Introduction
Bereavement can lead to a range of mental health problems such as prolonged grief disorder (PGD), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression. Successful psychological therapies address these difficulties by targeting cognitive processes thought to be involved in the development and maintenance of each problem (Beck, Reference Beck1979; Boelen, Reference Boelen2006; Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Shear et al., Reference Shear, Monk, Houck, Melhem, Frank, Reynolds and Sillowash2007). Of particular importance is the identification of modifiable maintenance factors of distress. These can be defined as variables that block the natural resolution of symptoms. Cognitive theory makes a theoretical prediction that unhelpful coping strategies prevent self-correction of negative appraisals (Beck, Reference Beck1979). When negative appraisals about the self, world, future, and others form following a loss, associated avoidance of situations, places, or people prevents individuals from making positive experiences that have the potential to shift unhelpful beliefs (e.g. the appraisal that the future is hopeless without the deceased persists if new activities or previously enjoyed ones are avoided) (Shear et al., Reference Shear, Monk, Houck, Melhem, Frank, Reynolds and Sillowash2007).
Within Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model for PTSD, unhelpful coping strategies are hypothesised to maintain symptoms of PTSD by preventing change of excessively negative meanings of the trauma and its aftermath and by preventing the trauma memory from being updated with information that changes the currently threatening meaning of its worst moments (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Ehlers, Hackmann, & Michael, Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004). More recently, psychological models of prolonged or complex grief propose a similar role for coping strategies in PGD (Boelen, van den Hout, & van den Bout, Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006; Shear et al., Reference Shear, Monk, Houck, Melhem, Frank, Reynolds and Sillowash2007). It is hypothesised that any failure to integrate new information about the loss within its context of other autobiographical memories will give rise to feelings of disbelief, confusion and yearning. Typically, such failures are predicted by the extent to which an individual actively blocks integration via unhelpful coping strategies such as avoidance of loss-related memories, thoughts and associated activities (Boelen et al., Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006; Shear et al., Reference Shear, Monk, Houck, Melhem, Frank, Reynolds and Sillowash2007). A number of observational longitudinal studies have demonstrated the mediating role of coping strategies on the relationship between predisposing factors such as attachment (Delespaux, Ryckebosch-Dayez, Heeren, & Zech, Reference Delespaux, Ryckebosch-Dayez, Heeren and Zech2013), neuroticism (van der Houwen, Stroebe, Schut, Stroebe, & Bout, Reference van der Houwen, Stroebe, Schut, Stroebe and Bout2010), violent loss (Boelen, de Keijser, & Smid, Reference Boelen, de Keijser and Smid2015), loss-centrality (Boelen, Reference Boelen2012) and psychopathology following bereavement. These studies have helped elucidate how coping strategies are employed by different personality types with different loss circumstances and how these relationships influence symptoms of PGD. Similarly, randomised controlled trials have shown that reductions in avoidant coping mediate treatment effects to reduce symptoms of grief and depression symptoms when measured concurrently (Smith, Tarakeshwar, Hansen, Kochman, & Sikkema, Reference Smith, Tarakeshwar, Hansen, Kochman and Sikkema2009).
However, to our knowledge, no studies have yet utilised a bereaved sample to examine the core hypotheses of Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model of PTSD and Boelen et al.'s (Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006) model of PGD, i.e. that difficulties in memory integration and negative appraisals lead to bereavement-related mental health problems such as PGD, PTSD and depression, and that these effects are mediated by unhelpful coping strategies.
