Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T19:53:24.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Risk factors for interpersonal violence: an umbrella review of meta-analyses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Seena Fazel*
Affiliation:
Professor of Forensic Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
E. Naomi Smith
Affiliation:
Registrar in General and Older Adult Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Zheng Chang
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
John Richard Geddes
Affiliation:
Professor of Epidemiological Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
*
Correspondence: Seena Fazel, MD, FRCPsych, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Interpersonal violence is a leading cause of morbidity and mortality. The strength and population effect of modifiable risk factors for interpersonal violence, and the quality of the research evidence is not known.

Aims

We aimed to examine the strength and population effect of modifiable risk factors for interpersonal violence, and the quality and reproducibility of the research evidence.

Method

We conducted an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of risk factors for interpersonal violence. A systematic search was conducted to identify systematic reviews and meta-analyses in general population samples. Effect sizes were extracted, converted into odds ratios and synthesised, and population attributable risk fractions (PAF) were calculated. Quality analyses were performed, including of small study effects, adjustment for confounders and heterogeneity. Secondary analyses for aggression, intimate partner violence and homicide were conducted, and systematic reviews (without meta-analyses) were summarised.

Results

We identified 22 meta-analyses reporting on risk factors for interpersonal violence. Neuropsychiatric disorders were among the strongest in relative and absolute terms. The neuropsychiatric risk factor that had the largest effect at a population level were substance use disorders, with a PAF of 14.8% (95% CI 9.0–21.6%), and the most important historical factor was witnessing or being a victim of violence in childhood (PAF = 12.2%, 95% CI 6.5–17.4%). There was evidence of small study effects and large heterogeneity.

Conclusions

National strategies for the prevention of interpersonal violence may need to review policies concerning the identification and treatment of modifiable risk factors.

Declarations of interest

J.R.G. is an NIHR Senior Investigator. The views expressed within this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Type
Review article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018

Interpersonal violence is among the most important preventable causes of premature mortality and morbidity. Excluding war, it leads to around 410 000 deaths per year and is the 19th most common cause of death globally.Reference Wang, Naghavi, Allen, Barber, Bhutta and Carter1 Morbidity is also substantial, although there are large variations, it is in the top five causes of disability-adjusted living years in central and tropical Latin America, and southern Sub-Saharan Africa.Reference Murray, Barber, Foreman, Ozgoren, Abd-Allah and Abera2 Trends in violence vary depending on the outcome used: decreases in violence-related mortality have been reported from 2000 to 2015,Reference Wang, Naghavi, Allen, Barber, Bhutta and Carter1 whereas morbidity has remained unchanged.Reference Wang, Naghavi, Allen, Barber, Bhutta and Carter1, Reference Murray, Barber, Foreman, Ozgoren, Abd-Allah and Abera2

Public health has moved toward a prevention model for violence,Reference Krug, Mercy, Dahlberg and Zwi3 and influential World Health Organization reports have focused on delineating risk factors.4 Identifying modifiable risk factors could potentially reduce risks and assist in developing interventions. However, these reports are limited by being narrative reviews of the evidence without quantitative methods to evaluate the strength, quality and consistency of risk factors.

To address limitations in previous work and provide an overview, we conducted an umbrella review of the evidence from existing systematic reviews and meta-analyses on risk factors for violence.

Methods

No specific ethical approval was required for this research as it was a synthesis of secondary data from published sources.

Search strategy

The systematic search strategy was prospectively registered on PROSPERO5 (registration number CRD42014010400). The original search incorporated both risk factors for violence and suicide, and this paper reports the violence search. Three databases were searched from their start dates until January 2018: PsycINFO (1 January 1806 to 5 January 2018), Medline (1 January 1946 to 5 January 2018) and Global Health (1 January 1973 to 5 January 2018), supplemented by targeted searches on Google Scholar (1 January 2004 to 5 January 2018) and PubMed (1 January 1996 to 5 January 2018).

Keywords for violence (violen*, crim*, offen*, antisocial and delinq*) were combined with search terms for risk factors (risk, predict*’) and publications (meta*, systematic review). Citations and reference lists of relevant reviews were hand-searched. Targeted searches were used to identify additional studies by first author names and specific risk factors that were not identified in our initial search (including developmental disorders).

