Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T07:53:14.687Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Unemployment

from Part III - How Our Experience Affects Our Wellbeing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2023

Richard Layard
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Jan-Emmanuel De Neve
Affiliation:
University of Oxford

Summary

The unemployed are generally significantly and substantially less satisfied with their lives than the employed. This relationship tends to be stronger in high-income countries where there are sharper differences between employment and unemployment. In studies that look at within-person changes over time, unemployment typically reduces wellbeing by at least 0.6 points (out of 10).

Studying plant closures allows researchers to distinguish between endogenous and exogenous effects of unemployment. Workers who lose their jobs due to reasons outside of their control are generally more dissatisfied, although the effect of job loss remains negative and statistically significant for both groups.

The negative wellbeing impacts of unemployment can also spillover onto the general population – and this cause more total loss of wellbeing than the direct effect on the unemployed. Longer periods of unemployment can have scarring effects with long-lasting negative implications for wellbeing even after those affected have returned to work.

The psychosocial effects of unemployment on wellbeing are greater than the effect of lost income. Policy approaches targeting unemployment are therefore likely to be most conducive to wellbeing if they are able to protect and provide for the psychological and social benefits of work, as opposed to simply providing income support.

Type
Chapter
Information
Wellbeing
Science and Policy
, pp. 166 - 177
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

The insupportable labour of doing nothing.

Sir Richard Steele

Introduction

In the next two chapters, we turn to the issue of work: do people have it and do they like it? Unemployment is another major cause of low wellbeing (see Chapter 8). It damages the individual and it often damages their family. And a high unemployment rate causes anxiety throughout the population. It also reduces the aggregate income of the community. So in this chapter, we ask four main questions:

  • How does unemployment affect the unemployed individual?

  • Why is unemployment so painful?

  • How does high unemployment affect the rest of the community?

  • What policies can reduce equilibrium unemployment?

How Important Is Work?

To begin answering this question, we can look at average differences in wellbeing between people according to their employment status. In Figure 11.1, these differences are plotted for six large countries using data from the Gallup World Poll. Here, we consider differences in life satisfaction between adults employed full-time, part-time, self-employed, underemployed, unemployed, and out of the labour force. In this case, ‘underemployed’ means working part-time but wanting to work full-time, and ‘out of the labour force’ means not having a job and not actively looking for one. This last category is mainly composed of homemakers, early retirees, students and those unable to work due to disability.

Figure 11.1 Average life satisfaction (0–10) by employment status

Note: 95% confidence intervals displayed.

Source: Gallup World Poll 2005–2019, Cantril ladder, adults 18–65.

As Figure 11.1, shows, unemployed people are less happy on average than employed people in every country. The crude difference is over 1 point (out of 10) in the United States and the UK and rather less in poorer countries. This is partly because employment classifications become less meaningful in lower-income countries. In Africa, 85% of all employment is informal. In Asia, this figure is roughly 70%.Footnote 1 In countries without welfare states or labour protections, the concept of unemployment itself becomes much harder to define. Yet even in these regions, wellbeing differences between working and non-working adults tend to remain statistically significant.

However, looking at raw differences alone can be misleading. Averages can tell us about the distribution of happiness in a population but much less about its underlying causes. There may be any number of confounding variables that complicate the story. Relative to those who work full-time, the unemployed are, for example, more likely to be young, female and without a college education.Footnote 2 All of these other differences can independently affect wellbeing. If we fail to account for them, we risk misattributing differences in happiness to differences in employment status rather than to other personal characteristics.

In the first wave of empirical wellbeing research, many researchers attempted to address this problem using cross-sectional regressions. These models typically take the form:

Wi=α0+α1Employmenti+α2Xi+ei.(1)

Here, wellbeing is treated as a continuous variable and modelled as a function of employment status and a vector of controls. The coefficients α2 represent average differences in wellbeing attributable to varying demographic characteristics including income, education, marital status, age and so on. The coefficient α1 then estimates the extent to which any remaining variation in wellbeing can be explained by differences in employment status. In other words, α1 measures the psychic impact of unemployment.

Using this approach, Helliwell analysed global data from the World Values Survey. In this case, jobless adults were found to be 0.6 points less satisfied with their lives than full-time employees on a 10-point scale, other things equal.Footnote 3 By contrast, a halving of income (which unemployed people might also experience) would have a smaller effect (see Chapter 13).