One reason for this may be the lack of adequate measurement tools for the characteristics of memory that may be linked to loss integration. Previous research aimed at measuring whether PGD is associated with the extent to which a bereavement has been integrated into the autobiographical memory base has focused on overgeneral memory using the autobiographical memory task (Boelen, Huntjens, van Deursen, & van den Hout, Reference Boelen, Huntjens, van Deursen and van den Hout2010; Maccallum & Bryant, Reference Maccallum and Bryant2010) memory intrusiveness from a trauma questionnaire (Boelen, Reference Boelen2012; Halligan, Michael, Clark, & Ehlers, Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003), and a sense of unrealness regarding the loss (Boelen, Reference Boelen2010). Although these are important candidate processes in failures of loss integration, it is also possible that they represent only some of the memory characteristics that likely play a role. Here, we used the newly developed Oxford Grief-Memory Characteristics scale (OG-M) to measure the characteristics of loss memories, which we hypothesise may act as a metric for the extent to which a loss has been integrated within the context of other autobiographical memories (Ehlers, Reference Ehlers2006). This scale includes different aspects of loss-related memories such as triggers, content, valence, physical, emotional and behavioural consequences, intrusiveness, unrealness and vividness.
Following the theoretical predictions of Boelen et al.'s (Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006) cognitive behavioural model (CBT) of PGD and Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000), we aimed to determine whether longitudinal data collected from the Oxford Grief Study were compatible with the hypothesis that the effects of loss-related memory characteristics and negative loss appraisals on symptoms of PGD and PTSD would be in part mediated by unhelpful strategies of coping with the loss. We aimed to determine the specificity of these predictions by investigating mediation models in their association with symptoms of depression, as a common comorbid difficulty observed alongside PGD and PTSD. Theoretical accounts of depression predict coping strategies mediates the relationship between negative appraisals about the self, world and others and symptoms of depression (Beck, Reference Beck1979), however the relationship between memories and depression via coping remains unclear.
Furthermore, to aid specific clinical recommendations, we aimed to identify what types of coping strategies are important for symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression by conducting multiple mediation analyses of the four coping strategies subscales (avoidance, proximity seeking, loss rumination and injustice rumination).
Methods
Participants
Participants were 275 adults (age; M = 46.43, s.d. = 13.24, 79% women) recruited between 0 and 6 months after bereavement (months since loss; M = 2.94, s.d. = 2.01, range 0–8 months) and followed up 6 and 12 months later. Nine percent of the sample had lost a loved one to violent means, 30% (N = 83) had lost a partner, 9% (N = 24) had lost a child, 38% (N = 105) had lost a parent, 6% (N = 16) had lost a sibling, 14% (N = 39) another relative and 3% (N = 8%) a close non-relative (see also Smith & Ehlers, Reference Smith and Ehlers2020). All participants were recruited via the internet through bereavement charity email lists, social media advertisements and through keyword-based advertising on the Google content network.
Procedure
Measures of loss-related memory characteristics, negative appraisals at T1 (0–6 months), unhelpful coping strategies at T2 (6–12 months) and measures of psychopathology at T3 (12–18 months) were completed online in accordance with ethical guidelines (Smith, Thew, & Graham, Reference Smith, Thew and Graham2018).
Measures
Symptom measures
Prolonged Grief Disorder Inventory (PG-13)
The PG-13 measures the PGD criteria proposed by Prigerson et al. (Reference Prigerson, Horowitz, Jacobs, Parkes, Aslan, Goodkin and Neimeyer2009). It assesses the prevalence and intensity of PGD symptoms in the past month (e.g. yearning for the deceased, feelings of emotional numbness/detachment from others and feeling that a part of oneself died along with the deceased). A continuous PGD total score can be determined by summing the items. Internal consistency of the PG-13 was excellent (α = 0.92). This criterion has been cited as the closest conceptualisation to, and the empirical basis for, the newly developed ICD-11 and DSM-5-TR criteria for PGD (Killikelly & Maercker, Reference Killikelly and Maercker2018; Prigerson, Boelen, Xu, Smith, & Maciejewski, Reference Prigerson, Boelen, Xu, Smith and Maciejewski2021).
Posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5)
The PCL-5 (Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Litz, Keane, Palmieri, Marx and Schnurr2013) assesses distress associated with the 20 symptoms of PTSD specified in DSM-5 over the past month. Items are rated on a five-point scale, from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) with respect to symptoms arising as a result of their bereavement. The PCL-5 demonstrated excellent internal consistency (α = 0.96).
Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)
The PHQ-9 (Kroenke, Spitzer, and Williams, Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001) is based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th Edition, Text Revision criteria for major depressive disorder (DSM-IV-TR, American Psychiatric Association, 2000). It mirrors the nine major depressive symptoms, with each item scored on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) scale in the last 2 weeks and demonstrated excellent internal consistency (α = 0.93).
Cognitive measures
The Oxford Grief measures were developed as a battery of cognitive and behavioural factors associated with the development and maintenance of PGD. Content development was informed by the literature on cognitive processes in PTSD (Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin, & Orsillo, Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999; Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003) and grief (Boelen & Lensvelt-Mulders, Reference Boelen and Lensvelt-Mulders2005; Eisma et al., Reference Eisma, Stroebe, Schut, van den Bout, Boelen and Stroebe2014), the expertise of therapists working in traumatic bereavement, and through qualitative interviews with bereaved individuals experiencing typical and prolonged grief (Smith, Rankin, & Ehlers, Reference Smith, Rankin and Ehlers2020). These measures were all found to have good to excellent internal consistency, test–retest reliability and criterion and convergent validity (Smith & Ehlers, Reference Smith and Ehlers2019). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of the OG measures supported a unidimensional structure for the memory characteristics scale, a five factor structure for the negative appraisals scale and a four factor structure for the coping strategies scale (Smith & Ehlers, Reference Smith and Ehlers2019).
Loss-related memory characteristics (OG-M)
Twenty-seven items probed memory triggers and their consequences (e.g. ‘I am reminded of the loss for no apparent reason’), qualities of memory (e.g. ‘Memories of things we did together are painful’), the physical impact of loss-related memories (e.g. ‘The memories of [-]'s death make my body ache with overwhelming fatigue’) and were rated on a 5-point scale (0 – not at all to 4 – very strongly) in the past month, as well as the frequency and qualities of unintentional memories of the loss. Internal consistency was excellent (ω = 0.97).
Negative grief appraisals (OG-A)
This 35-item questionnaire asks participants to indicate on a 7-point scale (1 – totally disagree to 7 – totally agree) the extent to which they agree with the statements in the past month. Items pertain to five content domains: (1) Loss of self and life – 10 items (e.g. ‘Without [-] I can never be strong again’), (2) Regret – 3 items (e.g. ‘I blame myself for things I did or did not do when [-] was alive’), (3) Catastrophic consequences of grief – 7 items (e.g. ‘If I start to cry I won't be able to stop’), (4) Loss of relationships and future – 9 items (e.g. ‘I cannot maintain previous relationships without [-]’) and (5) Fear of losing connection to the deceased – 6 items (e.g. ‘If I don't do everything I can to feel close to [-] I will lose them forever’). Internal consistency was excellent (α = 0.97).
Unhelpful coping strategies (OG-CS)
This 23-item questionnaire asks participants on a 5-point scale (1 – never to 5 – always) to indicate how often they used particular strategies to cope with their loss. Items pertain to four content domains: (1) Avoidance – 6 items (e.g. ‘I avoid places we went together’), (2) Proximity seeking – 7 items (e.g. ‘I neglect other things because I spend a lot of time doing things for [-] (e.g. creating memorials, fundraising’), (3) Loss rumination – 7 items (e.g. ‘I dwell on moments that could have changed the outcome’) and (4) Injustice rumination – 4 items (e.g. ‘I think over and over about how it could be that this happened’). Internal consistency was excellent (α = 0.93).
