Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T00:23:07.996Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory free classification of native and nonnative speech by nonnative listeners

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 December 2014

ERIKO ATAGI
Affiliation:
Indiana University
TESSA BENT*
Affiliation:
Indiana University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Tessa Bent, Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, 200 South Jordan Avenue, Bloomington, IN 47405. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Nonnative listeners are less accurate than native listeners at identifying, categorizing, and classifying native talkers’ accents, suggesting a decrement in nonnative listeners’ representation of native variation. Nonnative listeners’ representation of nonnative variation, however, may be as robust as, or more robust than, that of native listeners. To compare native and nonnative listeners’ representations of linguistic variation, the current study examined native English, Korean, and Spanish listeners’ classification of native and nonnative talkers by native language. Overall, the listener groups performed with similar accuracy, but they exhibited different perceptual similarity spaces. Specifically, listener groups demonstrated heightened perceptual sensitivity to talkers with whom they share a native language. The results suggest that listeners’ linguistic experiences significantly shape their perceptual representation of nonnative varieties.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Adank, P., Evans, B. G., Stuart-Smith, J., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Comprehension of familiar and unfamiliar native accents under adverse listening conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 520529.Google ScholarPubMed
Atagi, E., & Bent, T. (2013). Auditory free classification of nonnative speech. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 509519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bent, T. (2014). Children's perception of foreign-accented words. Journal of Child Language, 41, 13341355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bent, T., & Bradlow, A. R. (2003). The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 16001610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Goodell, E. (2001). Discrimination of non-native consonant contrasts varying in perceptual assimilation to the listener's native phonological system. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 775794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D., Gooding, T. N., Orlando, C. B., & Quann, C. A. (2009). Development of phonological constancy: Toddlers’ perception of native- and Jamaican-accented words. Psychological Science, 20, 539542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, J., Floccia, C., Goslin, J., & Panneton, R. (2011). Infants’ discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar accents in speech. Infancy, 16, 392417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, C. G. (2008). Auditory free classification: Methods and analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 575581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, C. G., & Bradlow, A. R. (2009). Free classification of American English dialects by native and non-native listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 436451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, C. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004a). Homebodies and army brats: Some effects of early linguistic experience and residential history on dialect categorization. Language Variation and Change, 16, 3148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, C. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004b). Some acoustic cues for the perceptual categorization of American English regional dialects. Journal of Phonetics, 32, 111140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, C. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2007). Free classification of regional dialects of American English. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 421438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derwing, T. M., & Munro, M. J. (1997). Accent, intelligibility, and comprehensibility: Evidence from four L1s. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenstein, M. (1982). A study of social variation in adult second language acquisition. Language Learning, 32, 367391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1984). The detection of French accent by American listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 692707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flege, J. E. (1988). Factors affecting degree of perceived foreign accent in English sentences. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84, 7079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W. (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Floccia, C., Butler, J., Girard, F., & Goslin, J. (2009). Categorization of regional and foreign accent in 5-to 7-year-old British children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 366375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Floccia, C., Butler, J., Goslin, J., & Ellis, L. (2009). Regional and foreign accent processing in English: Can listeners adapt? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38, 379412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floccia, C., Goslin, J., Girard, F., & Konopczynski, G. (2006). Does a regional accent perturb speech processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 12761293.Google ScholarPubMed
Fox, R. A., & McGory, J. T. (2007). Second language acquisition of a regional dialect of American English by native Japanese speakers. In Bohn, O. S. & Munro, M. J. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Girard, F., Floccia, C., & Goslin, J. (2008). Perception and awareness of accents in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 409433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes-Harb, R., Smith, B. L., Bent, T., & Bradlow, A. R. (2008). The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit for native speakers of Mandarin: Production and perception of English word-final voicing contrasts. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 664679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ikeno, A., & Hansen, J. H. L. (2007). The effect of listener accent background on accent perception and comprehension. EURASIP Journal on Audio, Speech, and Music Processing, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvella, R. J., Bang, E., Jakobsen, A. L., & Mees, I. M. (2001). Of mouths and men: Non-native listeners’ identification and evaluation of varieties of English. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11, 3756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1257712580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 363B, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W., Ash, S., & Boberg, C. (2006). Atlas of North American English. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladegaard, H. J. (1998). National stereotypes and language attitudes: The perception of British, American and Australian language and culture in Denmark. Language & Communication, 18, 251274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Moss, H. E., & van Halen, S. (1996). Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 13761392.Google ScholarPubMed
Maye, J., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The Weckud Wetch of the Wast: Lexical adaptation to a novel accent. Cognitive Science, 32, 543562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munro, M. J., Derwing, T. M., & Morton, S. L. (2006). The mutual intelligibility of L2 speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 111131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, M., Soli, S. D., & Gelnett, D. J. (1996). Development of the Hearing in Noise Test for Children (HINT-C). Los Angeles: House Ear Institute.Google Scholar
Park, H. (2013). Detecting foreign accent in monosyllables: The role of L1 phonotactics. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 7887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson Education. (2011). VERSANT Test of Spoken English. Retrieved from http://www.versanttest.comGoogle Scholar
Pinet, M., Iverson, P., & Huckvale, M. (2011). Second-language experience and speech-in-noise recognition: Effects of talker–listener accent similarity. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 16531662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sattath, S., & Tversky, A. (1977). Additive similarity trees. Psychometrika, 42, 319345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stibbard, R. M., & Lee, J. I. (2006). Evidence against the mismatched interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit hypothesis. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 433442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 487501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieru, B., Boula de Mareüil, P., & Adda-Decker, M. (2011). Characterisation and identification of non-native French accents. Speech Communication, 53, 292310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, L., Clopper, C. G., & Pate, J. K. (2013). Children's perception of dialect variation. Journal of Child Language. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Witteman, M. J., Weber, A., & McQueen, J. M. (2013). Foreign accent strength and listener familiarity with an accent codetermine speed of perceptual adaptation. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 75, 537556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, X., & Fowler, C. A. (2013). Listening with a foreign-accent: The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit in Mandarin speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 369378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yook, C., & Lindemann, S. (2012). The role of speaker identification in Korean university students’ attitudes towards five varieties of English. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 34, 118.Google Scholar