Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T12:49:02.431Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prehension

from Part VII - Motor and related development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2017

Brian Hopkins
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Elena Geangu
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Sally Linkenauger
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Further reading

Jeannerod, M. (1999). Visuomotor channels: Their integration in goal-directed prehension. Human Movement Science, 18, 201218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Sacrey, L.A., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2012). Subsystems of sensory attention for skilled reaching: Vision for transport and pre-shaping and somatosensation for grasping, withdrawal and release. Behavioural Brain Research, 231, 356365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Kelso, J.A.S., & Fogel, A. (1987). Self-organizing systems and infant motor development. Developmental Review, 7, 3965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L.B., Castle, P., & Held, R. (1964). Observations on the development of visually-directed reaching. Child Development, 35, 349364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Butterworth, G., Verweij, E., & Hopkins, B. (1997). The development of prehension in infants: Halverson revisited. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 223236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, R., Muir, K., Ashmead, D.W., & Clarkson, M.G. (1993). Is visually guided reaching in early infancy a myth? Child Development, 64, 10991110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, D., Thurman, S.L., Wiener, R.F., Guan, Y., & Williams, J.L. (2014). Mapping the feel of the arm with the sight of the object: On the embodied origins of infant reaching. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 576.Google Scholar
Foroud, A., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2012). The consummatory origins of visually guided reaching in human infants: A dynamic integration of whole-body and upper-limb movements. Behavioural Brain Research, 231, 343355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadders-Algra, M. (2005). Development of postural control during the first 18 months of life. Neural Plasticity, 12, 99108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadders-Algra, M. (2013). Typical and atypical development of reaching and postural control in infancy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 55, 58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanazawa, H., Kawai, M., Kinai, T., Iwanaga, K., Mima, T., & Heike, T. (2014). Cortical muscle control of spontaneous movements in human neonates. European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 25482553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karl, J.M., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2014). Haptic grasping configurations in early infancy reveal different developmental profiles for visual guidance of the Reach versus the Grasp. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 33013316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karl, J.M., Sacrey, L.A., Doan, J.B., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2012). Hand shaping using hapsis resembles visually guided hand shaping. Experimental Brain Research, 219, 5974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konczak, J., Borutta, M., & Dichgans, J. (1997). The development of goal-directed reaching in infants II: Learning to produce task-adequate patterns of joint torque. Experimental Brain Research, 113, 465474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J.P., Stolze, H., Jöhnk, K., Boczek-Funcke, A., & Illert, M. (1998). Development of prehension movements in children: A kinematic study. Experimental Brain Research, 122, 424432.Google Scholar
Loenneker, T., Klaver, P., Bucher, K., Lichtensteiger, J., Imfeld, A., & Martin, E. (2011). Microstructural development: Organizational differences of the fiber architecture between children and adults in dorsal and ventral visual streams. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 935946.Google Scholar
McCarty, M.E., Clifton, R.K., Ashmead, D.H., Lee, P., & Goubet, N. (2001). How infants use vision for grasping objects. Child Development, 72, 973987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, A.D., & Goodale, M.A. (2006). The visual brain in action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piek, J.P. (2002). The role of variability in early motor development. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 452465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B., Aydin, E., Mason, J., & Schaal, B. (2014). The development of anticipation in the fetus: A longitudinal account of human fetal mouth movements in reaction to and anticipation of touch. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 955963.Google Scholar
Sacrey, L.A., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2010). Development of collection precedes targeted reaching: Resting shapes of the hands and digits in 1–6-month old human infants. Behavioural Brain Research, 214, 125129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacrey, L.A., Karl, J.M., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2012). Development of rotational movements, hand shaping, and accuracy in advance and withdrawal for the reach-to-eat movement in human infants aged 6–12 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 35, 543560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schum, N., Jovanovic, B., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). Ten- and twelve-month-olds’ visual anticipation of orientation and size during grasping. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 218231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., Kamm, K., Spencer, J.P., Schneider, K., & Zernicke, R.F. (1993). The transition to reaching: Mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development, 64, 10581098.Google Scholar
Thomas, B.L., Karl, J.M., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2014). Independent development of the Reach and the Grasp in spontaneous self-touching by human infants in the first six months. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1526.Google Scholar
van Doorn, H., van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G.J. P. (2007). Grasping the Müller–Lyer illusion: The contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia, 45, 19391947.Google Scholar
von Hofsten, C. (1984). Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements. Developmental Psychology, 20, 378388.Google Scholar
von Hofsten, C. (1991). Structuring of early reaching movements: A longitudinal study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 280292.Google Scholar
von Hofsten, C., & Fazel-Zandy, S. (1984). Development of visually guided hand orientation in reaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 208219.Google Scholar
von Hofsten, C., & Rönnqvist, L. (1988). Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 610621.Google ScholarPubMed
Wallace, P.S., & Whishaw, I.Q. (2003). Independent digit movements and precision grip patterns in 1–5-month-old human infants: Hand babbling, including vacuous then self-directed hand and digit movements, precedes targeted reaching. Neuropsychologia, 41, 19121918.Google Scholar
Witherington, D.C. (2005). The development of prospective grasping control between 5 and 7 months: A longitudinal study. Infancy, 7, 143161.Google Scholar
Yamada, Y., Fujii, K., & Kuniyoshi, Y. (2013). Impacts of environment, nervous system and movements of preterms on body map development: Fetus stimulation with spiking neural network. The 3rd joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning-EpiRob. Osaka, Japan.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×