Data analysis
Data were analysed using counterfactually based causal definitions of direct and indirect effects in Mplus (Version 7.4; Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2007; Pearl, Reference Pearl2011). Using this method the pure natural direct effect (PNDE) and total natural indirect effect (TNIE) are extracted as the product of the total effect the independent variables on the outcome (Muthén & Asparouhov, Reference Muthén and Asparouhov2015). The TNIE (indirect) reflects the effect of appraisals and memory characteristics on symptoms via unhelpful coping strategies. The PNDE (direct) is the effect of appraisals and memory characteristics on symptoms not accounted for by unhelpful coping strategies. Calculation of the PNDE and the TNIE allows us to make causal inferences about the impact of coping strategies as a mediator of post-loss mental health problems. These paths are calculated by specifying two levels of each predictor. In the case of continuous predictors the mean and the standard deviation of the latent variables is chosen (Muthén & Asparouhov, Reference Muthén and Asparouhov2015). Causal mediation assumes that, conditional on covariates, there are no unmeasured confounders of the predictor–mediator, predictor–outcome and mediator–outcome relationships, also known as sequential ignorability (Forastiere, Mattei, & Ding, Reference Forastiere, Mattei and Ding2018). By estimating the direct and indirect effects in the presence of an increasing value of rho ρ (i.e. the correlation of the residuals of the mediator and outcome variables) one can estimate the extent to which the mediation results are likely to remain important when including unmeasured confounders (e.g. autocorrelations)Footnote †Footnote 1 (Imai, Keele, & Yamamoto, Reference Imai, Keele and Yamamoto2010). Here, we report ρ as a measure of sensitivity when the lower confidence interval (CI) of the indirect effect includes 0 (i.e. becomes non-significant). These models estimated direct and indirect relationships of cognitive predictors simultaneously controlling for the shared variance between memory characteristics and appraisals.
The robust weighted least squares (WLSMV) extraction method suitable for outcomes with mixed distributions was used to evaluate hypotheses. As a precursor to mediation analyses, and to preserve the latent factor structure of the data, three confirmatory factor analyses were run to determine whether models specifying a relationship between memory characteristics, appraisals and coping strategies and PGD (model 1), PTSD (model 2) and depression (model 3), respectively, were an adequate fit to the data. Next, SEM was used to specify three mediation models (Fig. 1) in which coping strategies at T2 are specified as a mediator between appraisals and memory characteristics at T1 and symptom outcomes of PGD, PTSD and depression at T3. The PNDE and the TNIE are reported after adjustments for demographic variables and loss characteristics of importance. Smith and Ehlers (Reference Smith and Ehlers2020) found gender, level of education and type of loss – specifically losing a child (Yes/No) and losing a partner (Yes/No) to be associated with higher grief. Other research has reported months since loss and experience of a violent loss as predictors of grief severity (Lobb et al., Reference Lobb, Kristjanson, Aoun, Monterosso, Halkett and Davies2010; Smith & Ehlers, Reference Smith and Ehlers2021). Therefore, mediation analyses are reported after adjusting for gender, education, losing a child or a parent, violent loss and months since loss.
To further examine the direction of the mediated effect, a series of models were computed which were identical to those in Fig. 1 apart from the mediator and outcome variables, which were swapped. These, therefore, examined the ‘reverse’ mediation hypothesis, using PGD, PTSD or depression at T2 as the potential mediator, and coping strategies at T3 the dependent variable after adjusting for demographics and loss characteristics. These models allow a comparison of the influence of the outcome on the mediator, further adding to our understanding of causality.Footnote 2
Determining the unique contribution of multiple parallel latent mediators in a counterfactual framework is not yet easily derived using Mplus (Muthén, Muthén, & Asparouhov, Reference Muthén, Muthén and Asparouhov2017). Therefore, we modelled six multiple mediation analyses (2 predictors × 3 symptom variables) in an SEM framework using the subscales of the OG-CS as parallel mediators and report the unique indirect effects for each mediator on symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression.
Model adequacy was assessed by the χ2 goodness-of-fit test where the χ2:df ratio is smaller than 3:1 (Kline, Reference Kline2015), comparative fit index (CFI) of 0.90 or higher as acceptable and 0.95 or higher as good, and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of 0.08 or lower as acceptable and 0.06 as good (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Significance of paths in the mediation analyses was determined using 100 bootstrap resamples and examining bias-corrected CIs.
