Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T12:19:21.575Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do processing resource limitations shape heritage language grammars?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 July 2019

Claudia Felser*
Affiliation:
University of Potsdam, Germany
*
Address for correspondence: Claudia Felser, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Peer Commentaries
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Understanding the mechanisms that contribute to shaping heritage language grammars is crucial for developing a model that is well grounded both empirically and theoretically, and from which testable predictions can be derived. Polinsky and Scontras (Polinsky & Scontras, Reference Polinsky and Scontras2019) deserve praise for moving the field closer to developing such a model by identifying two possible triggers for heritage language grammars’ divergence from their baseline varieties: reduced input and economy of online resources. In this brief commentary I will focus on the latter of these.

The hypothesis that grammars are shaped by processing economy constraints or computational resource limitations is increasingly gaining support (e.g., Christiansen & Chater, Reference Christiansen and Chater2008; Gibson, Futrell, Piantadosi, Dautriche, Mahowald, Bergen & Levy, Reference Gibson, Futrell, Piantadosi, Dautriche, Mahowald, Bergen and Levy2019; Hawkins, Reference Hawkins2004; Fedzechkina, Chu & Jaeger, Reference Fedzechkina, Chu and Jaeger2018). Extending this hypothesis to heritage language grammars, Polinsky and Scontras discuss how online resource limitations might be partly responsible for three typical features of heritage language grammars: the avoidance of ambiguity, resistance to irregularity, and structural reduction. Although considering how computational resource limitations might shape heritage language speakers’ competence grammars is undoubtedly intriguing, empirical evidence from processing studies that supports the assumptions underlying the authors’ hypothesis is currently lacking.

First, there is at present little evidence for the authors’ assumption that producing or comprehending a heritage language is computationally more costly than processing a non-heritage native language. Although heritage language speakers may be less fluent readers than monolingual speakers due to reduced exposure to written heritage language input, limited literacy instruction or a smaller vocabulary, even speakers with intermediate heritage language proficiency do not necessarily show a general slow-down in reading-based experiments, or otherwise divergent processing patterns (e.g., Jegerski, Reference Jegerski2018). We need more evidence from real-time processing studies to see whether the above assumption does indeed hold true. Individual differences in cognitive capacities (such as working memory) might be expected to affect heritage language speakers’ performance such that high-capacity heritage language speakers should be less likely to show evidence of an economy-driven reduction in grammatical variability compared to low-capacity speakers.

Neither do we know whether heritage language speakers process their dominant language more efficiently than their non-dominant heritage language. To find out whether this is the case, heritage language speakers would need to be tested in parallel processing tasks in both of their languages. As Polinsky and Scontras point out in their article, doing so would also allow researchers to test whether divergent performance in a heritage language results from transfer from the dominant language. Using online processing measures can furthermore help reveal divergence that is undetectable in offline tasks, as has previously been demonstrated in second language processing research (e.g., Boxell & Felser, Reference Boxell and Felser2017; Felser & Cunnings, Reference Felser and Cunnings2012).

There is likewise no evidence that maintaining two grammars in parallel should create a particular challenge for heritage language speakers. Across the globe, bilingualism or multilingualism is extremely common, and there is no reason to think that maintaining two or more grammars threatens to exhaust an individuals’ representational capacities or processing resources. Even if it did, it is not transparent why heritage language speakers should resort to restructuring their native language grammar so as to free up processing resources; rather than to restructuring the dominant language grammar to the same end, or indeed restructuring both grammars. Again, testing heritage language speakers in both of their languages can be useful as it might reveal not only parallel acquisition or maintenance (Kupisch, Belikova, Özçelik, Stangen & White, Reference Kupisch, Belikova, Özçelik, Stangen and White2017) but also parallel restructuring effects (Felser & Arslan, Reference Felser and Arslan2019).

Finally, considering the reduction of ambiguity in heritage language grammars, it is not obvious why online resource limitations should lead to a grammatical system that avoids ambiguity. Ambiguity has been argued to aid processing efficiency by allowing speakers to re-use the same linguistic forms for different communicative purposes (Piantadosi, Tily & Gibson, Reference Piantadosi, Tily and Gibson2012). Research on real-time language comprehension has shown that comprehenders usually disambiguate ambiguous input within a matter of milliseconds and without any measurable effort or conscious deliberation. There is often a clear preference for one of the available form-to-interpretation mappings, and this preference may be subject to cross-linguistic variation. Results from processing studies show that heritage-language-specific ambiguity resolution preferences can be maintained even if the dominant language shows different resolution biases (Jegerski, Reference Jegerski2018, Knospe, Reference Knospe2019). Dispreferred mappings – such as inverse scope readings – may indeed be more difficult to compute than preferred mappings, but they will also be less frequently attested in heritage language learners’ input and so may be more likely to be lost.

To conclude, Polinsky and Scontras's hypothesis about heritage language grammars being partly shaped by online resource limitations should encourage more researchers to include real-time processing measures in their repertoire of test instruments. One challenge for future research will be to tease apart the roles of processing economy constraints and linguistic experience in triggering the restructuring of heritage language grammars.

Author ORCIDs

Claudia Felser, 0000-0001-9424-0950.

References

Boxell, O and Felser, C (2017) Sensitivity to parasitic gaps inside subject islands in native and non-native sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 494511.Google Scholar
Christiansen, MH and Chater, N (2008) Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, 489558.Google Scholar
Fedzechkina, M, Chu, B and Jaeger, TF (2018) Human information processing shapes language change. Psychological Science 29, 7282.Google Scholar
Felser, C and Arslan, S (2019) Inappropriate choice of definites in Turkish heritage speakers of German. Heritage Language Journal 16, 2243.Google Scholar
Felser, C and Cunnings, I (2012) Processing reflexives in English as a second language: The role of structural and discourse-level constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics 33, 571603.Google Scholar
Gibson, E, Futrell, R, Piantadosi, S, Dautriche, I, Mahowald, K, Bergen, L and Levy, RP (2019) How efficiency shapes human language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23, 389407.Google Scholar
Hawkins, JA (2004) Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jegerski, J (2018) Sentence processing in Spanish as a heritage language: Relative clause attachment in early bilinguals. Language Learning 68, 598634.Google Scholar
Knospe, G-M (2019) Processing of pronouns and reflexives in Turkish-German bilinguals. PhD thesis, University of Potsdam.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T, Belikova, A, Özçelik, Ö, Stangen, I and White, L (2017) On complete acquisition in heritage speakers: The definiteness effect in German-Turkish bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7, 132.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, ST, Tily, H and Gibson, E (2012) The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition 122, 280291.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M and Scontras, G (2019) Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000245Google Scholar