But cross-sectional estimates produced by OLS are still only capable of telling us about average between-person differences. Even with the addition of control variables, there are still two important potential sources of bias to consider:

  • Omitted variables: For example, happiness can be influenced by unmeasured genetic or personality traits:Footnote 4 those who become unemployed may simply be predisposed to be unhappy.

  • Reverse causality: Happiness itself affects labour market outcomes.Footnote 5 If unhappiness precedes unemployment, it would be a mistake to conclude that the latter causes the former.

To counter these biases, researchers look at changes in happiness experienced by workers before, during and after becoming unemployed using fixed-effects regressions. Instead of comparing adults with jobs to adults without them, fixed-effects regressions estimate the effect of unemployment by comparing people who become unemployed to their former selves. Running these types of analyses requires panel data in which the same people are surveyed multiple times over a given period of time. These models typically take the form:

Wit=α0+α1Employmentit+α2Xit+fi+eit.(2)

Here, the wellbeing of an individual i at time t is modelled as a function of employment status and control variables. However, in this case, fi is introduced to capture unobserved time-invariant individual effects, like genetic or personality traits. As a result, we are no longer considering between-person differences but rather within-person changes. The coefficients for all variables included on the right-hand side of the equation then represent the effect of transitioning from one state to another – for example, employed to unemployed, childless to parent, single to married. In this way, we can estimate the wellbeing impact of changes in life circumstances from one period to the next.

Early versions of this approach were presented by the economists Liliana and Rainer Winkelmann, using large-scale representative data from the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP).Footnote 6 The authors found that unemployment lowered life satisfaction by roughly 1-point on a scale from 0 to 10. To put this effect into context, it is roughly analogous to the drop in happiness associated with becoming widowed.Footnote 7

To see how this works over time, the effect of unemployment on life satisfaction is plotted for German workers using SOEP data in Figure 11.2.Footnote 8 These effects are estimated using fixed-effects regressions controlling for age, nationality, level of education, income, number of children, health and marital status. Measures of life satisfaction are then normalised relative to a baseline level recorded five years prior to workers losing their jobs. For both men and women, unemployment substantially reduces life satisfaction on top of the effect through lost income. The negative effect of unemployment is roughly 30% larger for men than women, a trend generally reflected in the literature.Footnote 9 Importantly, workers who remain unemployed for longer periods of time struggle to improve their happiness. Even after four years, men and women who lose their jobs are still as unhappy as when they first became unemployed.

Figure 11.2 Effect of unemployment on life satisfaction (0–10) over time (Germany)

Note: Estimated using fixed-effects (within-person) regressions. Controls included for age, nationality, education, income, number of children, health and marital status. Levels are normalised relative to the baseline happiness level recorded five years before becoming unemployed; 95% confidence intervals displayed.

Source: De Neve and Ward (Reference De Neve, Ward, Helliwell, Layard and Sachs2017); SOEP data

Broadly similar results have been found in Britain, the United States and AustraliaFootnote 10 – as well as Russia,Footnote 11 South KoreaFootnote 12 and Switzerland.Footnote 13 The psychic effect of unemployment is large and there is little adaptation. Given the high degree of adaptation observed in response to many other life events, the lack of adaptation to losing a job is notable.Footnote 14 In fact, when the passage of time is taken into account, the cumulative negative effect of long-term unemployment is greater than the long-term impact of becoming married, divorced, widowed or having children.Footnote 15

But does the impact of unemployment differ between workers who choose to leave their jobs and those who become unemployed for reasons outside their control? While quitting is an endogenous driver of unemployment, redundancy is exogenous. So it is interesting to look at the two groups separately. An analysis of this type in Germany found that workers who lost their jobs due to company closures experienced declines in wellbeing (0.8 points) that were larger than those who resigned from their jobs (0.6 points), although the effect for both groups was statistically significant.Footnote 16 At the same time, self-employed workers who had to shut down their business experienced the largest declines overall (1.5 points).

Given this weight of evidence, the substantial negative impact of unemployment on wellbeing is widely regarded to be one of the largest and most robust findings to emerge from empirical happiness research.

