Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-89wxm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-07T10:57:00.353Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Facial attractiveness and preference of sexual dimorphism: A comparison across five populations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2021

Vojtěch Fiala*
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy and History of Science, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Viničná 7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic
Vít Třebický
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy and History of Science, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Viničná 7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic Faculty of Physical Education and Sport, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
Farid Pazhoohi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 1Z4, Canada
Juan David Leongómez
Affiliation:
Human Behaviour Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad El Bosque, Bogota, Colombia
Petr Tureček
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy and History of Science, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Viničná 7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic
S. Adil Saribay
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Kadir Has University, Istanbul, Turkey
Robert Mbe Akoko
Affiliation:
Department of Communication and Development Studies, University of Bamenda, Cameroon
Karel Kleisner
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy and History of Science, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Viničná 7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Despite intensive research, evolutionary psychology has not yet reached a consensus regarding the association between sexual dimorphism and attractiveness. This study examines associations between perceived and morphological facial sexual dimorphism and perceived attractiveness in samples from five distant countries (Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia, Iran and Turkey). We also examined possible moderating effects of skin lightness, averageness, age, body mass and facial width. Our results suggest that in all samples, women's perceived femininity was positively related to their perceived attractiveness. Women found perceived masculinity in men attractive only in Czechia and Colombia, two distant populations. The association between perceived sexual dimorphism and attractiveness is thus potentially universal only for women. Across populations, morphological sexual dimorphism and averageness are not universally associated with either perceived facial sexual dimorphism or attractiveness. With our exploratory approach, results highlight the need for control of which measure of sexual dimorphism is used (perceived or measured) because they affect perceived attractiveness differently. Morphological averageness and sexual dimorphism are not good predictors of perceived attractiveness. It is noted that future studies should use samples from multiple populations to allow for identification of specific effects of local environmental and socioeconomic conditions on preferred traits in unmanipulated local facial stimuli.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Morphological sexual dimorphism is not universally associated with perceived facial sexual dimorphism and attractiveness

1. Introduction

According to the signalling theory, facial traits which are perceived as attractive are considered honest cues of biological fitness (Gangestad & Scheyd, Reference Gangestad and Scheyd2005; Thornhill & Gangestad, Reference Thornhill and Gangestad1999; Kościński, Reference Kościński2007, Reference Kościński2008), in particular healthiness and viability (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Holzleitner, Talamas and Perrett2016; Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Chan, Zebrowitz and Simmons2003), hormone-based development of secondary sexual characteristics and fertility (Law Smith et al., Reference Law Smith, Perrett, Jones, Cornwell, Moore, Feinberg and Hillier2006; Rantala et al., Reference Rantala, Moore, Skrinda, Krama, Kivleniece, Kecko and Krams2012; Whitehouse et al., Reference Whitehouse, Gilani, Shafait, Mian, Tan, Maybery and Eastwood2015). They also provide specific cues to psychological characteristics that are important in partnership and childbearing, such as faithfulness (Boothroyd et al., Reference Boothroyd, Jones, DeBruine and Perrett2008) and willingness to cooperate (Stirrat & Perrett, Reference Stirrat and Perrett2010).

Although there is some variation in the perception of facial attractiveness between individuals belonging to the same local population (Bronstad & Russell, Reference Bronstad and Russell2007; Germine et al., Reference Germine, Russell, Bronstad, Blokland, Smoller, Kwok and Wilmer2015; Hönekopp, Reference Hönekopp2006; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Mileva and Ritchie2018), people from similar cultural backgrounds tend to perceive facial attractiveness similarly (Kowner & Ogawa, Reference Kowner and Ogawa1995; Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Kalakanis, Rubenstein, Larson, Hallam and Smoot2000; Little & Hancock, Reference Little and Hancock2002; Strzałko & Kaszycka, Reference Strzałko and Kaszycka1992).