In order to investigate the influence of conceptual overlap between the characterisation of the cognitive mechanisms detailed in the questionnaires and the symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression, 10 additional causal mediation models were analysed.Footnote 3 Symptoms representing potential overlap (e.g. the avoidance item from the PGD conceptualisation) were systematically removed and reanalysed to determine the impact of potential confounding. The pattern of results reported below was unchanged.
Results
Confirmatory factor analyses
The confirmatory factor analyses (Table 1) showed adequate fit according to χ2 and CFI, and a good fit according to the RMSEA. All cognitive predictors were significantly associated with the psychological distress outcomes (p < 0.001).
Note: Predictor estimates are correlations between predictors (memory characteristics, appraisals and coping strategies) and psychological symptoms (PGD, PTSD and depression).
T1 = time point 1 (0–6 months), T2 (6–12 months), T3 (12–18 months). Dx = diagnosis.
*** p < 0.001.
Causal mediation analyses
Standardised path coefficients, bias-corrected bootstrapped CIs, total variance accounted for in psychopathology by the mediation model, sensitivity metric ρ and percentage of the variance accounted for by the indirect path via coping strategies are reported in Table 2. All three adjusted mediation models were an acceptable fit to the data.
P M, percent mediation (i.e. the percentage of the total effect the predictor that is accounted for by the mediated path ab). ρ is the value of the correlated residuals of the mediator and outcome resulting from unmeasured confounders necessary to result in non-significance of the indirect effect. Covariates: 1 = gender, 2 = child loss, 3 = partner loss, 4 = education, 5 = months since death, 6 = violent loss.
Note. Path a represents the effect of the predictor (memory characteristics or appraisals) on the mediator variable (coping strategies). Path b represents the effect of the coping strategies on psychological symptoms PGD, PTSD and depression. PNDE represents the direct effect of the predictor on psychological distress controlling for the effect of coping strategies and demographic and loss characteristics. TNIE represents the indirect effect of the predictor on the symptom variables via unhelpful coping strategies. Coefficients are standardised. CIs are 95%.
a χ2 = 6228, df = 4065, χ2/df = 1.53, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.044.
b χ2 = 6236, df = 4065, χ2/df = 1.53, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.044.
c χ2 = 6219, df = 4065, χ2/df = 1.53, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.044.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Prolonged grief disorder
The adjusted mediation model accounted for 69% of the variance in PGD. Bias-corrected bootstrapped CIs for the true natural indirect effects (memory characteristics: TNIE1 = 0.12, 95% CI 0.06–0.21; appraisals: TNIE2 = 0.20, 95% CI 0.10–0.31) based on 100 bootstrap samples were entirely above zero indicating that the paths from memory characteristics and appraisals to PGD were mediated by coping strategies. For both predictors, the PNDEs were not significant indicating that memory characteristics and appraisals at time point 1 do not lead to symptoms of PGD a year later via any other mechanism than coping strategies at time point 2. These indirect effects accounted for 48% and 51% of the effect of memory characteristics and appraisals on PGD respectively. The sensitivity analysis suggested that these relationships remained significant as long as ρ ⩽ about 0.4, indicating moderate sensitivity to violations of the sequential ignorability assumption.
We tested the reverse mediation model, where PGD at T2 was entered as the mediator and coping strategies at T3 as the outcome. The true natural indirect effects for memory characteristics and appraisals on coping via PGD symptoms were significant (memory characteristics: TNIE1 = 0.08, 95% CI 0.05–0.14; appraisals: TNIE2 = 0.08, 95% CI 0.05–0.15). The PNDEs of memory characteristics on coping strategies was significant (PNDE = 0.24, 95% CI 0.08–0.41) but the direct effect of appraisals was not (PNDE = 0.20, 95% CI −0.04 to 0.35). Sensitivity analyses were not available for reversed models as the outcome variable is latent.