Study eligibility

Eligible studies were meta-analyses or systematic reviews that examined risk factors for violence in the general population, and provided effect sizes and data to calculate 95% confidence intervals. We aimed to measure interpersonal violence and included a broad range of violence outcomes, such as assault, violent crime and sexual violence. Although this is a broad scope, we aimed to include only those reviews that used some measure of interpersonal violence as outcome (so that verbal aggression, minor criminality and antisocial behaviour were excluded). Published and unpublished reviews in any language were considered.

Excluded studies were those with methodologies other than a meta-analysis or systematic review, such as individual case–control or cohort studies. As the primary research question was risk factors in the general population, reviews that investigated selected populations, such as prisoners or those with a specific diagnosis, were excluded. Reviews that focused on reoffending risks or those examined interventions for violence were also excluded.Reference Asscher, van Vaugt, Stams, Eichelsheim, Deković and Yousfi6Reference Lipsey8 If more than one eligible review was found on the same risk factor, the most recent one was included.

Data extraction

Data were extracted with a standardised form. Reported effect sizes with 95% confidence intervals were recorded with other key information. Separate effect sizes for gender, the effect size of the largest study included in each meta-analysis and the effect size for the different study designs were extracted. When these data were not recorded, we corresponded directly with authors. Extracted data were independently cross-checked by a post-doctoral researcher (Z.C.), and any queries were resolved by discussion with the project supervisor (S.F.).

Statistical analyses

As the reporting of effect sizes varied between studies (including odds ratios, Cohen's d, correlation coefficients, relative risks and standardised mortality ratios), they were converted to comparable measures. For the primary outcome, all effect sizes were converted to odds ratios (for selected formulae, see Supplementary Appendix 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.145). For those reported as Cohen's d, log-transformed odds ratios were calculated.Reference Douglas, Guy and Hart9Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein11 Effect sizes reported as correlation coefficients were converted first to Cohen's d and then to log-transformed odds ratios. Odds ratios were categorised as follows: weak, 1.0–1.5; moderate, 1.6–2.5; strong, 2.6–9.9 and very strong, ≥10.0.Reference Chesney, Goodwin and Fazel12

Categorisation of risk factors and outcome measures

Risk factors and outcome measures were qualitatively analysed after the search, and common categories were identified. We identified distinct categories of outcome measures (any interpersonal violence, intimate partner violence, sexual violence and homicide) that were reported separately. Meta-analyses with other related outcome measures, such as aggression and hostility, were reported as secondary outcomes in Supplementary Appendix 2.

Population attributable risk fractions

Population attributable risk fractions (PAFs) indicate the proportion of an outcome that would theoretically not occur in a population if a given risk factor was eliminated, assuming causality between risk factor and outcome. We estimated the proportion of cases that could be attributed to each risk factor in the general population (see Supplementary Appendix 1 for formulae). Although causal inferences were not possible for some risk factors, PAFs provide a measure of the maximum possible effect that each risk factor has at a population level by taking into account the risk factor's prevalence.Reference Li, Page, Martin and Taylor13 Thus, if a risk factor has a large effect size but low prevalence, its effect at a population level will be lower than a risk factor with low or moderate effect but a high prevalence.