Scarring

But is that all, or do unemployed people continue to have reduced wellbeing even after finding new jobs? In some studies, unemployment has been shown to have lingering effects on wellbeing after returning to work. In a seminal analysis, a team of researchers examined the effect of the fraction of the previous five years that had been spent in unemployment. For every year that a person had been unemployed in the previous five years, people were on average 0.1 points (out of 10) less happy.Footnote 17 Taking a longer-term perspective, two studies of British workers in the United Kingdom find that spells of youth unemployment predict lower levels of life satisfaction well into adulthood.Footnote 18 These results again remain significant after controlling for a host of personal, parental and childhood characteristics. Along similar lines, young people who come of age during a recession (rather than a boom) care more highly in later life about their financial security.Footnote 19

What are we to make of these effects? One possible interpretation is that workers who spend longer periods of time unemployed become more insecure about losing their jobs in the future. By this account, it would be job insecurity itself that drives down wellbeing. Some authors have noted that once feelings of job insecurity are accounted for, the effect of past unemployment on wellbeing does become much weaker.Footnote 20 However, in a more recent test, a team of researchers have studied retired people, for whom job insecurity is not an issue. They found that people who retired from a position of involuntary unemployment are more dissatisfied with their lives than those who retired straight from work.Footnote 21 This effect goes beyond what may be expected from losses in retirement income and looks like a direct effect on mood and outlook.

Why Is It So Painful To Be Unemployed?

But why is unemployment so painful? One obvious answer might be the loss of income. But we have already taken this into account. And in fact we can easily compare the size of the non-pecuniary effects with those of the pecuniary effects.Footnote 22 These comparisons have been done by a number of authors, all of whom found the non-pecuniary effects to be more than the pecuniary effects. For example, one widely cited analysis found them to be twice as large, and this is a typical estimate.Footnote 23 So the costs of unemployment go far beyond the income loss.

The seeds of this realisation were planted in the academic literature as far back as 1933. That year, a team of sociologists led by the husband-and-wife team of Paul Felix Lazarsfeld and Marie Jahoda published the findings of an extensive field experiment in the Austrian town of Marienthal, following a plant closure that left most of town unemployed. At the time, Austria offered generous unemployment insurance, providing workers who lost their jobs with considerable financial benefits. Yet rather than experiencing gains in wellbeing as a result of more leisure time, affected workers became increasingly despondent. Social and community life in the town quickly disintegrated. The researchers concluded that employment is not simply a pathway to income but rather something that ‘imposes a time structure on the waking day, implies regularly shared experiences and contacts with people outside the nuclear family, links individuals to goals and purposes that transcend their own, enforces activity, and defines aspects of personal status and identity’.Footnote 24

Many decades later, modern theoretical understandings of employment continue to focus on three related channels through which work relates to wellbeing: (1) identity, (2) social network and (3) routine.Footnote 25 We will dive into the empirical evidence for these channels in greater detail later in Chapter 12.

Spillovers on the Community

The family

If unemployment changes the unemployed individual, it also damages the rest of the family and the wider community. Partners of workers who lose their jobs suffer declines in wellbeing. One early study observed significant declines in female partner’s life satisfaction following their spouse’s job loss – of the order of 0.5 points (out of 10) – although similar effects were not observed for male partners.Footnote 26 More recently, a study looking at unemployment following plant closures in Germany again found that cohabitating partners of unemployed workers experienced significant declines in wellbeing. On average, the spillover effect of unemployment for partners was roughly one fourth of the direct effect on the worker. These negative impacts were largely similar for men and women – roughly 0.3 points on a 0 to 10-point scale.Footnote 27 Related studies in the United Kingdom,Footnote 28 AustraliaFootnote 29 and GermanyFootnote 30 have found analogous declines in the mental health of spouses following partners’ entry into unemployment

Spells of parental unemployment can also have negative effects on the children’s wellbeing. These effects are generally small but tend to be particularly significant when parental unemployment is experienced when you are in your teens. In one of the first studies conducted along these lines in the United Kingdom, the authors found mostly insignificant effects of parental unemployment on happiness for children under 12 years of age. But the effects turn significant for older children – among 15-year-olds, father’s unemployment produces a decline in happiness of 0.4 points (out of 7), while mother’s unemployment produces a decline of 1 point.Footnote 31