Posttraumatic stress disorder
As with PGD both the true natural indirect effects for memory characteristics and appraisals, adjusted for demographics and loss characteristics, were significant according to the bootstrapped CIs (memory characteristics: TNIE1 = 0.10, 95% CI 0.04–0.20; appraisals: TNIE2 = 0.17, 95% CI 0.06–0.25). Direct effects were not significant after controlling for the impact of coping strategies. The total model accounted for 54% of the variance in PTSD symptoms and indirect effects accounted for 50% and 43% of the total effect of appraisals and memory characteristics on PTSD, respectively. The indirect effects remained significant up to ρ = 0.20.
In the reverse-adjusted mediation model the TNIEs were significant for appraisals (TNIE = 0.11, 95% CI 0.05–0.19) but not memory characteristics (TNIE = 0.05, 95% CI −0.02 to 0.10). Although the PNDE of memory characteristics on coping strategies was significant (PNDE = 0.28, 95% CI 0.15–0.47) but the PNDE of appraisal on coping strategies was not (PNDE = 0.16, 95% CI −0.15 to 0.29).
Depression
The indirect paths of appraisals and memory characteristics on symptoms of depression via coping strategies were significant (memory characteristics: TNIE1 = 0.07, 95% CI 0.02–0.16; appraisals: TNIE2 = 0.13, 95% CI 0.04–0.22). Direct effects were not significant. Indirect effects accounted for 47% of the effect of memory characteristics on depression and 76% of the effect of appraisals on depression. The total model explained 38% of the variance in depression. The value of ρ = 0.10 indicated that the model was highly sensitive to violations of the sequential ignorability hypothesis suggesting that unmeasured confounders may affect the results of the model.
The reverse mediation model indicated that only the TNIE for appraisals via depression was significant (memory characteristics: TNIE1 = 0.02, 95% CI −0.01 to 0.05; appraisals: TNIE2 = 0.04, 95% CI 0.02–0.08). The direct effects were significant (memory characteristics: PNDE1 = 0.33, 95% CI 0.15–0.49; appraisals: PNDE2 = 0.26, 95% CI 0.05–0.41).
Investigating conceptual overlap
In all 10 additional causal mediation models significant indirect effects persisted with comparable levels of ρ (0.40–0.49 for PGD, 0.20–0.35 for PTSD, 0.10 for depression). All direct paths were non-significant as above.
Multiple mediation
Standardised path coefficients and bias-corrected bootstrapped CIs are reported in Table 3. All six models were an acceptable fit to the data. All four subscales (avoidance, proximity seeking, loss rumination and injustice rumination) mediated the relationship of both memory characteristics and appraisals on symptoms of PGD. The direct effect of appraisals on PGD was not significant. However, the direct effect of memory characteristics on PGD was significant and negative, indicating that once unhelpful coping is accounted for by the model the remaining effect of memory characteristics predicted lower grief symptoms. When considering trauma symptoms, avoidance, proximity seeking and loss rumination significantly mediated the effect of memory characteristics on PTSD, while only avoidance and loss rumination mediated the effect of appraisals on PTSD. The direct effect of memory characteristics on PTSD was significant and negative, as with PGD, while the direct effect of appraisals was not significant. Finally, avoidance and loss rumination significantly mediated the effect of both memory characteristics and appraisals on depressive symptoms. The direct effects in the depression models were not significant.
Est., estimate; CI, 95% confidence intervals; DE, direct effect; IndE, indirect effect.
Coefficients are standardised.
a χ2 = 2093, df = 1216, χ2/df = 1.72, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.051.
b χ2 = 2100, df = 1216, χ2/df = 1.73, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.051.
c χ2 = 2086, df = 1216, χ2/df = 1.72, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.051.
d χ2 = 3015, df = 1639, χ2/df = 1.84, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.055.
e χ2 = 3008, df = 1639, χ2/df = 1.83, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.055.
f χ2 = 3005, df = 1639, χ2/df = 1.83, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.055.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Discussion
The current study used counterfactually defined causal effects to investigate the core cognitive processes (loss-related memory characteristics, negative appraisals and maladaptive coping strategies) in Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model for PTSD and Boelen et al.'s CBT model for PGD in their prediction of symptoms of PGD and PTSD in a longitudinal community sample of bereaved individuals. The loss-related cognitive predictors were also tested for their utility in predicting depression, as a common comorbidity following loss.
