Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T04:36:34.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Emulation learning: the integration of technical and social cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 December 2009

Ludwig Huber
Affiliation:
Institute of Zoology, University of Vienna, Austria
Chrystopher L. Nehaniv
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Kerstin Dautenhahn
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Get access

Summary

A decade ago, the term emulation had been used by Tomasello (1990) to describe the performance of chimpanzees that seemed to him neither imitative nor a result of simple motivational or attentional influences. However, some researchers have questioned if emulation has been sufficiently well established to be used as a valid alternative in attempts to discover imitation, and whether its definition is precise enough for reliable identification of its operation (Byrne, 1998). As is a common feature of the social learning literature, the term has different meanings in different domains (e.g. Wood, 1989; Whiten and Ham, 1992). Let me first recall what Tomasello and his colleagues (Tomasello et al., 1987; Tomasello, 1990, 1996, 1998) have found and then add some recent evidence, including our findings, for this underestimated learning phenomenon. This endeavour will not only reveal that many findings reported in the literature support the emulation learning account if re-evaluated by recent methodological and theoretical developments (Huang et al., 2002; Want and Harris, 2002; Horowitz, 2003; Whiten et al., 2004; Zentall, 2004), but also that imitation has been dramatically overestimated in terms of occurrence and importance.

In a study by Tomasello et al. (1987) different groups of chimpanzees were presented with three experimental conditions. The first group observed a conspecific demonstrator using a T-bar to rake food items into reach. The second group observed a chimpanzee simply manipulating the tool without a food item being present. The third group did not observe any demonstrator.

Type
Chapter
Information
Imitation and Social Learning in Robots, Humans and Animals
Behavioural, Social and Communicative Dimensions
, pp. 427 - 440
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bugnyar, T. and Huber, L. (1997). Push or pull: an experimental study on imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 54, 817–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. (1998). Comment on Boesch and Tomasello's ‘chimpanzee and human cultures’. Current Anthropology, 39(5), 604–5.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. and Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of information in social learning. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 211–28.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. and Bond, A. B. (1999). Kea, Bird of Paradox. The Evolution and Behavior of a New Zealand Parrot. Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fritz, J., Bisenberger, A. and Kotrschal, K. (2000). Stimulus enhancement in greylag geese (Anser anser): socially mediated learning of an operant task. Animal Behaviour, 59, 1119–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritz, J. and Kotrschal, K. (1999). Social learning in common ravens, Corvus corax. Animal Behaviour, 57(4), 785–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gajdon, G. K., Fijn, N. and Huber, L. (2004). Testing social learning in a wild mountain parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis). Learning and Behavior, 32, 62–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinrich, B. (1995). An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens (Corvus corax). Auk, 112, 994–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. M. and Ray, E. D. (2002). Distinguishing intention-sensitive from outcome-sensitive imitation. Developmental Science, 5, 34–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought. Exploring the origins of thinking. London: Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
Horner, V. and Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, A. C. (2003). Do humans ape? Or do apes human? Imitation and intention in humans (Homo sapiens) and other animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 325–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, C.-T., Heyes, C. M. and Charman, T. (2002). Infants' behavioral reenactment of ‘failed attempts’. Exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 840–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, L. (1998). Movement imitation as faithful copying in the absence of insight. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(5), 694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, L., Rechberger, S. and Taborsky, M. (2001). Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour, 62, 945–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, R. (1999). The Kea (Nestor notabilis): A New Zealand Problem or Problem Solver? Unpublished Master thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1921). The Mentality of Apes. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Klein, E. D. and Zentall, T. R. (2003). Imitation and affordance learning by pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 414–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. (2004). ‘Insightful’ string pulling in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is affected by vocal competence. Animal Cognition, 7, 263–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, K. W. (1937). Experimental studies of learning and higher mental processes in infra-human primates. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 806–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebbich, S., Taborsky, M. and Winkler, H. (1996). Social manipulation causes cooperation in keas. Animal Behaviour, 52, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. (1956). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (eds.), “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 274–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In Galef, B. and Heyes, C. (eds.), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego: Academic Press, 319–46.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1998). Emulation learning and cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 703–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Davis-Dasilva, M., Camak, L. and Bard, K. (1987). Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees. Human Evolution, 2(2), 175–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Völkl, B. and Huber, L. (2000). True imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 60, 195–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Want, S. C. and Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of ‘imitation’ in children. Developmental Science, 5, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werdenich, D. and Huber, L. (2006). A case of quick problem solving in birds: string-pulling in keas (Nestor notabilis). Animal Behaviour, 71(4), 855–63.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Custance, D. M., Gomez, J.-C., Teixidor, P. and Bard, K. A. (1996). Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. and Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In Slater, P. J. B., Rosenblatt, J. S., Beer, C. and Milkinski, M. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior. New York: Academic Press, Vol. 21, 239–83.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A. and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape?Learning and Behavior, 32, 36–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, D. (1989). Social interaction as tutoring. In Bornstein, M. H. and Bruner, J. S. (eds.), Interaction in Human Development. Hove, England: Erlbaum, 59–80.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentall, T. R. (2004). Action imitation in birds. Learning and Behavior, 32, 15–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×