Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:32:18.159Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Deborah J. Curtis
Affiliation:
University of Surrey Roehampton
Joanna M. Setchell
Affiliation:
University of Surrey Roehampton
Joanna M. Setchell
Affiliation:
University of Surrey, Roehampton
Deborah J. Curtis
Affiliation:
University of Surrey, Roehampton
Get access

Summary

The ideal program for the future study of animal behaviour would involve the coordination of field and laboratory investigation.

Schaller (1965, p. 624)

In the early days of expeditions, a naturalist's field equipment consisted of little more than a gun, the means to preserve specimens, and luggage bearers. Later on, field primatologists relied on pencil and paper, binoculars, a compass and, if they were studying nocturnal species, a torch. More recently, a shift in emphasis towards integration of methods has led to collaboration between laboratory and field researchers working on wild primates. Technological advances have presented fieldworkers with the opportunity to collect more sophisticated data, store samples for later laboratory analysis, analyse samples in the field and collect information remotely. This has led to an increase in data concerning (for example): population and group structure; growth, development and physiology; and the application of non-invasive techniques for DNA analyses and hormonal assays. Ecological methods and techniques available for monitoring primate habitats have also improved, with the application of remote sensing, mapping (Global Positioning System, GPS), and data integration (Geographical Information Systems, GIS). These methods open up possibilities for collecting new information on previously studied populations, and a means for collecting data on species that, for example, cannot be habituated for behavioural observations. However, these technological advances come at a time when 64% of primate species are under threat and insufficient data are available to assess the status of a further 11% of species (2000 IUCN Redlist of threatened species).

Type
Chapter
Information
Field and Laboratory Methods in Primatology
A Practical Guide
, pp. 1 - 14
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ancrenaz, M., Lackman-Ancrenaz, I. & Mundy, N. (1994). Field observations of aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) in Madagascar. Folia Primatol. 62, 22–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, L. (1882). Notes on the habits of the aye-aye of Madagascar in its native state. Proc. Royal Soc. London XⅬIII, 639–40Google Scholar
Cheney D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Smuts, B. B. & Wrangham, R. W. (1987). The study of primate societies. In Primate Societies, ed. B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham & T. T. Struhsaker. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press
Curtis, D. J. & Zaramody, A. (1998). Group size, home range use and seasonal variation in the ecology of Eulemur mongoz. Int. J. Primatol. 19, 811–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleagle, J. G. (1999). Primate Adaptation and Evolution. San Diego: Academic Press
Greif, R. & Schmutz, H.-K. (1995). Hans Bluntschli als Morphologe. Gesnerus 52, 133–57Google Scholar
Lamberton, C. (1911). Contribution à l'étude des moeurs du aye-aye. Bull. Acad. Malg. VIII, 129–140Google Scholar
Lavauden, L. (1933). Le aye-aye. La Terre et la Vie Paris 3, 77–9Google Scholar
Lehner, P. N. (1998). Handbook of Ethological Methods, 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Martin, P. & Bateson, P. (1993). Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Owen, R. (1866). On the aye-aye. Chiromys, Cuvier. Trans. Zool. Soc. Lond. 68, 33–101Google Scholar
Peters, W. (1865). Ueber die Säugethiergattung Chiromys. Abh. Akad. Wiss. Berlin, 79–100
Petter, J. J. (1962). Recherches sur l'écologie et l'éthologie des Lémuriens malgaches. Má m. Mus. Nat. Hist. Nat. Sä r. A, Zool. 27, 115–20Google Scholar
Pollen, F. (1863). Enumération des animaux vertébrés de l'île de Madagascar. R. Amsterdam, Nederl. Tijdschr. Dierk. 1, 277–345Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1965). Field procedures. In Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes, ed. I. De Vore, pp. 623–9. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston
Siegel, S. & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioural Sciences, 2nd edition. New York: McGraw-Hill
Sterling, E. J. (1993). Behavioral ecology of the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) on Nosy Mangabe, Madagascar. Ph.D. thesis, University of Yale
Sterling, E. J., Dierenfeld, E. S., Ashbourne, C. J. & Feistner, A. T. C. (1994). Dietary intake, food composition and nutrient intake in wild and captive populations of Daubentonia madagascariensis. Folia Primatol. 62, 115–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strier, K. (2000). Primate Behavioural Ecology. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon
Strum, S. C. & Fedigan, L. M. (2000). Changing views of primate society: a situated North American perspective. In Primate Encounters: Models of Science, Gender and Society, ed. S. C. Strum & L. M. Fedigan, pp. 3–49. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Sussmann, R. W. (1999). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, vol. 1, Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing
Sussmann, R. W. (2000). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, vol. 2, New World Monkeys. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing
Sussmann, R. W. (in press). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, vol. 3, Old World Monkeys and Apes. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing
Vinson, A. (1855). Observations sur un Aye-aye gardé deux mois à la Réunion. C. R. Acad. Sc. XⅬI, 638–40Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×