While there is some evidence that supports a hypothesis of cross-cultural consensus on attractiveness ratings (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Nolan, Hayward, Russell and Sulikowski2013; Coetzee et al., Reference Coetzee, Greeff, Stephen and Perrett2014; Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Kalakanis, Rubenstein, Larson, Hallam and Smoot2000), a growing number of studies report mixed results regarding agreement between samples from distant countries (Apicella et al., Reference Apicella, Little and Marlowe2007; Jones & Hill, Reference Jones and Hill1993; Sorokowski, Kościński, et al., Reference Sorokowski, Kościński and Sorokowska2013; Zebrowitz et al., Reference Zebrowitz, Wang, Bronstad, Eisenberg, Undurraga, Reyes-García and Godoy2012). It is not, however, the case that members of distant populations either agree or disagree entirely on which traits are perceived as attractive. Instead, some facial traits are preferred across distant samples while others are not. For example, youthfulness (Buss, Reference Buss1989; Maestripieri et al., Reference Maestripieri, Klimczuk, Traficonte and Wilson2014; McLellan & McKelvie, Reference McLellan and McKelvie1993) and average facial traits (Deffenbacher et al., Reference Deffenbacher, Vetter, Johanson and O'Toole1998; Komori et al., Reference Komori, Kawamura and Ishihara2009; Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Yoshikawa, Clark, Kieran, McKay and Akamatsu2001; cf. Apicella et al., Reference Apicella, Little and Marlowe2007) are preferred universally.

Aside from that, there is a cross-cultural agreement concerning preferences regarding facial skin colouration. Lighter-skinned women are perceived as more attractive than darker-skinned women within a given population (Aoki, Reference Aoki2002; Badaruddoza, 2007; Carrito et al., Reference Carrito, Santos, Lefevre, Whitehead, Silva and Perrett2016; Dixson et al., Reference Dixson, Dixson, Bishop and Parish2010; van den Berghe & Frost, Reference van den Berghe and Frost1986; Wagatsuma, Reference Wagatsuma1967), but for some dark-skinned populations, the results are less conclusive (Dixson et al., Reference Dixson, Little, Dixson and Brooks2017; Sorokowski, Sorokowska, et al., Reference Sorokowski, Sorokowska and Kras2013). Moreover, in a cross-cultural comparison, skin colouration variance is an important trait for ratings of ethnic typicality and attractiveness for African raters, while it is less important for ratings made by Europeans regardless of the ethnic origin of the presented stimulus faces (Coetzee et al., Reference Coetzee, Greeff, Stephen and Perrett2014; Strom et al., Reference Strom, Zebrowitz, Zhang, Bronstad and Lee2012; Kleisner et al., Reference Kleisner, Kočnar, Tureček, Stella, Akoko, Třebický and Havlíček2017).

On the other hand, preference for facial sexual dimorphism (how well the development of facial shape and colouration represents features typical for a given sex) varies substantially across populations from distant countries, especially with respect to male faces (DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, Welling and Little2010; Marcinkowska et al., Reference Marcinkowska, Rantala, Lee, Kozlov, Aavik, Cai and Dixson2019, and citations below).

Such differences in attractiveness perception between populations from distant countries contradict the assumption that facial attractiveness serves as a cue of biological quality. A plausible evolutionary-based explanation is that in populations that live in different environments, preferred traits may vary because different characteristics are optimal for survival and reproduction under different environmental conditions (DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, Welling and Little2010; Lee & Zietsch, Reference Lee and Zietsch2011; Little et al., Reference Little, Cohen, Jones and Belsky2007).

1.1. Morphological and perceived sexual dimorphism and attractiveness

During ontogeny, the facial traits of men and women gradually diverge owing to the action of sex steroids (Marečková et al., Reference Marečková, Weinbrand, Chakravarty, Lawrence, Aleong, Leonard and Paus2011; Whitehouse et al., Reference Whitehouse, Gilani, Shafait, Mian, Tan, Maybery and Eastwood2015). As a result, adult faces acquire sexually dimorphic features (Hausman, Reference Hausman1999; Mooradian et al., Reference Mooradian, Morley and Korenman1987; Worthman, Reference Worthman1995).