Coping strategies mediated the relationship between negative appraisals and PGD, PTSD and depression and the relationship between memory characteristics and PGD, PTSD and depression. These results fit with predictions from both Boelen et al.'s (Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006) CBT of PGD, Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model for PTSD, and previous research that found therapies targeting unhelpful coping strategies such as avoidance through exposure to avoided emotions or stimuli were effective at reducing symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Kenny, Joscelyne, Rawson, Maccallum, Cahill and Nickerson2014; Eisma et al., Reference Eisma, Boelen, van den Bout, Stroebe, Schut, Lancee and Stroebe2015; Papa, Sewell, Garrison-Diehn, & Rummel, Reference Papa, Sewell, Garrison-Diehn and Rummel2013). The direct effects for each model were not significant after controlling for the effect of unhelpful coping strategies. This suggests that the coping strategies at 6 months measured here fully capture the impact of initial appraisals and memory characteristics at T1 on symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression 12 months later. Analyses were adjusted for demographics and loss characteristics previously shown to be related to symptoms of PGD, but the existence of other significant contributors cannot be ruled out. Therefore, we conducted sensitivity analyses to understand the extent to which these results would persist in the presence of unmeasured confounders.
The sensitivity analyses indicate that these results are the most robust for PGD, with PTSD and depression demonstrating high sensitivity to unmeasured confounders. This is perhaps not surprising given that the measures utilised in this study were designed to measure appraisals, memory characteristics and coping strategies typical of people with PGD, as opposed to depression and PTSD, which may have contributed to the lower sensitivity in these analyses. However, despite being designed specifically for grief reactions, the results show that the Oxford Grief measures are helpful in understanding symptoms of PTSD and depression and therefore may be useful in predicting a variety of symptoms following bereavement.
All three reverse mediation models showed indirect effects of reduced magnitude compared with the forwards mediation models. Although indirect effects of memory characteristics and appraisals were significant via PGD, only appraisals predicted coping strategies via PTSD and depression. These results support the causal relationships defined in the theoretical models of PGD (Boelen et al., Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006) and PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). The forwards mediation supports the development and maintenance of symptoms via cognitive processes while the reverse mediation points to a dynamic feedback loop, or vicious cycle, whereby symptoms influence the effect of cognitive variables on coping strategies. For example, believing that meaningful relationships are impossible without the deceased, may trigger intense emotional pain, prompting rumination about how the death could have been prevented.
In order to make some specific clinical predictions about the use of unhelpful coping following bereavement we used multiple mediation analyses to determine whether the subscales of the OG-CS differentially predicted symptoms of PGD, PTSD and depression. The results indicate avoidance, proximity seeking, loss rumination and injustice rumination all significantly mediated of the effect of memory characteristics and appraisals on PGD. This supports the utility of the individual subscales of the OG-CS as a clinical tool for guiding treatment of PGD by indicating the most pressing targets for treatment and in which order they should be addressed. For bereaved individuals suffering with trauma symptoms a specific focus on strategies of avoidance, proximity seeking and loss rumination may be more appropriate, whereas depressed individuals may benefit most from reducing strategies of avoidance and loss rumination.