Tests of quality of evidence

Reviews were assessed for quality by various approaches. First, we scored the Assessing the Methodological Quality of Systematic Reviews (AMSTAR) tool.Reference Shea, Grimshaw, Wells, Boers, Andersson and Hamel14 Scores of 0–3 are considered low, 4–7 are medium and 8–11 are high.Reference Chesney, Goodwin and Fazel12 Second, we compared the effect size for the largest included study in each meta-analysis with the overall quoted meta-analysis effect size. Results where the largest included study effect size (assumed to be the most accurate) was close to the overall meta-analysis effect size were deemed to be more precise.Reference Kirkwood and Sterne15 Third, we calculated ratios between overall meta-analysis effect size and that of the largest included study in each meta-analysis. A meta-analysis overall effect size/largest included study effect size ratio of more than one indicates a larger effect size in the meta-analyses compared with its largest included study, and is an indication of bias.Reference Kavvoura, McQueen, Khoury, Tanzi, Bertram and Ioannidis16 Fourth, a comparison was made between meta-analyses' overall effect size and the number of cases included in each meta-analysis (meta-analyses with large sample sizes were deemed to be more preciseReference Kirkwood and Sterne15), when sufficient data were available. Fifth, we assessed the relationship between study design and effect size. Where sufficient data were available, results were extracted for pooled overall effect sizes of prospective studies alone and compared with overall meta-analysis' effect sizes. Finally, we presented prediction interval calculations for risk factors. Prediction intervals provide an estimate of the ranges in which future observations will fall. Risk factors with prediction intervals that did not cross the null value were deemed to be of higher quality. Those that cross the null value suggest that they may not be significant if tested in a new population.Reference Riley, Higgins and Deeks17 To summarise these quality tests, a scoring system was developed, which also included between-study heterogeneity (with I 2 <50% categorised as low heterogeneity) and whether adequate adjustments for confounders was conducted (see Table 1 for details on the scoring system). All analyses were performed with STATA-IC version 13.

Table 1 Top five risk factors for interpersonal violence ranked by quality of evidence

Scores: Prediction interval excluding null value = 1; P-value <0.05 for random effects model = 1; low heterogeneity (I 2 < 50%) = 1; number of cases > 1000 = 1; no evidence to suggest small study effects = 1; confounders adjusted for = 1.

Results

Twenty-two meta-analyses on risk factors for violence (Supplementary Appendix 3) were identified.Reference Fazel, Lichtenstein, Grann, Goodwin and Långström18Reference Whitaker, Le, Hanson, Baker, McMahon and Ryan39 This included information from over 120 000 individuals from 1139 individual studies across 14 different countries. Risk factors were grouped into broad categories or domains of neuropsychiatric, historic and other. Because of high heterogeneity and non-comparability, results were not further pooled. The largest effect sizes for violence were found in the neuropsychiatric category (Fig. 1), with substance misuse ranking most highly. Antisocial personality disorder had the strongest link to violence within the category of personality disorders.

Fig. 1 Effect sizes of risk factors (identified in meta-analyses) for interpersonal violence, ranked by strength of association and subcategory. Adjusted odds ratios were used when possible.

Some childhood and adolescent factors were important (particularly youth antisocial behaviour). Four meta-analyses examined parental factors that were associated with violenceReference Derzon20, Reference Murray, Farrington and Sekol26, Reference Hoeve, Dubas, Eichelsheim, van der Laan, Smeenk and Gerris31, Reference Pratt, Cullen, Sellers, Winfree, Madensen and Daigle35 (Supplementary Appendix 4). These factors included poor attachment to parents, parental incarceration, antisocial attitudes in parents and more general problems within the family.

Intimate partner violence

Six meta-analyses focused on intimate partner violence.Reference Stith, Smith, Penn, Ward and Tritt32Reference Stith, Rosen, Middleton, Busch, Lunderberg and Carlton37 Two risk factors overlapped with risk factors for any interpersonal violence, namely substance misuse and exposure to violence. Other risk factors for intimate partner violence appeared to be specific to relationships, such as marital dissatisfaction and previous abuse by one partner toward the other (Supplementary Appendix 5).

Sexual violence and homicide

Two reviews provided data for risk factors for sexual violence alone,Reference Jespersen, Lalumière and Seto38, Reference Whitaker, Le, Hanson, Baker, McMahon and Ryan39 and only one review provided separate risk estimates for homicideReference Fazel, Gulati, Linsell, Geddes and Grann21 (Supplementary Appendix 6). Risk factors for sexual violence broadly overlapped with risk factors for any interpersonal violence. Data were more limited for the homicide review although two neuropsychiatric risk factors (schizophrenia and substance misuse) overlapped with interpersonal violence.

Risk factors stratified by gender

Where possible, results were stratified by gender (Supplementary Appendix 7). Effect sizes for women appeared to be larger than for men for all neuropsychiatric violence risk factors.