Higher levels of perceived feminine characteristics in women's faces are associated with higher perceived attractiveness, as evidenced by previous studies that used non-manipulated women's faces (Foo, Simmons, et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; Muñoz-Reyes et al., Reference Muñoz-Reyes, Iglesias-Julios, Pita and Turiegano2015; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Pound, Stephen, Clark and Penton-Voak2010), manipulated composite female facial stimuli (Perrett et al., Reference Perrett, May and Yoshikawa1994, Reference Perrett, Lee, Penton-Voak, Rowland, Yoshikawa, Burt and Akamatsu1998; Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Hickford and Jeffery2000; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jones, Debruine and Little2009) and even manipulated individual women's faces (Mogilski & Welling, Reference Mogilski and Welling2017; for a review, see Rhodes, Reference Rhodes2006). Women with more sex-typical (more feminine) facial features were also shown to have relatively higher oestrogen levels (Durante & Li, Reference Durante and Li2009; Law Smith et al., Reference Law Smith, Perrett, Jones, Cornwell, Moore, Feinberg and Hillier2006; Probst et al., Reference Probst, Bobst and Lobmaier2016). There is evidence from a US sample to the effect that fertility is positively associated with oestrogen levels (Lipson & Ellison, Reference Lipson and Ellison1996). On the other hand, in deprived, poorer and rural populations – where women generally have lower sex hormone levels – fertility is relatively high (Vitzhum, Reference Vitzthum2009; Vitzhum et al., Reference Vitzthum, Bentley, Spielvogel, Caceres, Thornburg, Jones and Chatterton2002). It has also been shown that the preference for femininity in women's faces is weaker in deprived populations and populations with worse health indices (De Barra et al., Reference De Barra, DeBruine, Jones, Mahmud and Curtis2013; Marcinkowska et al., Reference Marcinkowska, Kozlov, Cai, Contreras-Garduño, Dixson, Oana and Rantala2014; Penton-Voak et al., Reference Penton-Voak, Jacobson and Trivers2004).

With respect to preference for men's facial sexual dimorphism, the evidence is mixed. Some studies report preferences for less masculine features in men's faces (Perrett et al., Reference Perrett, Lee, Penton-Voak, Rowland, Yoshikawa, Burt and Akamatsu1998; Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Hickford and Jeffery2000), others show preferences for more masculine features (Foo, Simmons, et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, Hagel, Franklin, Fink and Grammer2001; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Rhodes and Simmons2008; Skrinda et al., Reference Skrinda, Krama, Kecko, Moore, Kaasik, Meija and Krams2014). A number of studies found preferences for neither masculine nor feminine features in male faces (Mogilski & Welling, Reference Mogilski and Welling2017; Penton-Voak & Chen, Reference Penton-Voak and Chen2004; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Pound, Stephen, Clark and Penton-Voak2010; Stephen et al., Reference Stephen, Scott, Coetzee, Pound, Perrett and Penton-Voak2012). It has been suggested that methodological differences in stimuli manipulation are at least in part responsible for such mixed results (Rennels et al., Reference Rennels, Bronstad and Langlois2008; Rhodes, Reference Rhodes2006), but it is also possible that distant populations actually differ in preferred male facial traits. The usual approach to addressing this variation in results is to test adaptive hypotheses on fitness outcomes of preference of certain traits across various populations (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Scott, Maklakov, Kasumovic, Clark and Penton-Voak2011; DeBruine et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Little, Crawford and Welling2011; DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, Welling and Little2010).

Masculine features may be cues to health. Men with masculine facial features have been considered more immunocompetent (Foo, Nakagawa, et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; cf. Nowak et al., Reference Nowak, Pawłowski, Borkowska, Augustyniak and Drulis-Kawa2018; Rantala et al., Reference Rantala, Moore, Skrinda, Krama, Kivleniece, Kecko and Krams2012) and healthier than their less masculine peers (Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Chan, Zebrowitz and Simmons2003; Thornhill & Gangestad, Reference Thornhill and Gangestad2006; see also Moore et al., Reference Moore, Cornwell, Law Smith, Al Dujaili, Sharp and Perrett2011; Rantala et al., Reference Rantala, Moore, Skrinda, Krama, Kivleniece, Kecko and Krams2012). It is thus hypothesised that by preferring more masculine men, women try to increase their chances of acquiring a healthier mate who would have more immunocompetent offspring.

