge eating symptoms and emotional eating: preliminary results of a quasi-experimental study. Eat Weight Disord 23, 561570. doi: 10.1007/s40519-018-0547-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lv, W, Finlayson, G & Dando, R (2018) Sleep, food cravings and taste. Appetite 125, 210216. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Macedo, IC, de Freitas, JS & da Silva Torres, IL (2016) The influence of palatable diets in reward system activation: a mini review. Adv Pharmacol Sci 2016, 7238679. doi: 10.1155/2016/7238679.Google Scholar
St-Onge, MP, Wolfe, S, Sy, M, Shechter, A & Hirsch, J (2014) Sleep restriction increases the neuronal response to unhealthy food in normal-weight individuals. Int J Obes (Lond) 38, 411416. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fang, Z, Spaeth, AM, Ma, N, et al. (2015) Altered salience network connectivity predicts macronutrient intake after sleep deprivation. Sci Rep 5, 8215. doi: 10.1038/srep08215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kracht, CL, Chaput, JP, Martin, CK, Champagne, CM, Katzmarzyk, PT & Staiano, AE (2019) Associations of sleep with food cravings, diet, and obesity in adolescence. Nutrients 11, 2899. doi: 10.3390/nu11122899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al Khatib, H, Dikariyanto, V, Bermingham, KM, Gibson, R & Hall, WL (2022) Short sleep and social jetlag are associated with higher intakes of non-milk extrinsic sugars, and social jetlag is associated with lower fibre intakes in those with adequate sleep duration: a cross-sectional analysis from the National Diet and Nutrition Survey Rolling Programme (Years 1–9). Public Health Nutr 25, 25702581. doi: 10.1017/S1368980022000167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeMay-Russell, S, Tanofsky-Kraff, M, Schvey, NA, et al. (2019) Associations of weekday and weekend sleep with children’s reported eating in the absence of hunger. Nutrients 11, 1658. doi: 10.3390/nu11071658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buxton, OM, Quintiliani, LM, Yang, MH, et al. (2009) Association of sleep adequacy with more healthful food choices and positive workplace experiences among motor freight workers. Am J Public Health 99(Suppl 3), S636S643. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.158501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henst, RHP, Pienaar, PR, Roden, LC & Rae, DE (2019) The effects of sleep extension on cardiometabolic risk factors: a systematic review. J Sleep Res 28, e12865. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duraccio, KM, Krietsch, KN, Zhang, N, et al. (2021)The impact of short sleep on food reward processes in adolescents. J Sleep Res 30, e13054. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunet, JF, McNeil, J, Jaeger Hintze, L, Doucet, É & Forest, G (2022) Inter-individual differences in energy intake after sleep restriction: the role of personality and implicit attitudes toward food. Appetite 169, 105844. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golley, RK, Maher, CA, Matricciani, L & Olds, TS (2013) Sleep duration or bedtime? Exploring the association between sleep timing behaviour, diet and BMI in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond) 37, 546551. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Farinós, N, Villar-Villalba, C, López Sobaler, AM, et al. (2017) The relationship between hours of sleep, screen time and frequency of food and drink consumption in Spain in the 2011 and 2013 ALADINO: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 17, 33. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3962-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Börnhorst, C, Wijnhoven, TM, Kunešová, M, et al. (2015) WHO European Childhood Obesity Surveillance Initiative: associations between sleep duration, screen time and food consumption frequencies. BMC Public Health 15, 442. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1793-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Min, C, Kim, HJ, Park, IS, et al. (2018) The association between sleep duration, sleep quality, and food consumption in adolescents: a cross-sectional study using the Korea Youth Risk Behavior Web-based Survey. BMJ Open 8, e022848. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-022848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asarnow, LD, Greer, SM, Walker, MP & Harvey, AG (2017) The impact of sleep improvement on food choices in adolescents with late bedtimes. J Adolesc Health 60, 570576. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.11.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, RA, McTighe, S & Juarez, M (1986) Sleep duration and eating behaviors of college students. Percept Mot Skills 62, 2526. doi: 10.2466/pms.1986.62.1.25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A, Xu, F, Storfer-Isser, A, Thomas, A, Ievers-Landis, CE & Redline, S (2010) The association of sleep duration with adolescents’ fat and carbohydrate consumption. Sleep 33, 12011209. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.9.1201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kjeldsen, JS, Hjorth, MF, Andersen, R, et al. (2014) Short sleep duration and large variability in sleep duration are independently associated with dietary risk factors for obesity in Danish school children. Int J Obes (Lond) 38, 3239. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajiri, E, Yoshimura, E, Hatamoto, Y, Shiratsuchi, H, Tanaka, S & Shimoda, S (2020) Acute sleep curtailment increases sweet taste preference, appetite and food intake in healthy young adults: a randomized crossover trial. Behav Sci (Basel) 10, 47. doi: 10.3390/bs10020047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szczygiel, EJ, Cho, S & Tucker, RM (2019) The effect of sleep curtailment on hedonic responses to liquid and solid food. Foods 8, 465. doi: 10.3390/foods8100465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papp, M, Willner, P & Muscat, R (1991) An animal model of anhedonia: attenuation of sucrose consumption and place preference conditioning by chronic unpredictable mild stress. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 104, 255259. doi: 10.1007/BF02244188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, CL, Schnepp, J & Tucker, RM (2019) Increased hunger, food cravings, food reward, and portion size selection after sleep curtailment in women without obesity. Nutrients 11, 663. doi: 10.3390/nu11030663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beebe, DW, Simon, S, Summer, S, Hemmer, S, Strotman, D & Dolan, LM (2013) Dietary intake following experimentally restricted sleep in adolescents. Sleep 36, 827834. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duraccio, KM, Whitacre, C, Krietsch, KN, et al. (2022) Losing sleep by staying up late leads adolescents to consume more carbohydrates and a higher glycemic load. Sleep 45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajhashemy, Z, Mirzaei, S, Asadi, A, Akhlaghi, M & Saneei, P (2022) Association of dietary insulin index and dietary insulin load with metabolic health status in Iranian overweight and obese adolescents. Front Nutr 9, 821089. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.821089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, P, Mohebbi, M, Seo, HS & Hummel, T (2020) Sensitivity to sweetness correlates to elevated reward brain responses to sweet and high-fat food odors in young healthy volunteers. Neuroimage 208, 116413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohanmoo, A, Faghih, S & Akhlaghi, M (2020) Effect of short- and long-term protein consumption on appetite and appetite-regulating gastrointestinal hormones, a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Physiol Behav 226, 113123. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saleh-Ghadimi, S, Dehghan, P, Abbasalizad Farhangi, M, Asghari-Jafarabadi, M & Jafari-Vayghan, H (2019) Could emotional eating act as a mediator between sleep quality and food intake in female students? Biopsychosoc Med 13, 15. doi: 10.1186/s13030-019-0154-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, Z, McEvoy, M, Luu, J & Attia, J (2008) Dietary fat and sleep duration in Chinese men and women. Int J Obes (Lond) 32, 18351840. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, J, Forest, G, Hintze, LJ, et al. (2017) The effects of partial sleep restriction and altered sleep timing on appetite and food reward. Appetite 109, 4856. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mi, SJ, Kelly, NR, Brychta, RJ, et al. (2019) Associations of sleep patterns with metabolic syndrome indices, body composition, and energy intake in children and adolescents. Pediatr Obes 14, e12507. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saidi, O, Rochette, E, Bovet, M, Merlin, E & Duché, P (2020) Acute intense exercise improves sleep and decreases next morning consumption of energy-dense food in adolescent girls with obesity and evening chronotype. Pediatr Obes 15, e12613. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galli, G, Piaggi, P, Mattingly, MS, et al. (2013) Inverse relationship of food and alcohol intake to sleep measures in obesity. Nutr Diabetes 3, e58. doi: 10.1038/nutd.2012.33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dashti, HS, Follis, JL, Smith, CE, et al. (2015) Habitual sleep duration is associated with BMI and macronutrient intake and may be modified by CLOCK genetic variants. Am J Clin Nutr 101, 135143. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.095026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez, SM, Tschann, JM, Butte, NF, et al. (2017) Short sleep duration is associated with eating more carbohydrates and less dietary fat in Mexican American children. Sleep 40. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spaeth, AM, Goel, N & Dinges, DF (2020) Caloric and macronutrient intake and meal timing responses to repeated sleep restriction exposures separated by varying intervening recovery nights in healthy adults. Nutrients 12, 2694. doi: 10.3390/nu12092694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCuen-Wurst, C, Ruggieri, M & Allison, KC (2018) Disordered eating and obesity: associations between binge-eating disorder, night-eating syndrome, and weight-related comorbidities. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1411, 96105. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutcu, C, Pamuk, G & Ongel, K (2021) Evaluation of night eating syndrome in individuals with and without obesity. Endokrynol Pol 72, 539544. doi: 10.5603/EP.a2021.0046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonken, LK, Workman, JL, Walton, JC, Weil, ZM, Morris, JS, Haim, A & Nelson, RJ (2010) Light at night increases body mass by shifting the time of food intake. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 1866418669. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008734107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crispim, CA, Zimberg, IZ, dos Reis, BG, Diniz, RM, Tufik, S & de Mello, MT (2011) Relationship between food intake and sleep pattern in healthy individuals. J Clin Sleep Med 7, 659664. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.1476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gwin, JA & Leidy, HJ (2018) Breakfast consumption augments appetite, eating behavior, and exploratory markers of sleep quality compared with skipping breakfast in healthy young adults. Curr Dev Nutr 2. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzy074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khan, MKA, Faught, EL, Chu, YL, Ekwaru, JP, Storey, KE & Veugelers, PJ (2017) Is it nutrients, food items, diet quality or eating behaviours that are responsible for the association of children’s diet with sleep? J Sleep Res 26, 468476. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menezes-Júnior, LAA, Andrade, ACS, Coletro, HN, et al. (2022) Food consumption according to the level of processing and sleep quality during the COVID-19 pandemic. Clin Nutr ESPEN 49, 348356. doi: 10.1016/j.clnesp.2022.03.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werneck, AO, Vancampfort, D, Oyeyemi, AL, Stubbs, B & Silva, DR (2020) Joint association of ultra-processed food and sedentary behavior with anxiety-induced sleep disturbance among Brazilian adolescents. J Affect Disord 266, 135142. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werneck, AO, Hoare, E & Silva, DR (2021) Do TV viewing and frequency of ultra-processed food consumption share mediators in relation to adolescent anxiety-induced sleep disturbance? Public Health Nutr 24, 54915497. doi: 10.1017/S1368980021000379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St-Onge, MP, Roberts, A, Shechter, A & Choudhury, AR (2016) Fiber and saturated fat are associated with sleep arousals and slow wave sleep. J Clin Sleep Med 12, 1924. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.5384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowlden, AP, Burns, M, Harcrow, A & Shewmake, ME (2016) Cross-sectional analysis of food choice frequency, sleep confounding beverages, and psychological distress predictors of sleep quality. Int J Adolesc Med Health 30. doi: 10.1515/ijamh-2015-0120.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, K, Ota, M, Minematsu, A, et al. (2018) Association between adherence to the japanese food guide spinning top and sleep quality in college students. Nutrients 10, 1996. doi: 10.3390/nu10121996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meng, X, Li, Y, Li, S, et al. (2017) Dietary sources and bioactivities of melatonin. Nutrients 9, 367. doi: 10.3390/nu9040367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castellucci, B, Barrea, L, Laudisio, D, et al. (2021) Improving sleep disturbances in obesity by nutritional strategies: review of current evidence and practical guide. Int J Food Sci Nutr 72, 579591. doi: 10.1080/09637486.2020.1851659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q (2021) Food insecurity and sleep disturbance among 223,561 adolescents: a multi-country analysis of cross-sectional surveys. Front Public Health 9, 693544. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.693544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gyasi, RM, Asamoah, E, Gyasi-Boadu, N, Zornu, O, Asiki, G & Phillips, DR (2022) Food insecurity and sleep quality among older adults: Findings from a population-based study in Ghana. Maturitas 157, 2733. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2021.10.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troxel, WM, Haas, A, Ghosh-Dastidar, B, et al. (2020) Food insecurity is associated with objectively measured sleep problems. Behav Sleep Med 18, 719729. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2019.1669605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, ML, Perez-Escamilla, R, Desai, MM & Shamah-Levy, T (2016) Household food insecurity and sleep patterns among Mexican adults: results from ENSANUT-2012. J Immigr Minor Health 18, 10931103. doi: 10.1007/s10903-015-0246-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y, Njai, RS, Greenlund, KJ, Chapman, DP & Croft, JB (2014) Relationships between housing and food insecurity, frequent mental distress, and insufficient sleep among adults in 12 US States, 2009. Prev Chronic Dis 11, E37. doi: 10.5888/pcd11.130334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ding, M, Keiley, MK, Garza, KB, Duffy, PA & Zizza, CA (2015) Food insecurity is associated with poor sleep outcomes among US adults. J Nutr 145, 615621. doi: 10.3945/jn.114.199919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arenas, DJ, Thomas, A, Wang, J & DeLisser, HM (2019) A systematic review and meta-analysis of depression, anxiety, and sleep disorders in us adults with food insecurity. J Gen Intern Med 34, 28742882. doi: 10.1007/s11606-019-05202-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagata, JM, Palar, K, Gooding, HC, et al. (2019) Food insecurity is associated with poorer mental health and sleep outcomes in young adults. J Adolesc Health 65, 805811. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.08.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narcisse, MR, Long, CR, Felix, H, Rowland, B, Bursac, Z & McElfish, PA (2018) The mediating role of sleep quality and quantity in the link between food insecurity and obesity across race and ethnicity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 26, 15091518. doi: 10.1002/oby.22266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, X, Wu, D, Qi, X, et al. (2018) The associations between carbohydrate and protein intakes with habitual sleep duration among adults living in urban and rural areas. Clin Nutr 37, 16311637. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2017.07.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanaka, E, Yatsuya, H, Uemura, M, et al. (2013) Associations of protein, fat, and carbohydrate intakes with insomnia symptoms among middle-aged Japanese workers. J Epidemiol 23, 132138. doi: 10.2188/jea.je20120101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Afaghi, A, O’Connor, H & Chow, CM (2007) High-glycemic-index carbohydrate meals shorten sleep onset. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 426430. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavakoli, A, Mirzababaei, A & Mirzaei, K (2021) Association between low carbohydrate diet (LCD) and sleep quality by mediating role of inflammatory factors in women with overweight and obesity: a cross-sectional study. Food Sci Nutr 9, 62526261. doi: 10.1002/fsn3.2584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneshzad, E, Keshavarz, SA, Qorbani, M, Larijani, B & Azadbakht, L (2020) Association between a low-carbohydrate diet and sleep status, depression, anxiety, and stress score. J Sci Food Agric 100, 29462952. doi: 10.1002/jsfa.10322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vlahoyiannis, A, Giannaki, CD, Sakkas, GK, Aphamis, G & Andreou, E (2021) A systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression on the effects of carbohydrates on sleep. Nutrients 13, 1283. doi: 10.3390/nu13041283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, S, Lee, Y & Lee, JH (2016) Association between energy drink intake, sleep, stress, and suicidality in Korean adolescents: energy drink use in isolation or in combination with junk food consumption. Nutr J 15, 87. doi: 10.1186/s12937-016-0204-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangwisch, JE, Hale, L, St-Onge, MP, et al. (2020) High glycemic index and glycemic load diets as risk factors for insomnia: analyses from the Women’s Health Initiative. Am J Clin Nutr 111, 429439. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, CJ, Cain, SW & Blumfield, ML (2019) Monounsaturated fat intake is associated with improved sleep quality in pregnancy. Midwifery 78, 6470. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2019.07.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cao, Y, Taylor, AW, Pan, X, Adams, R, Appleton, S & Shi, Z (2016) Dinner fat intake and sleep duration and self-reported sleep parameters over five years: findings from the Jiangsu Nutrition Study of Chinese adults. Nutrition 32, 970974. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2016.02.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, F, Morita, E, Miyagi, S, et al. (2021) Protein intake in inhabitants with regular exercise is associated with sleep quality: results of the Shika study. PLoS One 16, e0247926. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, J, Kim, JE, Armstrong, CL, Chen, N & Campbell, WW (2016) Higher-protein diets improve indexes of sleep in energy-restricted overweight and obese adults: results from 2 randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr 103, 766774. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.124669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, JL, Zhou, J & Campbell, WW (2020) Adults who are overweight or obese and consuming an energy-restricted healthy us-style eating pattern at either the recommended or a higher protein quantity perceive a shift from “poor” to “good” sleep: a randomized controlled trial. J Nutr 150, 32163223. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxaa302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhat, A, Pires, AS, Tan, V, Babu Chidambaram, S & Guillemin, GJ (2020) Effects of sleep deprivation on the tryptophan metabolism. Int J Tryptophan Res 13. doi: 10.1177/1178646920970902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, CR, Jonkman, LM, Lammers, JH, Deutz, NE, Messer, MH & Rigtering, N (2005) Evening intake of alpha-lactalbumin increases plasma tryptophan availability and improves morning alertness and brain measures of attention. Am J Clin Nutr 81, 10261033. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/81.5.1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutanto, CN, Loh, WW, Toh, DWK, Lee, DPS & Kim, JE (2022) Association between dietary protein intake and sleep quality in middle-aged and older adults in Singapore. Front Nutr 9, 832341. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.832341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherasse, Y & Urade, Y (2017) Dietary zinc acts as a sleep modulator. Int J Mol Sci 18, 2334. doi: 10.3390/ijms18112334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arab, A, Rafie, N, Amani, R & Shirani, F (2022) The role of magnesium in sleep health: a systematic review of available literature. Biol Trace Elem Res. doi: 10.1007/s12011-022-03162-1.Google Scholar