PAFs

Although PAFs assume causality, they provide an estimate of the maximum possible effect that removing a risk factor could have, and PAFs for individual risk factors may overlap and add up to more than 100%.Reference Rockhill, Newman and Weinberg40 The highest PAFs for violence were substance misuse, witnessing or being a victim of violence in childhood, and personality disorder (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Population attributable fractions (PAFs) of risk factors (identified in meta-analyses) for interpersonal violence.

Other reviews

We identified a further 13 systematic reviews and meta-analyses that provided additional information. For violence, these were for the secondary outcomes of aggression and hostility rather than interpersonal violence (Supplementary Appendix 2). Risk factors for aggression included two main themes: biological factors (serotonin and testosterone levels, heart rate, genetic influences and electrodermal activity) and witnessing violence (e.g. being exposed to television violence and violent videogames). Negative findings included the lack of evidence for candidate genes associated with aggression in a meta-analysis and field synopsis of 185 studies of the field.Reference Vassos, Collier and Fazel41

Quality assessments

Despite mostly high scores on AMSTAR, other analyses found indications of poorer quality. There were small study effects and around 60% of reviews had overall effect sizes larger than the effect size quoted in each meta-analysis' largest included study (Fig. 3; ratios in Supplementary Appendix 8). There was no statistically significant correlation between meta-analyses' overall effect size and the number of cases included in each meta-analysis, when sufficient data were available. Of the 12 included risk factors, seven were found to exclude the null value using prediction intervals (Supplementary Appendix 9).

Fig. 3 A comparison of pooled effect size of included meta-analyses and the effect size of the largest included study in these individual meta-analyses.

Three meta-analyses enabled investigation of study design.Reference Fazel, Gulati, Linsell, Geddes and Grann21, Reference Ttofi, Farrington and Lösel24, Reference Wilson, Stover and Berkowitz25 One review, which examined being bullied as a risk factor, reported a lower pooled effect size for prospective studies (odds ratio 1.8, 95% CI 1.3–2.3 versus overall odds ratio 4.9, 95% CI 2.1–11.2).Reference Ttofi, Farrington and Lösel24 Two other reviews did not find statistically significant differences by study design (one of which examined prospective studies versus case–control designs in schizophrenia,Reference Fazel, Gulati, Linsell, Geddes and Grann21 and the other examined nested case–control versus others in childhood witnessing of violenceReference Wilson, Stover and Berkowitz25).

Overall, using a scoring system (with a maximum of six) based on quality indicators and a threshold of four or above for high-quality studies, seven risk factors for violence met these criteria. None of the risk factors for intimate partner violence or sexual offending met this quality threshold (see Table 1 for top five risk factors based on quality scores; see Supplementary Appendix 10 for a full list and explanation of the scoring system).

Discussion

We have presented an overview of risk factors for interpersonal violence from 22 meta-analyses based on over 120 000 individuals. We have presented associations, PAFs and measures of evidence quality, and investigated risk factors for related outcomes of homicide, intimate partner violence and sexual offending. To our knowledge, this is the first quantitative meta-review of the field. In addition, novel features include bringing together relative risks and estimates of population effect, using tests of methodological quality to determine the strength of the underlying evidence, and the breadth of the outcomes and the ability to compare effect sizes between them.

There were three principal findings. First, based on relative risk, the strongest risk factors were typically in the neuropsychiatric domain. Second, in terms of population effect, there was some overlap with factors that had the strongest relative effects, with substance use disorders, schizophrenia and personality disorders having high PAFs and relative risks. Third, the overall quality of the underlying evidence was not strong, with the majority of reviews demonstrating small study effects and large heterogeneity. By focusing on risk factors, this umbrella review has identified individual-level determinants. Socioeconomic causes of violence will rely on ecological studies that were not included.