Moreover, formidability and resource holding potential, i.e. an individual's willingness to engage in conflict over resources stemming from his/her ability to obtain or withdraw resources from a rival (Třebický et al., Reference Třebický, Stirrat and Havlíček2019), are also cued by masculine facial features (Boothroyd et al., Reference Boothroyd, Jones, Burt and Perrett2007; Carre & McCormick, Reference Carre and McCormick2008; Stirrat & Perrett, Reference Stirrat and Perrett2010; Swaddle & Reierson, Reference Swaddle and Reierson2002; Třebický et al., Reference Třebický, Fialová, Kleisner, Roberts, Little and Havlíček2015). Women who are endangered by unequal resource distribution (e.g. owing to economic inequality) may prefer more masculine men because such men are more likely to obtain resources for their families (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Scott, Maklakov, Kasumovic, Clark and Penton-Voak2011; Little et al., Reference Little, DeBruine and Jones2013). Moreover, the preference for masculine men seems adaptive because a formidable and dominant partner can better protect his mate against violence and harm (Ryder et al., Reference Ryder, Maltby, Rai, Jones and Flowe2016).

Nevertheless, masculinity is also linked to the risk of testosterone-associated antisocial behaviours (van Bokhoven et al., Reference van Bokhoven, van Goozen, van Engeland, Schaal, Arseneault, Séguin and Tremblay2006), higher divorce probability (Booth et al., Reference Booth, Dabbs and Dabbs1993; Mazur & Michalek, Reference Mazur and Michalek1998) and low partner fidelity (Penton-Voak & Chen, Reference Penton-Voak and Chen2004; Polo et al., Reference Polo, Muñoz-Reyes, Pita, Shackelford and Fink2019), which may all have a negative impact on parental investment. All in all, women seem to face a trade-off between choosing a masculine, immunocompetent and formidable mate, who could also harm and/or leave her, or a less masculine but cooperative and more nurturing mate. Such a trade-off may lack a universal solution across populations from distant countries.

1.2. The current study

Research on differences in the association between sexual dimorphism and perceived attractiveness usually builds on a single set of manipulated facial stimuli, which are then used for a number of sets of raters from distant populations in a hypothesis-driven research paradigm (DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Crawford, Welling and Little2010; Marcinkowska et al., Reference Marcinkowska, Rantala, Lee, Kozlov, Aavik, Cai and Dixson2019; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Clark, Josephson, Boyette, Cuthill, Fried and Penton-Voak2014). It could be objected, however, that manipulation of sexual dimorphism may affect stimuli features in a way that need not be ecologically relevant to all of the investigated populations. Moreover, evaluation of faces by raters from a visually distinctive population may cause people to perform worse on trait attribution (Anzures et al., Reference Anzures, Quinn, Pascalis, Slater, Tanaka and Lee2013). The use of local faces therefore has clear advantages.

There are multiple evolutionary-based hypotheses which aim to explain both the consensus and differences in preferences of various facial traits across people from distant populations (see above). Moreover, it is not clear whether raters across distant populations prefer similar traits in local faces and whether they interpret sexually dimorphic facial traits similarly.

We have therefore conducted a set of non-confirmatory analyses (following Scheel et al., Reference Scheel, Tiokhin, Isager and Lakens2020; see also Nakamura & Watanabe, Reference Nakamura and Watanabe2019) on the association between perceived facial attractiveness, perceived and measured sexual dimorphism and other facial traits across populations from five distant countries (Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia, Iran and Turkey). In each country, we used only facial stimuli collected within the local population. Using various facial feature metrics and path analysis, we also explored sample-specific patterns of serial and parallel mediation among the predictors of perceived characteristics. Specifically, we used predictors that may affect perceived attractiveness, perceived sexual dimorphism and measured sexual dimorphism, and mediate their relationship, namely morphological averageness, skin lightness, age, body mass index (BMI) and facial width to height ratio (fWHR) (see Supplementary Material, Sections S1.1–S1.4).