Several limitations of the current study should be noted. Due to sample size constraints not all available data could be modelled in these analyses. Further studies with larger samples may be able to model cognitive mechanisms and symptoms at all time points in order to elucidate the role of potential unmeasured confounders such as autocorrelations. Given the strong relationships between cognitive predictors, likely inflated by high levels of resilience in our sample, validating these findings further in a clinical sample will be of particular importance. These analyses assess symptoms of PGD using the Prigerson et al. (Reference Prigerson, Horowitz, Jacobs, Parkes, Aslan, Goodkin and Neimeyer2009) criterion. Several sets of diagnostic criteria for severe and enduring grief exist within the research literature and research has shown prevalence rates and predictive validity may differ by conceptualisation (Cozza et al., Reference Cozza, Fisher, Mauro, Zhou, Ortiz, Skritskaya and Shear2016; Maciejewski, Maercker, Boelen, & Prigerson, Reference Maciejewski, Maercker, Boelen and Prigerson2016). Recent guidance has suggested that clinical interviews covering multiple criteria sets may be helpful in creating consensus (Lenferink, Boelen, Smid, & Paap, Reference Lenferink, Boelen, Smid and Paap2019). The current study relied on self-report measures to assess the predictors, mediators and outcomes. Future research that utilises clinician administered assessments will likely be helpful in supporting the validity of these findings. Study participants were predominantly Caucasian. As such, the observed patterns may not generalise to other populations. Future studies would benefit from confirming these results in different cultural contexts. Furthermore, confirming the multiple mediation results within a causal framework when possible will add further tests of these findings. The measures we used were designed to measure appraisals and memory characteristics typical of people with PGD, and we did not assess depression- and PTSD-specific memory characteristics or appraisals, which may have contributed to the lower variance explained in these models (PTSD: R 2 = 54; depression: R 2 = 39) compared to those for PGD (R 2 = 69). However, despite being designed specifically for grief reactions, the results show that the Oxford Grief measures predict as much variance in PTSD following loss as a previous study using a general trauma sample and PTSD specific measures of memory characteristics, appraisals and coping strategies (Beierl, Böllinghaus, Clark, Glucksman, & Ehlers, Reference Beierl, Böllinghaus, Clark, Glucksman and Ehlers2020).
Finally, the multiple mediation analyses were conducted using conventional direct and indirect effect within an SEM framework. Implementation of counterfactually based parallel multiple mediation analyses is not yet easily derived in Mplus. Given the second-order latent variable model using counterfactually defined effects indicated significant mediation for PGD, PTSD and depression we felt confident in the validity of moving to an SEM framework for our multiple mediation analyses. However, assessment of multiple mediators using sensitivity analyses will be helpful in future studies to confirm the generalisability of these results.
Notwithstanding these limitations, the current study has important clinical implications. Given that memory characteristics, appraisals and coping strategies are modifiable, they are appropriate targets for interventions with bereaved individuals seeking help. In addition, our finding that the relationship between memory characteristics, appraisals and PGD, PTSD and depression was fully mediated by unhelpful coping suggests that interventions aimed at reducing unhelpful strategies, and developing healthy coping, such as behavioural activation (Papa et al., Reference Papa, Sewell, Garrison-Diehn and Rummel2013), ‘then v. now’ discrimination of memory triggers (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000), exposure to avoided memories (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Kenny, Joscelyne, Rawson, Maccallum, Cahill and Nickerson2014; Shear et al., Reference Shear, Wang, Skritskaya, Duan, Mauro and Ghesquiere2014) or stimuli (Eisma et al., Reference Eisma, Boelen, van den Bout, Stroebe, Schut, Lancee and Stroebe2015) may be particularly helpful in reducing trauma symptoms following bereavement. Clinicians offering bereavement support may want to consider initial levels of loss-related memory characteristics, and appraisals when developing a treatment plan and assess unhelpful coping to promote successful treatment implementation.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003123
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Jennifer Wild, Emma Warnock-Parkes, and Nick Grey for their input in preparing the cognitive measures. Further thanks go to Erin H. Thompson founder of The Loss Foundation, The Compassionate Friends, Macmillan Cancer Care and Assist Trauma Care. Heartfelt thanks also to the participants of The Oxford Grief Study for their bravery and altruism.
Financial support
This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (AE: 200796); the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, based at Oxford University Hospitals NHS Trust (KS: NIHR-INF-0085) and the Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre (AE and KS) and the Medical Research Council (KS: MR/V001841/1). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
Conflict of interest
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.