What about the longer-term effects of parental unemployment? Only a few studies have investigated this. One noted that 18- to 31-year-olds who experienced spells of parental employment as a result of plant closures as young children (0–5) or in adolescence (11–15) reported lower levels of life satisfaction than counterparts whose parents remained employed, controlling for other factors. The magnitude of this effect was about 0.6 points (out of 10).Footnote 32 Similarly, another study found that adult wellbeing is lower for people whose parents were unemployed, especially if those parents were from more privileged backgrounds (where the shock is greater).Footnote 33

The community

The final – and most important spillover from unemployment – is on the population at large. High unemployment makes everyone feel less secure, even if they have a job. For, if unemployment is high and you lose your job, you will find it more difficult to get another one. Table 11.1 reports the results of a cross-sectional analysis of data from four countries. Life satisfaction of person i is regressed on:

  • first, whether the individual person i is unemployed, and

  • second, the regional unemployment rate (expressed as a proportion).

As can be seen, the coefficient on own employment is less than the coefficient on the regional unemployment rate.

So how does average wellbeing in a region change when average unemployment in the region changes? The wellbeing of individual i in region r is given by

Wir=a0+a1Uir+a2U¯r+etc(3)

and the average wellbeing in region r is therefore given by

W¯r=a0+a1+a2U¯r+etc.(4)

In all our countries α1 < α2. This means that, when unemployment rises, the total loss of wellbeing is higher among employed people than among those who are newly unemployed.Footnote 34

Table 11.1 How life satisfaction (0–10) is affected by your own unemployment and by the regional unemployment rate (Household data, cross-section)

Own unemployment rate (1 or 0)Regional unemployment rate (0–1)
Britain−0.71 (.09)−1.38 (.56)
Germany−0.96 (.07)−1.58 (.36)
Australia−0.35 (.11)−0.37 (.42)
USA−0.45 (.06)−1.44 (.47)
Source: A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) Table 4.4; slightly adapted; Understanding Society (Britain), SOEP (Germany), HILDA (Australia) and BRFSS (United States); data for many years pooled with year dummies and usual controls

Policy Implications

Clearly, we would like to reduce unemployment to the lowest level compatible with stable inflation.Footnote 35 There are two main practical issues.

  • The approach to redundancy.

  • Active labour market policy to stimulate employment.

Redundancy

As we have seen, a high unemployment rate reduces the wellbeing of the unemployed and of workers.Footnote 36 Moreover, worker wellbeing (and hence productivity) is increased by a sense of job security. This creates a presumption in favour of adjusting to shocks through reduced hours or furlough (where workers do not lose their jobs) rather than through redundancy. A test case of this choice arose in the COVID-19 pandemic.

Broadly speaking, high-income countries opted for one of two approaches to the downturn – either job retention or income replacement. Job retention policies aim to maintain employment contracts by subsidising firms to keep workers on the staff, while income replacement policies generally focus on providing financial relief for workers who lost their jobs.Footnote 37 Taking a wellbeing perspective, we may expect the former approach to be preferable to the latter. Unlike income replacement schemes, policies aimed at keeping workers in their jobs are better poised to keep the non-pecuniary benefits of work intact. While empirical research on the topic is still emerging, countries favouring job retention policies did in fact see both lower levels of unemployment and less severe declines in wellbeing in the first year of the crisis.Footnote 38

A parallel issue arises even in terms of economic stability. Some countries have stricter laws to discourage redundancy than others. Clearly, such laws reduce the number of workers who get fired, but they also reduce the number of new hires that employers are willing to take on. On balance these effects probably cancel out, and employment protection has little effect on aggregate unemployment.Footnote 39

Active labour market policies

But some people inevitably lose their jobs. What then becomes crucial is whether or not they drift into long-term unemployment, where their chances of re-employment deteriorate sharply. For a key issue of equity and efficiency is how to prevent long-term unemployment. It may thus be important to shorten spells of unemployment. To this end, active labour market policies (ALMPs) have shown effective macroeconomic results.Footnote 40 ALMPs include (a) subsidised hiring of unemployed workers, (b) training programmes (on and off the job) and (c) job-search assistance for the unemployed. Many of these interventions have now been evaluated using properly controlled methods. A meta-analysis of these studies found that on average they raised the probability of being in employment after the end of the programme by 2 percentage points in the short-term, rising to 9 points in the long-run.Footnote 41 Subsidised employment was the most effective policy and those who benefitted most from this were those who were already long-term unemployed. But within each type of programme, there was a wide spread of results, depending on the effectiveness of the design. There is also the issue of whether helping unemployed people disadvantages other workers, through displacement or substitution effects. This has been little studied, with mixed results.Footnote 42