A number of implications arise from this work. First, it suggests that many important risk factors for violence are modifiable, and public health can realistically include substantial reductions globally if these factors are confirmed in treatment trials as causal.Reference Norheim, Jha, Admasu, Godal, Hum and Kruk42 Second, violence prevention strategies should incorporate guidelines and targets for the identification, assessment and treatment of psychiatric disorders. However, diagnostic categories themselves are not sole treatment goals, and active symptoms and comorbidities, which mediate the above-reported associations with violence, should also be targeted. Our findings challenge the current view of criminology as a field that appears to under-recognise mental health in the aetiology of violent crime.Reference Farrington, MacKenzie, Sherman and Welsh43 In contrast, this umbrella review found no relevant meta-analyses that were among the top five risk factors in terms of quality for socioeconomic variables, and only one for a psychosocial factor (moral judgement). One possible explanation is that the focus of many included reviews were neuropsychiatric conditions rather than socioeconomic factors. In addition, within the former, the variation in socioeconomic factors is limited, and thus studying their effects will require more general population samples.

At the same time, it should be noted that criminal history variables are among the strongest for individuals with psychiatric disorders, underscoring the need to strengthen the relationship between criminal justice and mental health services to manage future risks. Third, on a population level, antisocial personality disorder is an important risk factor for violence, and more research on links between such disorders and these outcomes is warranted. Although little evidence exists to suggest that the underlying personality disorders are treatable, some common symptoms arising from them are modifiable.44 Another risk factor identified, which has been less widely discussed, is witnessing or being a victim of violence in childhood. The mechanism for how this contributes to adult violence perpetration needs examination, and may provide targets for intervention. Nevertheless, it suggests that interventions in childhood and adolescence for antisocial behaviour should consider any such history and broaden treatments for victims to include children who have witnessed violence. Finally, research should focus on longitudinal studies, investigate sources of heterogeneity and improve adjustment for confounding. Sibling controls are one powerful approach to do so,Reference D'Onofrio, Lahey, Turkheimer and Lichtenstein45 and can provide important evidence as they account for familial confounding (early environmental and genetic factors). Ultimately, strong evidence of causal inference for identified risk factors will need to be tested in trials. However, many trials in this area may not be feasible for practical and ethical reasons, and quasi-experimental designs (such as observational studies using family designs and natural experiments) will play an important role in developing the evidence base.

Limitations of the current meta-review include the possibility that the included meta-analyses have been superseded by more recent, high-quality individual studies. For example, the reviews on traumatic brain injury and schizophrenia are from 2009.Reference Fazel, Gulati, Linsell, Geddes and Grann21, Reference Fazel, Philipson, Gardiner, Merritt and Grann22 However, both of these have been confirmed by more recent, large population-based studies.Reference Sariaslan, Larsson and Fazel46 In relation to traumatic brain injury, a large Swedish population and sibling comparison investigation found robust links with violent crime after adjustment for sociodemographic confounders,Reference Fazel, Lichtenstein, Grann and Långström47 and an Australian study also found a link when violent crime (as opposed to any crime) was used as an outcome (with additional adjustment for previous criminality).Reference Schofield, Malacova, Preen, D'Este, Tate and Reekie48 In addition, how violence was operationalised was necessarily heterogeneous, reflecting the lack of a consensus in the field for the best outcome.Reference Chambers, Yiend, Barrett, Burns, Helen and Fazel49 Importantly, although these will alter prevalence of outcomes, they does not appear to affect risk estimates as the prevalence of outcomes is consistently reported in the cases (subgroups defined by exposure to a particular risk factor) and general population controls.

How might treatment reduce violence? One approach is simply to target and treat underlying psychiatric disorders as well as symptoms and other mediators of risk. Randomised controlled trials provide little evidence for this approach as they are not usually powered or designed to investigate rare outcomes. Observational data provide stronger support for antipsychotic medication reducing violence risk,Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, Långström, Larsson and Fazel50 and are important sources of evidence when randomised controlled trials are not feasible. For example, clozapine may have specific violence-reducing effectsReference Frogley, Taylor, Dickens and Picchioni51 and psychological therapies that specifically target aggression could also be considered. There is some evidence for structured group therapy in drug-using offenders to prevent reoffending.Reference Perry, Neilson, Martyn-St James, Glanville, Woodhouse and Godfrey52 Screening for violence risk in selected populationsReference Fazel, Chang, Fanshawe, Långström and Lichtenstein53 needs further research to clarify its potential role, including use of trial methodology. Targeting high-risk groups, such as released prisoners and individuals with antisocial personality disorder, should be prioritised for future intervention research. Treatments in childhood and adolescence require improvement.Reference Farrington, Gaffney and Ttofi54 In addition, preventative approaches should be developed to address the potential importance of the two childhood risk factors that we have identified: being bullied and witnessing or experiencing violence.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.145.