To avoid any confounding effects of stimuli manipulation on trait attribution (DeBruine, Jones, Smith, et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Smith and Little2010; see also Kleisner et al., Reference Kleisner, Pokorný and Saribay2019; Rennels et al., Reference Rennels, Bronstad and Langlois2008), we used unmanipulated facial stimuli. Moreover, we used two measures of facial sexual dimorphism: (a) perceived sex-typicality (perceived femininity in women and perceived masculinity in men); and (b) sexual shape dimorphism of facial shape calculated from landmark-based geometric morphometrics. According to a recent study, these two measures of facial sexual dimorphism are only moderately correlated (Mitteroecker et al., Reference Mitteroecker, Windhager, Møller and Schaefer2015), presumably because perceived sex-typicality is also affected by skin lightness across populations (Carrito & Semin, Reference Carrito and Semin2019; van den Berghe & Frost, Reference van den Berghe and Frost1986).

Although this study is of an exploratory nature, we made several predictions (see Table 3a): based on previous studies on the association between perceived and morphological sexual dimorphism (Komori et al., Reference Komori, Kawamura and Ishihara2011; Mitteroecker et al., Reference Mitteroecker, Windhager, Møller and Schaefer2015), we expect that perceived and measured sexual dimorphism will be correlated in every sampled population positively, albeit weakly (r ≈ 0.3). Moreover, morphological averageness of facial configurations should be moderately positively associated with perceived attractiveness (Jones & Jaeger, Reference Jones and Jaeger2019).

Skin lightness may affect perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness mainly in Cameroon, given that people of sub-Saharan African origin are more sensitive to facial skin colouration and lightness variability as a cue for both facial attractiveness (Kleisner et al., Reference Kleisner, Kočnar, Tureček, Stella, Akoko, Třebický and Havlíček2017) and ethnic typicality (Strom et al., Reference Strom, Zebrowitz, Zhang, Bronstad and Lee2012). Moreover, skin lightness has also been associated with perceived male sex-typicality and healthiness in previous research based on people of European origin (Carrito & Semin, Reference Carrito and Semin2019). Skin lightness should therefore negatively relate to perceived masculinity in male faces in all five samples.

We further predict that perceived femininity of women's faces will be associated with higher ratings of attractiveness across all five population samples (Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia, Iran and Turkey) regardless of their mutual distance and eventual cultural differences. It has been demonstrated that perceived sex-typicality of women is an important component of their perceived facial attractiveness. We therefore predict a strong association between perceived attractiveness and perceived femininity in women in samples from all of the five populations (r ≈ 0.8; see Foo, Simmons, et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; Koehler et al., Reference Koehler, Simmons, Rhodes and Peters2004).

Concerning the preference for male sex-typicality, there is substantial disagreement across populations from distant countries (Marcinkowska et al., Reference Marcinkowska, Rantala, Lee, Kozlov, Aavik, Cai and Dixson2019) and even between same-country samples recruited from or primed to different socioeconomic conditions (DeBruine et al., Reference DeBruine, Jones, Little, Crawford and Welling2011; Little, Cohen, et al., Reference Little, Cohen, Jones and Belsky2007). This suggests the existence of various factors that may shape masculinity preferences uniquely in each country. We sampled populations from only five countries, which is why we have only five data points on the cross-population level and therefore cannot anticipate which of those forces would drive preferences in our samples. All predictions are summarised in Table 3a.

2. Methods

For detailed descriptions of stimuli acquisition, measurements, rating procedures and analyses, see Supplementary Materials in the OSF entry for this study at https://osf.io/va8pg/?view_only=021e11321855463f82fc64e6ceb5716a.

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Charles University, Faculty of Science. The photographed individuals and raters were informed about the purpose of data collection. Photographed individuals signed informed consent and raters consented by clicking ‘I agree’ in the questionnaire.