In many cases, unemployed people who are offered subsidised employment are told that they can no longer continue to receive unemployment benefits if they refuse to accept the offer of employment. An alternative is ‘workfare’, which means working for your benefits (whereas most workers on ALMPs receive at least the minimum wage). Under both schemes there is an element of compulsion. So the question naturally arises ‘Are workers on these schemes happier than they would have been if they had remained unemployed?’ Only a handful of studies have addressed this issue. They find that, though workers on these schemes are less happy than workers in normal employment, they are more satisfied than those who remain unemployed.Footnote 43 This is because work provides important psychological and social benefits as well as income. But we should also remember the importance of not letting people become ‘locked-in’ to subsidised activity, rather than moving on as quickly as possible to regular employment.Footnote 44

Conclusions

  • The unemployed are generally significantly and substantially less satisfied with their lives than the employed. This relationship tends to be stronger in high-income countries where there are sharper differences between employment and unemployment.

  • In studies that look at within-person changes over time, unemployment typically reduces wellbeing by at least 0.6 points (out of 10).

  • Studying plant closures allows researchers to distinguish between endogenous and exogenous effects of unemployment. Workers who lose their jobs due to reasons outside of their control are generally more dissatisfied, although the effect of job loss remains negative and statistically significant for both groups.

  • Longer periods of unemployment can have scarring effects with long-lasting negative implications for wellbeing even after those affected have returned to work.

  • Aggregate unemployment also affects the wellbeing of people in work. This causes greater total losses of wellbeing than the loss of wellbeing on the part of the unemployed.

  • The psychosocial effects of unemployment on wellbeing are greater than the effect of lost income. Policy approaches targeting unemployment are therefore likely to be most conducive to wellbeing if they are able to protect and provide for the psychological and social benefits of work, as opposed to simply providing income support.

Questions for discussion

  1. (1) What are two benefits of using fixed-effects regressions to model the effect of unemployment relative to cross-sectional OLS regressions?

  2. (2) What do you think explains the lack of adaptation to unemployment?

  3. (3) Several countries around the world have begun debating proposals to provide citizens with an unconditional basic income. What do the results of this chapter suggest about the potential wellbeing impacts of these policies?

  4. (4) Some programmes require welfare recipients to accept work in order to receive support. Do you think this is reasonable?

Footnotes

1 International Labour Organization (2018).

2 Authors’ estimations using Gallup World Poll data.

4 For evidence, see Lykken and Tellegen (Reference Lykken and Tellegen1996); Diener and Lucas (Reference Diener1999); Bartels and Boomsma (Reference Bartels and Boomsma2009).

5 For evidence, see Frijters et al. (Reference Frijters, Johnston and Shields2011); De Neve et al. (Reference Clark and Lelkes2012); Oswald et al. (Reference Oswald, Proto and Sgroi2015).

7 The effects of unemployment were generally stronger for men than for women. Frijters et al. (Reference Frijters, Haisken-DeNew and Shields2004) replicated these results and showed that unemployment was found to have worse effects for East German women than West German women.

8 The regression is performed for people who had at least one spell of unemployment. It estimates the effect of becoming unemployed and of continuing to be unemployed 1, 2, 3 and 4 years later.

9 For example, Theodossiou (Reference Theodossiou1998); A. E. Clark (Reference Clark2003); A. E. Clark and Georgellis (Reference Clark and Georgellis2013). Also see Figure 11.2. In contrast to these results, Frijters et al. (Reference Frijters, Geishecker, Haisken‐DeNew and Shields2006); and N. Carroll (Reference Carroll2007) find similar effects of unemployment for men and women in Russia and Australia , respectively.

10 See A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark2018) p. 43.

12 Rudolf and Kang (Reference Rudolf and Kang2015).

14 A. E. Clark and Georgellis (Reference Clark and Georgellis2013).

15 A. E. Clark and Georgellis (Reference Clark and Georgellis2013).

16 Hetschko (Reference Hetschko2016).

17 A. E. Clark et al. (Reference DiJulio, Hamel, Munana and Brodie2018). The results were similar in Britain, Germany and Australia.

