Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:19:38.394Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘Major depression’ in Ethiopia: validity is the problem

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

D. Summerfield*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London SE5 8AF, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2007 

Mogga et al (Reference Mogga, Prince and Alem2006) like the majority of published studies of people from low- and middle-income countries rely exclusively on Western measures of psychopathology (Reference Hollifield, Warner and LianHollifield et al, 2002). Culture is seen as mere packaging and is disregarded while standardised methodologies (‘reliability’) applied to universal psychobiological man get at the ‘real’ problem (Reference Summerfield and RosenSummerfield, 2004). This is a form of imperialism.

‘Reliablity' cannot redeem a study that commits a category error: the assumption that because phenomena can be identified from one setting to another, they mean the same everywhere. African cultures emphatically do not share a Western ethnopsychology that defines ‘emotion’ as a feature of individuals rather than situations, being internal, often biological, involuntary, distinct from cognition, a cause of pathology and targetable by technical interventions (Reference Lutz, Kleinman and GoodLutz, 1985). ‘Major depression’ is not a timeless, free-standing, internally coherent, universally valid, pathological entity requiring medical intervention (Reference SummerfieldSummerfield, 2006).

The hard truth, which if owned would totally disrupt business as usual, is that psychiatric measures are the products of a Western epistemology, including models of mind and definition of personhood. They simply cannot be turned into universally valid instruments – no matter how much tinkering with criteria and translation.

Noting the raised ‘disability’ scores and increased attendance at traditional healers, I do not doubt that something was ailing some of those with ‘persistent depression’. However, it is likely that this was a very heterogeneous group and that undiagnosed physical illness, particularly the diseases of poverty, was a major determinant. The only solution offered was antidepressants and it is no surprise that adherence was poor.

In the last few lines Mogga et al state that ‘more information is needed regarding the characteristics, beliefs, knowledge and illness attributes’ of the population. These domains should have been the point of departure of the study, not a mere after-thought. What can emerge when researchers know so little of the lived lives of participants?

References

Hollifield, M., Warner, T., Lian, N., et al (2002) Measuring trauma and health status in refugees: a critical review. JAMA, 288, 611616.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. (1985) Depression and the translation of emotional worlds. In Culture and Depression. Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of Affect and Disorder (eds Kleinman, A. & Good, B.), pp. 63100. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mogga, S., Prince, M., Alem, A., et al (2006) Outcome of major depression in Ethiopia. Population-based study British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 241246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Summerfield, D. (2004) Cross-cultural perspectives on the medicalisation of human suffering. In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Issues and Controversies (ed. Rosen, G.), pp. 233245. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Summerfield, D. (2006) Depression: epidemic or pseudo-epidemic? Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 99, 161162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.