Haarhuis, JE, Kardinaal, A & Kortman, GAM (2022) Probiotics, prebiotics and postbiotics for better sleep quality: a narrative review. Benef Microbes 13, 169182. doi: 10.3920/BM2021.0122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szentirmai, É, Millican, NS, Massie, AR & Kapás, L (2019) Butyrate, a metabolite of intestinal bacteria, enhances sleep. Sci Rep 9, 7035. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43502-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandner, MA, Jackson, N, Gerstner, JR & Knutson, KL (2013) Dietary nutrients associated with short and long sleep duration. Data from a nationally representative sample. Appetite 64, 7180. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.01.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atrooz, F & Salim, S (2020) Sleep deprivation, oxidative stress and inflammation. Adv Protein Chem Struct Biol 119, 309336. doi: 10.1016/bs.apcsb.2019.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jówko, E, Różański, P & Tomczak, A (2018) Effects of a 36-h survival training with sleep deprivation on oxidative stress and muscle damage biomarkers in young healthy men. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15, 2066. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reimund, E (1994) The free radical flux theory of sleep. Med Hypotheses 43, 231233. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(94)90071-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beydoun, MA, Gamaldo, AA, Canas, JA, et al. (2014) Serum nutritional biomarkers and their associations with sleep among US adults in recent national surveys. PLoS One 9, e103490. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiong, S, Liu, Z, Yao, N, Zhang, X & Ge, Q (2022) The independent association between vitamin B12 and insomnia in Chinese patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus: a cross-sectional study. Nutr Diabetes 12, 3. doi: 10.1038/s41387-022-00181-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, G, Kröger, M & Meier-Ewert, K (1996) Effects of vitamin B12 on performance and circadian rhythm in normal subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 15, 456464. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00055-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Proposed schematic of sleep and wake regulation by two homoeostatic and circadian processes as well as a simplistic view of diurnal changes in melatonin and cortisol levels. Homoeostatic process (blue curve) is hypothesised to be regulated by adenosine levels in the brain. In homoeostatic cycle, neuronal activity gradually increases adenosine levels in the brain, reaching its highest level concomitant with sleepiness and desire to sleep at the time of sleep. In circadian process (red curve), sunlight inhibits the circadian clock located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), thus inhibiting the SCN signals. In dark, SCN signals stimulate the pineal gland to produce melatonin, a molecule synthesised from tryptophan that has an important role in regulation of sleep–wake cycle. SCN also sends signals to the superior cervical ganglion, which regulates circadian rhythm in other parts of the body such as heart and circulatory system. In sleep-deprivation, the regular sleep–wake cycle is disrupted and this leads to misalignment of homoeostatic (demonstrated with brown colour) and circadian processes, leading to a number of brain and systemic dysregulations. Melatonin levels (dashed green curve), which are positively associated with drowsiness and sleep, are lowest during the day and highest during the night. Conversely, cortisol levels (dotted pink curve) are lowest at midnight, gradually increase during night sleep, peak at approximately 09.00, and then gradually decline to reach a minimum again at midnight.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Possible mechanisms by which sleep deprivation lead to overweight and obesity. CCK, cholecystokinin; GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide-1.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Hypothetical scheme for the possible effect of poor sleep quality on physical activity.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. The effect of sleep deprivation on males and females. GLP-1, glucagon like peptide-1.

Figure 4

Fig. 5. Differential response to sleep restriction according to body weight. Both normal-weight individuals and individuals who are overweight/obese are assumed to stimulate the food reward system more strongly during sleep deprivation, but individuals who are overweight/obese indicate a weaker inhibitory control over eating compared with normal-weight individuals.

Figure 5

Fig. 6. Effect of sleep deprivation on eating behaviours and food selection.

Figure 6

Fig. 7. Nutrients with possible influence on sleep health. GI, glycemic index; PUFA, polyunsaturated fatty acids; SFA, saturated fatty acids; Trp, tryptophan.