Funding

S.F. is funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant #202836/Z/16/Z). The funding source had no involvement in any aspect of the study.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr Rongqin Yu for assistance with updating our systematic search to January 2018 and to Mr Alex Horn for technical support.

References

1Wang, H, Naghavi, M, Allen, C, Barber, RM, Bhutta, Z, Carter, A, et al. Global, regional, and national life expectancy, all-cause mortality, and cause-specific mortality for 249 causes of death, 1980–2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet 2015; 388: 1459–544.Google Scholar
2Murray, CJ, Barber, RM, Foreman, KJ, Ozgoren, AA, Abd-Allah, F, Abera, SF, et al. Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 306 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 188 countries, 1990–2013: quantifying the epidemiological transition. Lancet 2015; 386: 2145–91.Google Scholar
3Krug, EG, Mercy, JA, Dahlberg, LL, Zwi, AB. The world report on violence and health. Lancet 2002; 360: 1083–8.Google Scholar
4 World Health Organization (WHO). Global Status Report on Violence Prevention 2014. WHO, 2014 (http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/violence/status_report/2014/en/).Google Scholar
5Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. PROSPERO. University of York, 2011 (http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/).Google Scholar
6Asscher, JJ, van Vaugt, ES, Stams, GJ, Eichelsheim, V, Deković, M, Yousfi, S. The relationship between juvenile psychopathic traits, delinquency and (violent) recidivism: a meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52: 1134–43.Google Scholar
7Gutierrez, L, Wilson, HA, Rugge, T, Bonta, J. The prediction of recidivism with aboriginal offenders: a theoretically informed meta-analysis. Can J Criminol Criminal Justice 2013; 55: 5599.Google Scholar
8Lipsey, MW. The primary factors that characterize effective interventions with juvenile offenders: a meta-analytic overview. Vict Offenders 2009; 4: 124–47.Google Scholar
9Douglas, KS, Guy, LS, Hart, SD. Psychosis as a risk factor for violence to others: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2009; 135: 679706.Google Scholar
10Zhang, J, Yu, KF. What's the relative risk? A method of correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes. JAMA 1998; 18: 1690–1.Google Scholar
11Borenstein, M, Hedges, LV, Higgins, JP, Rothstein, HR. Introduction to Meta-Analysis: Converting among Effect Sizes. Wiley, 2009.Google Scholar
12Chesney, E, Goodwin, GM, Fazel, S. Risks of all-cause and suicide mortality in mental disorders: a meta-review. World Psychiatry 2014; 13: 153–60.Google Scholar
13Li, Z, Page, A, Martin, G, Taylor, R. Attributable risk of psychiatric and socio-economic factors for suicide from individual-level, population-based studies: a systematic review. Soc Sci Med 2011; 72: 608–16.Google Scholar
14Shea, BJ, Grimshaw, JM, Wells, GA, Boers, M, Andersson, N, Hamel, C, et al. Development of AMSTAR: a measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews. BMC Med Res Methodol 2007; 7: 1.Google Scholar
15Kirkwood, BR, Sterne, JA. Essential Medical Statistics (2nd edn). Blackwell Publishing, 2000.Google Scholar
16Kavvoura, FK, McQueen, MB, Khoury, MJ, Tanzi, RE, Bertram, L, Ioannidis, JP. Evaluation of the potential excess of statistically significant findings in published genetic association studies: application to Alzheimer's disease. Am J Epidemiol 2008; 168: 855–65.Google Scholar
17Riley, RD, Higgins, JP, Deeks, JJ. Interpretation of random effects meta-analyses. BMJ 2011; 342: 549.Google Scholar
18Fazel, S, Lichtenstein, P, Grann, M, Goodwin, GM, Långström, N. Bipolar disorder and violent crime: new evidence from population-based longitudinal studies and systematic review. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 931–8.Google Scholar
