Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:26:54.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Infant Memory

from Part III - Cognitive Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2020

Jeffrey J. Lockman
Affiliation:
Tulane University, Louisiana
Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Researchers and parents alike have long assumed that experiences that occur during infancy are fundamental to both behavioral and cognitive development. Paradoxically, not only do adults typically fail to recall events that occurred prior to the age of 3, but until the middle part of the twentieth century, there was limited evidence of long-term retention during the infancy period itself. By way of example, although acquiring our first words or taking our first steps was undoubtedly monumental at the time it occurred, we have no conscious recollection of achieving these milestones. In contrast, our memories of other important achievements that took place slightly later in development, like our first day of school or the first time we rode a bike without training wheels, often survive the test of time and eventually form part of our autobiography.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development
Brain, Behavior, and Cultural Context
, pp. 341 - 362
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolph, K. E., & Hoch, J. E. (2019). Motor development: Embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 26.1–26.24.Google Scholar
Anderson, D. I., Campos, J. J., Witherington, D. C., Dahl, A., Rivera, M., He, M., … Barbu-Roth, M. (2013). The role of locomotion in psychological development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 440.Google Scholar
Artioli, F., & Reese, E. (2013). Early memories in young adults from separated and non-separated families, Memory, 22, 10821102Google Scholar
Artioli, F., Reese, E., & Hayne, H. (2015). Benchmarking the past: Children’s early memories and maternal reminiscing as a function of family structure. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4, 136143.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L., & Pickens, J. (1995). Infant memory for object motion across a period of three months: Implications for a four-phase attention function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 343371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnat, S. A., Klein, P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Deferred imitation across changes in context and object: Memory and generalization in 14-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 241251.Google Scholar
Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 205210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, R., Dowden, A., & Hayne, H. (1996). Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6- to 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 159170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 10671081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R., (2000). Age-related changes in imitation: Implications for memory development. In Rovee-Collier, C., Lipsitt, L. P., & Hayne, H. (Eds.), Progress in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 2167). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Barr, R., Muentener, R., Garcia, A., Fujimoto, M., & Chavez, V. (2007). The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 196207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R., Walker, J., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2014). Age-related changes in spreading activation during infancy. Child Development, 85, 549563.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. (2015). A complementary processes account of the development of childhood amnesia and a personal past. Psychological Review, 122, 204231.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J., Wenner, J. A., Dropik, P. L., & Wewerka, S. S. (2000). Parameters of remembering and forgetting in the transition from infancy to early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, 1204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2012). Influence of bilingualism on memory generalisation during infancy. Developmental Science, 15, 812816.Google Scholar
Brito, N., (2014). Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiobiology, 56, 11561163.Google Scholar
Brito, N., Barr, R., McIntyre, P., & Simcock, G. (2012). Long-term transfer of learning from books and video during infanthood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 108119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1989). Contextual gating of memory retrieval. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 533552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005) Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 104110.Google Scholar
Cornell, E. (1979). Infants’ recognition memory, forgetting, and savings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 359374.Google Scholar
Davis, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1983). Alleviated forgetting of a learned contingency in 8-week-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 19, 353365.Google Scholar
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Spelke, E. S. (2015). The infancy of the human brain. Neuron, 88, 93109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagen, J. W., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1983). Memory retrieval: A time-locked process in infancy. Science, 222, 13491352.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., Haden, C., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 15681588.Google Scholar
Freud, S. [1905] (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In Strachey, J. (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7, pp. 125248). London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Gilmore, J. H., Shi, F., Woolson, S. L., Knickmeyer, R. C., Short, S. J., Lin, W., … Shen, D. (2012). Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 24782485.Google Scholar
Greco, C., Hayne, H., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). Roles of function, reminding, and variability in categorization by 3-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 617633.Google Scholar
Greco, C., Rovee-Collier, C., Hayne, H., Griesler, P., & Earley, L. (1986). Ontogeny of early event memory I: Forgetting and retrieval by 2- and 3-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 441460.Google Scholar
Gross, J., Gardiner, B., & Hayne, H. (2016). Developmental reversals in recognition memory in children and adults. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haartsen, R., Jones, E. J. H., & Johnson, M. H. (2016). Human brain development over the early years. Current Opinion in Behavioral Science, 10, 149–54.Google Scholar
Hanna, E., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1993). Peer imitation by toddlers in laboratory, home, and day-care contexts: Implications for social learning and memory. Developmental Psychology, 29, 701710.Google Scholar
Hartshorn, K. (2003). Reinstatement maintains a memory in human infants for 1½ years. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 269282.Google Scholar