2.1. Facial photos acquisition

As stimuli, we collected a total of 709 standardised frontal facial photographs (357 men and 352 women) from five countries (Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia, Iran and Turkey). Data about age, height and body weight were collected from participants in Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia and Turkey, while only information about age was collected from Iranian participants. We used a pre-existing available photograph dataset of Iranian faces that has been acquired prior to the decision to use those photographs in this research and which lacked the information on the height and weight of the models. The effect of relative weight therefore could not be tested in the Iranian sample. The number of stimuli per sample and sex and samples’ descriptives are summarised in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the stimuli sample

fWHR, Facial width to height ratio; BMI, body mass index; CIELab L, skin lightness; SShD, sexual shape dimorphism (measured facial sexual dimorphism); Avrg, morphometrical averageness; Fem/masc, perceived femininity(women)/masculinity(men); Attr, perceived attractiveness; SD, standard deviation; ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient (measure of inter-rater agreement), ICC (3,k) if not otherwise stated. a Cameroonian samples from 2013 and 2016 were rated separately, ICCs are as follows: 2013 Sample/2016 Sample

The Cameroonian stimuli were collected in two separate runs (2013 and 2016) and these two sets were also rated separately. For the purpose of this study, the two Cameroonian samples were combined, but analyses of the separate samples yielded results similar to analyses of the combined sample (the alternative analyses are presented in Table S3 and Figure S5 in the Supplementary Material.).

The photographs were taken with digital cameras, using external light sources and homogeneous white or grey backgrounds. Lighting conditions were not standardised across the samples but were uniform within each sample (each country). All participants were asked to remove their glasses, facial jewellery, other adornments or cosmetics, adopt a neutral facial expression and look directly into the camera. All women in the Iranian set wore a hijab that covered hair, ears and part of their cheeks.

All photographs were adjusted to set the eyes horizontally at the same height and leave approximately the same length of neck visible. They were subsequently cropped and exported at ~500 × 700 px resolution; see Section S2.1 in the Supplementary Material for further details of photo acquisition.

The stimuli were sampled by convenience and they do not represent the populations as a whole. Nonetheless, each sample represents similar social strata within the society (young to middle-aged people, mostly university students, academic staff and members of the general public who were willing to ‘help the science’). The composition of the samples was therefore comparable across the sampled populations.

2.2. Rating sessions

Raters from each country assessed only stimuli from their own country. The ratings were self-paced and took part online (except for attractiveness of Cameroonian 2013 stimuli) using participants’ own electronic devices. Stimuli appeared on the screen in a pseudo-randomised order. The questionnaires were in English (Cameroon), Spanish (Colombia), Czech (Czechia), Farsi (Iran), and Turkish (Turkey). Raters were not compensated for their participation. Attractiveness was rated only by opposite-sex raters except for Turkey, where both male and female facial stimuli were rated by raters of both sexes (for alternative analyses with the subset of opposite-sex raters, see Table S4 and Figure S5 in the Supplementary Material). Perceived masculinity and femininity in the Cameroonian, Colombian, and Turkish sample were rated by raters of both sexes, while in Czechia and Iran, perceived masculinity and femininity were rated only by persons of the opposite sex. The number of stimuli in each country is reported in Table 1. The number of raters in each category and associated descriptive statistics are summarised in Table 2 and in Table S1 in the Supplementary Material. A detailed description of the rating process is in the Supplementary Material (Section S2.2, ‘Acquisition and processing of ratings’).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the rater sample

a In the Cameroonian and Turkish samples, perceived sex-typicality (masculinity of men, femininity of women) was rated by a combined set of male and female raters.

b Some Turkish raters did not report their attributes (age, sex, weight and height). For alternative analyses without those raters and split by raters’ sex, see Table S4 and Figure S5 in the online Supplementary Material.