19Yu, R, Geddes, JR, Fazel, S. Personality disorders, violence, and antisocial behaviour: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. J Pers Disord 2012; 26: 775–92.Google Scholar
20Derzon, JH. The correspondence of family features with problem, aggressive, criminal, and violent behaviour: a meta-analysis. J Exp Criminol 2010; 6: 263–92.Google Scholar
21Fazel, S, Gulati, G, Linsell, L, Geddes, JR, Grann, M. Schizophrenia and violence: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med 2009; 6: 8.Google Scholar
22Fazel, S, Philipson, J, Gardiner, L, Merritt, R, Grann, M. Neurological disorders and violence: a systematic review and meta-analysis with a focus on epilepsy and traumatic brain injury. J Neurol 2009; 256: 1591–602.Google Scholar
23Pratt, TC, Cullen, FT, Blevins, KR, Daigle, L, Unnever, FD. The relationship of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder to crime and delinquency: a meta-analysis. Int J Police Sci Manage 2002; 4: 344–60.Google Scholar
24Ttofi, MM, Farrington, DP, Lösel, F. School bullying as a predictor of violence later in life: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Aggress Violent Behav 2012; 17: 405–18.Google Scholar
25Wilson, HW, Stover, CS, Berkowitz, SJ. Research review: the relationship between childhood violence exposure and juvenile antisocial behaviour: a meta-analytic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50: 769–79.Google Scholar
26Murray, J, Farrington, DP, Sekol, I. Children's antisocial behavior, mental health, drug use, and educational performance after parental incarceration: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2012; 138: 175.Google Scholar
27Derzon, JH. Antisocial behaviour and the prediction of violence: a meta-analysis. Psychol Schools 2001; 38: 93106.Google Scholar
28Stams, GJ. The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2006; 34: 692708.Google Scholar
29Morgan, AB, Lilienfeld, SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev 2000; 20: 113–36.Google Scholar
30Jolliffe, D, Farrington, DP. Empathy and offending: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggress Violent Behav 2004; 9: 441–76.Google Scholar
31Hoeve, M, Dubas, JS, Eichelsheim, VI, van der Laan, PH, Smeenk, W, Gerris, JR. The relationship between parenting and delinquency: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2009; 37: 749–75.Google Scholar
32Stith, SM, Smith, DB, Penn, CE, Ward, DB, Tritt, D. Intimate partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: a meta-analytic review. Aggress Violent Behav 2004; 10: 6598.Google Scholar
33Stith, SM, Green, NM, Smith, DB, Ward, DB. Marital satisfaction and marital discord as risk markers for intimate partner violence: a meta-analytic review. J Fam Violence 2008; 23: 149–60.Google Scholar
34Gil-Gonzalez, D, Vives-Cases, C, Alvarez-Dardet, C, Latour-Pérez, J. Alcohol and intimate partner violence: do we have enough information to act? Eur J Pub Health 2006; 16: 278–84.Google Scholar
35Pratt, TC, Cullen, FT, Sellers, CS, Winfree, LT, Madensen, TD, Daigle, LE. The empirical status of social learning theory: a meta-analysis. Justice Quarterly 2010; 27: 765802.Google Scholar
36Moore, TM, Stuart, GL, Meehan, JC, Rhatigan, DL, Hellmuth, JC, Keen, SM. Drug abuse and aggression between intimate partners: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2008; 28: 247–74.Google Scholar
37Stith, S, Rosen, SM, Middleton, KH, Busch, KA, Lunderberg, AL, Carlton, K. The intergenerational transmission of spouse abuse: a meta-analysis. J Marriage Fam 2000; 62: 640–54.Google Scholar
38Jespersen, AF, Lalumière, ML, Seto, MC. Sexual abuse history among adult sex offenders and non-sex offenders: a meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl 2009; 33: 179–92.Google Scholar
39Whitaker, DJ, Le, B, Hanson, R, Baker, CK, McMahon, PM, Ryan, G, et al. Risk factors for the perpetration of child sexual abuse: a review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl 2008; 32: 529–48.Google Scholar