Hartshorn, K., Rovee-Collier, C., Gerhardstein, P. C., Bhatt, R. S., Klein, P. J., Aaron, F., … Wurtzel, N. (1998). Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the first year of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 33, 6178.Google Scholar
Hartshorn, K., Rovee-Collier, C., Gerhardstein, P. C, Bhatt, R. S., Wondoloski, T. L., Klein, P., … Campos-de-Carvalho, M. (1998). Ontogeny of long-term memory over the first year and a half of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 32, 6989.Google Scholar
Hayne, H. (1990). The effect of multiple reminders on long-term retention in human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 453477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: Implications for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24, 3373.Google Scholar
Hayne, H. (2006). Age-related changes in infant memory retrieval: Implications for knowledge acquisition. In Munakata, Y. & Johnson, M. H. (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development: Attention and performance XXI (pp. 209231). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayne, H., Barr, R., & Herbert, J. (2003). The effect of prior practice on memory reactivation and generalization. Child Development, 74, 16151627.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., Boniface, J., & Barr, R. (2000). The development of declarative memory in human infants: Age-related changes in deferred imitation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayne, H., & Findlay, N. (1995). Contextual control of memory retrieval in infancy: Evidence for associative priming. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 195207.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., Greco, C., Earley, L. A., Griesler, P. C., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1986). Ontogeny of early event memory II: Encoding and retrieval by 2- and 3-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 461472.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., Gross, J., Hildreth, K., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2000). Repeated reminders increase the speed of memory retrieval by 3-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 3, 312318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayne, H., & Herbert, H. (2004). Verbal cues facilitate memory retrieval during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 127139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. Developmental Science, 6, 254261.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., & Jack, F. (2011). Childhood amnesia. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews in Cognitive Science, 2, 136145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayne, H., Jaeger, K., Sonne, T., & Gross, J. (2016). Visual attention to meaningful stimuli by 1- to 3-year-olds: Implications for the measurement of memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 808816.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., MacDonald, S., & Barr, R. (1997). Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 237249.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., Rovee-Collier, C., & Borza, M. A. (1991). Infant memory for place information. Memory and Cognition, 19, 378386.Google Scholar
Herbert, J., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2007). Crawling is associated with more flexible memory retrieval by 9-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 10, 183189.Google Scholar
Herbert, J., & Hayne, H. (2000a). Memory retrieval by 18–30-month-olds: Age-related changes in representational flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 36, 473484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, J., (2000b). The ontogeny of long-term retention during the second year of life. Developmental Science, 3, 5056.Google Scholar
Hill, W. H., Borovsky, D., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1988). Continuities in infant memory development over the first half-year. Developmental Psychobiology, 21, 4362.Google Scholar
Hunter, M., & Ames, E. (1988). A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. In Rovee-Collier, C. & Lipsitt, L. P. (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 5, pp. 6995). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Imuta, K., Scarf, D., & Hayne, H. (2013). The effect of verbal reminders on memory reactivation in 2-, 3-, and 4-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 49, 10561065.Google Scholar
Jack, F., MacDonald, S., Reese, E., & Hayne, H. (2009). Maternal reminiscing style during early childhood predicts the age of adolescents’ earliest memories. Child Development, 80, 496505.Google Scholar
Jack, F., Simcock, G., & Hayne, H. (2011). Magic memories: Young children’s verbal recall after a 6-year delay. Child Development, 83, 159172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1999). Long-term memory, forgetting, and deferred imitation in 12-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 2, 102113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavenex, P., & Lavenex, P. B. (2013). Building hippocampal circuits to learn and remember: insights into the development of human memory. Behavioral Brain Research, 254, 821.Google Scholar
MacDonald, S., Uesiliana, K., & Hayne, H. (2000). Cross-cultural and gender differences in childhood amnesia. Memory, 8, 365376.Google Scholar
McDonough, L., Mandler, J. M., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1995). The deferred imitation task as a nonverbal measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 92, 75807584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1993). On the development of declarative memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 397404.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). What infant memory tells us about infantile amnesia: Long-term recall and deferred imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 497515.Google Scholar
Morgan, K., & Hayne, H. (2006a). Age-related changes in memory reactivation by 1- and 2-year-old human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 4857.Google Scholar
Morgan, K., (2006b). The effect of encoding time on retention by infants and young children. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 599602.Google Scholar
Morgan, K., (2007). Nonspecific verbal cues alleviate forgetting by young children. Developmental Science, 10, 727733.Google Scholar
Morgan, K., (2011). Age-related changes in visual recognition memory during infancy and early childhood. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 157165.Google Scholar
Morris, G., & Baker-Ward, L. (2007). Fragile but real: Children’s capacity to use newly acquired words to convey preverbal memories. Child Development, 78, 448458.Google Scholar
Morrison, C. M., & Conway, M. A. (2009, July). First words and first memories. Paper presented at the 8th Biennial Meeting of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Kyoto, Japan.Google Scholar