18 Bell and Blanchflower (Reference Bell and Blanchflower2011); and Clark and Lepinteur (Reference Clark and Lepinteur2019).

20 Knabe and Rätzel (Reference Knabe and Rätzel2011).

22 Suppose we have a regression, W=a1+a2UNEMP+a3logIncome; we just compare α2 with α3 ∆ log Income.

23 Knabe and Rätzel (Reference Knabe and Rätzel2011). The implications are remarkable. Suppose that, typically, unemployed people lose one half their income. Then the psychic cost is equivalent to being reduced to ¼ of your original income. It could only be compensated by a 4-fold increase in income.

24 Jahoda (Reference Jahoda1981) p. 188. Quoted from Hetschko et al. (Reference Hetschko, Knabe and Schöb2021).

25 For a short summary of the relevant theoretical models in psychology and organisational behaviour, see Suppa (Reference Suppa2021).

26 Winkelmann and Winkelmann (Reference Winkelmann and Winkelmann1995).

27 Nikolova and Ayhan (Reference Nikolova and Ayhan2019).

28 A. E. Clark (Reference Clark2003); Mendolia (Reference Mendolia2014).

31 Powdthavee and Vernoit (Reference Powdthavee and Vernoit2013). See also Kind and Haisken-DeNew (Reference Kind and Haisken-DeNew2012). A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark2018) find similar effects of father’s unemployment (but lack data on mother’s unemployment).

32 Nikolova and Nikolaev (2021).

33 A. E. Clark and Lepinteur (Reference Clark and Lepinteur2019).

34 In a study of world unemployment using the Gallup World Poll, De Neve and Ward (Reference De Neve, Ward, Helliwell, Layard and Sachs2017) also find that α1 < α2. They also find evidence that the pain of being unemployed is slightly reduced if more others are unemployed. But the effect is tiny. If the local unemployment rate is 10% (rather than 0) – a huge difference – the pain caused by unemployment is reduced by only 6%.

36 An underestimated issue is how far worker wellbeing is affected by the flow into unemployment as opposed to the stock of unemployed.

37 OECD (2020).

39 Layard et al. (Reference Martela and Riekki1991). With higher employment protection there is less short-term unemployment and more long-term unemployment.

41 Card et al. (Reference Card, Kluve and Weber2018) Tables 2 and 3.

44 In some studies, workfare programs have been found to have minor effectiveness at reducing overall unemployment (see Card et al. [Reference Card, Kluve and Weber2018]). But such analyses of workfare programmes are also likely to suffer from selection bias, since workers who join these programmes are those least likely to have found work before having to enroll in the programme.

References

Further Reading

Clark, A. E. (2003). Unemployment as a social norm: Psychological evidence from panel data. Journal of Labour Economics, 21(2), 323351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. E., and Georgellis, Y. (2013). Back to baseline in Britain: adaptation in the British household panel survey. Economica, 80(319), 496512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassenboehmer, S. C., and Haisken‐DeNew, J. P. (2009). You’re fired! The causal negative effect of entry unemployment on life satisfaction. The Economic Journal, 119(536), 448462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knabe, A., and Rätzel, S. (2011). Quantifying the psychological costs of unemployment: The role of permanent income. Applied Economics, 43(21), 27512763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winkelmann, L., and Winkelmann, R. (1998). Why are the unemployed so unhappy? Evidence from panel data. Economica, 65(257), 115.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 11.1 Average life satisfaction (0–10) by employment statusNote: 95% confidence intervals displayed.

Source: Gallup World Poll 2005–2019, Cantril ladder, adults 18–65.
Figure 1

Figure 11.2 Effect of unemployment on life satisfaction (0–10) over time (Germany)Note: Estimated using fixed-effects (within-person) regressions. Controls included for age, nationality, education, income, number of children, health and marital status. Levels are normalised relative to the baseline happiness level recorded five years before becoming unemployed; 95% confidence intervals displayed.

Source: De Neve and Ward (2017); SOEP data
Figure 2

Table 11.1 How life satisfaction (0–10) is affected by your own unemployment and by the regional unemployment rate (Household data, cross-section)

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (2018) Table 4.4; slightly adapted; Understanding Society (Britain), SOEP (Germany), HILDA (Australia) and BRFSS (United States); data for many years pooled with year dummies and usual controls

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×