40Rockhill, B, Newman, B, Weinberg, C. Use and misuse of population attributable fractions. Am J Public Health 1998; 88: 15–9.Google Scholar
41Vassos, E, Collier, DA, Fazel, S. Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies of violence and aggression. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19: 471–7.Google Scholar
42Norheim, OF, Jha, P, Admasu, K, Godal, T, Hum, RJ, Kruk, ME, et al. Avoiding 40% of the premature deaths in each country, 2010–30: review of national mortality trends to help quantify the UN sustainable development goal for health. Lancet 2015; 23: 239–52.Google Scholar
43Farrington, DP, MacKenzie, DL, Sherman, LW, Welsh, BC. Evidence-Based Crime Prevention. Routledge, 2003.Google Scholar
44National Institute of Clinical Excellence (NICE). Guidelines - Personality Disorders: Borderline and Antisocial 2016. NICE, 2016 (www.nice.org.uk/guidance/qs88).Google Scholar
45D'Onofrio, BM, Lahey, BB, Turkheimer, E, Lichtenstein, P. Critical need for family-based, quasi-experimental designs in integrating genetic and social science research. Am J Public Health 2013; 103: 4655.Google Scholar
46Sariaslan, A, Larsson, H, Fazel, S. Genetic and environmental determinants of violence risk in psychotic disorders: a multivariate quantitative genetic study of 1.8 million Swedish twins and siblings. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21: 12511256.Google Scholar
47Fazel, S, Lichtenstein, P, Grann, M, Långström, N. Risk of violent crime in individuals with epilepsy and traumatic brain injury: a 35-year Swedish population study. PLoS Med 2011; 8: 12.Google Scholar
48Schofield, PW, Malacova, E, Preen, DB, D'Este, C, Tate, R, Reekie, J, et al. Does traumatic brain injury lead to criminality? A whole-population retrospective cohort study using linked data. PLoS One 2015; 10: 7.Google Scholar
49Chambers, JC, Yiend, J, Barrett, B, Burns, T, Helen, D, Fazel, S, et al. Outcome measures used in forensic mental health research: a structured review. Crim Behav Ment Health 2009; 19: 927.Google Scholar
50Chang, Z, Lichtenstein, P, Långström, N, Larsson, H, Fazel, S. Association between prescription of major psychotropic medications and violent reoffending after prison release. JAMA 2016; 316: 1798–807.Google Scholar
51Frogley, C, Taylor, D, Dickens, G, Picchioni, MA. Systematic review of the evidence of clozapine's anti-aggressive effects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2012; 15: 121.Google Scholar
52Perry, AE, Neilson, M, Martyn-St James, M, Glanville, JM, Woodhouse, R, Godfrey, C, et al. Interventions for drug-using offenders with co-occurring mental illness. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2015; (6): CD010901.Google Scholar
53Fazel, S, Chang, Z, Fanshawe, T, Långström, N, Lichtenstein, P. Prediction of violent reoffending on release from prison: derivation and external validation of a scalable tool. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3: 535–43.Google Scholar
54Farrington, DP, Gaffney, H, Ttofi, MM. Systematic reviews of explanatory risk factors for violence offending and delinquency. Aggress Viol Behav 2016; 33: 2436.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Top five risk factors for interpersonal violence ranked by quality of evidence

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Effect sizes of risk factors (identified in meta-analyses) for interpersonal violence, ranked by strength of association and subcategory. Adjusted odds ratios were used when possible.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Population attributable fractions (PAFs) of risk factors (identified in meta-analyses) for interpersonal violence.

Figure 3

Fig. 3 A comparison of pooled effect size of included meta-analyses and the effect size of the largest included study in these individual meta-analyses.

Supplementary material: File

Fazel et al. supplementary material

Appendices 1-10

Download Fazel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 286.3 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.