Mullen, M. K. (1994). Earliest recollections of childhood: A demographic analysis. Cognition, 52, 5579.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A. (2000). Neural plasticity and human development: The role of early experience sculpting memory systems. Developmental Science, 3, 115130.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: a call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., Engelhardt, L. E., Brito, N. H., Mack, L. J., Nail, E. J., Angal, J., … Elliott, A. J. (2015). Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive development in the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 57, 535551.Google Scholar
Otto, B. (2018). Language development of infants and toddlers. In Otto, B., Language development in early childhood education (5th ed., pp. 111113). New York, NY: Pearson.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabre-Grenet, M. (1995). Mother’s face recognition by neonates: A replication and an extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 7985.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Grant, V., & Boland, L. (2005). Childhood amnesia in children and adolescents: Their earliest memories. Memory, 13, 622637.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Warren, K. L., & Short, M. M. (2011). Infantile amnesia across the years: A 2-year follow-up of children’s earliest memories. Child Development, 82, 10921105.Google Scholar
Pillemer, D. B., Picariello, M. L., & Pruett, J. C. (1994). Very long-term memories of a salient preschool event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 95106.Google Scholar
Reardon, S. F. (2011). The widening academic-achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations. In Duncan, G. J & Murnane, R. J. (Eds.), Whither opportunity?: Rising inequality, schools, and children’s life chances (pp. 91116). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother–child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403430.Google Scholar
Reese, E., Hayne, H., & MacDonald, S. (2008). Looking back to the future: Māori and Pakeha mother–child birth stories. Child Development, 79, 114125.Google Scholar
Reese, E., Jack, F., & White, N. (2010). Origins of adolescents’ autobiographical memories. Child Development, 25, 352367.Google Scholar
Reynolds, G. (2015). Infant visual attention and object recognition. Behavioral Brain Research, 285, 3443.Google Scholar
Richmond, J., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2007). Interpreting visual preferences in the visual paired-comparison task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 823831.Google Scholar
Richmond, J., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). Accounting for change in declarative memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review, 27, 349373.Google Scholar
Richmond, J., Sowerby, P., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2004). The effect of familiarization time, retention interval, and context change on adult’s performance in the visual paired-comparison task. Developmental Psychobiology, 44, 146155.Google Scholar
Riggins, T., Cheatham, C. L., Stark, E., & Bauer, P. J. (2013). Elicited imitation performance at 20 months predicts memory abilities in school-aged children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 593606.Google Scholar
Robinson, A. J., & Pascalis, O. (2004). Development of flexible visual recognition memory in human infants. Developmental Science, 7, 527533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S. A., Gottfried, A. W., Melloy-Carminar, P., & Bridger, W. H. (1982). Familiarity and novelty preferences in infant recognition memory: Implications for information processing. Developmental Psychology, 18, 704713.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., & Cuevas, K. (2009). Multiple memory systems are unnecessary to account for infant memory development: An ecological model. Developmental Psychology, 45, 160174.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., Griesler, P., & Earley, L. (1985). Contextual determinants of retrieval in three-month-old infants. Learning and Motivation, 16, 139157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. (1987). Reactivation of infant memory: Implications for cognitive development. In Reese, H. W. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 185238). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., Patterson, J., & Hayne, H. (1985). Specificity in the reactivation of infant memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 559574.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., Sullivan, M., Enright, M., Lucas, D., & Fagen, J. W. (1980). Reactivation of infant memory. Science, 208, 11591161.Google Scholar
Seehagen, S., Konrad, C., Herbert, J. S., & Schneider, S. (2015). Timely sleep facilitates declarative memory consolidation in infants. PNAS, 112, 16251629.Google Scholar
Seehagen, S., Zmyj, N., & Herbert, J. S. (2019). Remembering in the context of internal states: The role of sleep for infant memory. Child Development Perspectives, 13, 110115.Google Scholar
Simcock, G., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). The effect of repetition on infants’ imitation from picture books. Infancy, 13, 687697.Google Scholar
Simcock, G., & Hayne, H. (2002). Breaking the barrier: Children do not translate their preverbal memories into language. Psychological Science, 13, 225231.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Spear, N. E., & Parsons, P. J. (1976). Analysis of a reactivation treatment: Ontogenetic determinants of alleviated forgetting. In Medin, D. L., Roberts, W. A., & Davis, R. T., (Eds.), Process of animal memory (pp. 135165). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99(2), 195231.Google Scholar
Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2010). Early childhood matters: Evidence from the effective pre-school and primary education project. London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352373.Google Scholar
Tustin, K., & Hayne, H. (2010). Defining the boundary: Age-related changes in childhood amnesia. Developmental Psychology, 46, 10491061.Google Scholar
Waldfogel, S. (1948). The frequency and affective character of childhood memories. Psychological Monographs, 62, 139.Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2003). Infantile amnesia reconsidered: A cross-cultural analysis. Memory, 11, 6580.Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2006a). Earliest recollections of self and others in European American and Taiwanese young adults. Psychological Science, 17, 708714.Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2006b). Relations of maternal style and child self-concept to autobiographical memories in Chinese, Chinese immigrant, and European American 3-year-olds. Child Development, 77, 17941809.Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Conway, M., & Hou, Y. (2004). Infantile amnesia: A cross-cultural investigation. Cognitive Sciences, 1, 123135.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (n.d.). The global strategy for women’s, children’s and adolescents’ health (2016–2030). Geneva. Retrieved from www.who.int/life-course/partners/global-strategy/en.Google Scholar
Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch-screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 1326.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×