Cameroonian stimuli from 2013 and 2016 were rated in two separate runs. Of all the datasets, only the perceived attractiveness of Cameroonian stimuli collected in 2013 was not rated using Qualtrics. It was rated in an offline purpose-made ‘ImageRater’ program visually similar to the Qualtrics interface. Cameroonian raters assessed the stimuli on a verbally anchored seven-point Likert scale (1, not at all attractive/masculine/feminine; 7, very attractive/masculine/feminine face). Each rater rated all the stimuli within a given set. Raters were mostly university students who resided in towns and villages of western Cameroonian provinces.

In Colombia, raters were recruited by JDL and his co-workers. They were mostly university students from Bogota, D.C. The sample consisted of 997 raters (565 women). They rated only a randomly chosen opposite-sex subset (N = 20) of the stimuli. The rating scale ranged from 1.0 to 10.0 (with one decimal place). The endpoints were also anchored verbally, with 1.0 not at all attractive/masculine/feminine and 10.0 very attractive/masculine/feminine face. All participants identified themselves as heterosexual.

Czech raters were recruited by fliers in university buildings, asked face to face or by email by two of the authors (VF, KK) and KK's other co-workers. The link was also shared via social networks through groups of Charles University students and academic staff. Only raters who identified themselves as heterosexual and completed the whole questionnaire were entered into the analysis.

Iranian raters (see Section S.2.2.4 in the online Supplementary Material) were recruited via email with a link to the questionnaires sent by one of the authors (FP), who also translated the original questionnaires from English into Farsi. Raters who identified themselves as homosexual, did not complete the entire questionnaire or rated all stimuli identically were excluded from subsequent analyses.

In Turkey, the rating data were collected online by one of the authors (AS). Participants were asked to rate a subset of the Turkish face database (Bogazici Face Database; see Saribay et al., Reference Saribay, Biten, Meral, Aldan, Trebicky and Kleisner2018). Students from the Bogazici University were deliberately excluded from the pool of potential raters owing to potential bias stemming from familiarity with the stimuli. Raters saw only a subset of stimuli (N = 8 faces per rated attribute) in pseudorandomised order. They rated both the male and female stimuli.

2.3. Facial measures

2.3.1. Skin lightness

In the Cameroonian (2013), Colombian, Iranian and Turkish targets, we measured skin lightness from facial photographs using ImageJ software (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Rasband and Eliceiri2012) with the plugin Color Transformer 2. In the Cameroon 2016 and Czech sample, facial skin lightness was measured in vivo with a spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics Flame-S, 200–850 nm, optical resolution 2 nm). All measurements were taken on three patches of the target face (forehead, left and right cheek) and expressed as the L* dimension of CIELab colour space (Hunter, Reference Hunter1958). According to Coetzee et al. (Reference Coetzee, Faerber, Greeff, Lefevre, Re and Perrett2012), such difference in the lightness measurement (in vivo by spectrophotometer vs. using facial photographs) should not affect the results. Nevertheless, we also ran the analysis with L* measured from photos in ImageJ in samples where lightness was originally based on in vivo spectrophotometric measurements. Results for these analyses are available in the Supplementary Material (Table S5 and Figure S6). The results were not substantially affected by the method of skin lightness measurement.

2.3.2. Relative facial width

We measured bizygomatic facial width and upper facial height from the glabella to the border of the upper lip from facial photographs of the stimuli persons (Třebický et al., Reference Třebický, Fialová, Kleisner, Roberts, Little and Havlíček2015). Then we calculated the fWHR as facial width divided by facial height (see also Section S2.4.2 in the Supplementary Material for a detailed description of the measurement).

2.3.3. Facial shape analysis

We manually landmarked each facial photograph with 72 landmarks (36 landmarks and 36 semilandmarks) in tpsDig2 software, version 2.31 (Rohlf, Reference Rohlf2015). We followed the definitions of standard landmarks positions previously used by Kleisner et al. (Reference Kleisner, Pokorný and Saribay2019). Procrustes superimposition of all landmark configurations was done using the ‘gpagen()’ function in the R package Geomorph (Adams & Otárola-Castillo, Reference Adams and Otárola-Castillo2013). Semilandmark positions were optimised based on minimising the Procrustes distances between corresponding points. For a more detailed description of facial shape analysis, see Section S.2.4.4 of the Supplementary Material.

We computed facial morphological averageness as each face's distance from the average facial configuration of its bearer's sex and population sample (e.g. averageness of Turkish male targets). The higher the value, the less average the facial configuration.

Next, we computed the level of facial sexual shape dimorphism (morphological sexual dimorphism) by projecting each facial configuration on a vector connecting the male and female average within a given sample (e.g. Turkish male and female targets – the scale uses both sexes within a given population sample). Higher negative values of sexual shape dimorphism indicate a more female-like facial shape, while higher positive values indicate a more male-like facial shape.

Note that all Iranian women in our sample wore the hijab. To compute the averageness of Iranian women and sexual shape dimorphism of Iranian men and women, we used the configuration of 49 innermost facial landmarks. Computations of morphological facial averageness and sexual shape dimorphism in the remaining groups were always based on all 72 landmarks.

2.4. Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using the R software for statistical computing (version 3.6.0; R Core Team, 2020). All datasets and R script are available at https://osf.io/va8pg/?view_only=021e11321855463f82fc64e6ceb5716a.

To assess interrater agreement on perceived attractiveness and sex-typicality, we computed Intraclass correlations using the ‘ICC()’ function of the ‘psych’ package (Revelle, Reference Revelle2018). We ran two-way mixed average score Intraclass correlations (3,k) for the Cameroonian, Czech and Iranian datasets where raters saw all targets. Raters in Colombia and Turkey saw only a subsample of the relevant targets, which is why for these two samples, we ran one-way random, average score Intraclass correlations (1,k) (Shrout & Fleiss, Reference Shrout and Fleiss1979). All intraclass correlations were very high (ICC > 0.9, see Table 1). For subsequent analyses, we calculated mean perceived attractiveness and perceived sex-typicality ratings for each photographed person.

Two-tailed Pearson's correlation coefficients (and their 95% confidence intervals) were used to investigate bivariate associations between all collected variables. The resulting p-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini–Hochberg correction procedure. Unlike the Bonferroni correction, which controls for familywise error rate, Benjamini–Hochberg correction controls for the predicted (expected) proportion of errors among rejected null hypotheses, that is, the false discovery rate (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). It is therefore better suited for a non-confirmatory analysis where there is no dependence between multiple comparisons. The results of performed correlations and descriptive statistics (mean, SD, and range) for all variables are presented in Tables S2–S5 in the Supplementary Material. The associations were further visualised using heatmaps of Pearson correlation coefficients (see Figure S4 in Supplementary Material) using the ‘col_fun()’ function of the circlize package (Gu et al., Reference Gu, Gu, Eils, Schlesner and Brors2014) and the ‘Heatmap()’ function of the ComplexHeatmap package (Gu et al., Reference Gu, Eils and Schlesner2016) in R software, and subsequently edited in InkScape version 0.92.4. To visualise the strength of associations between measures of sexual dimorphism (SShD and perceived sex-typicality) and perceived attractiveness across the five samples, we created a forest plot (see Figure 1) using the ‘forestplot()’ function of the forestplot package (Gordon & Lumley, Reference Gordon and Lumley2020).

Figure 1. Forest plots displaying the relative strength of Pearson's correlations between perceived attractiveness and perceived sex-typicality (a), perceived attractiveness and sexual shape dimorphism (b) and between sexual shape dimorphism and perceived sex-typicality (c), with confidence intervals of each coefficient. Each row corresponds to a single sample (women from all five samples, men from all five samples, with sampled countries in alphabetical order). Blue circles represent Pearson's correlation coefficients (mean estimate on the given sample), while black lines stand for error bars defined as 95% confidence intervals around each mean correlation. A vertical line in zero (‘0’) enables us to inspect whether the confidence interval for a given correlation contains zero. The columns on the right side of the diagrams show coefficients of the associations. This figure facilitates a comparison of bivariate associations among the population samples.

To explore causal relationships between age, skin lightness, fWHR, BMI, averageness, morphological sexual shape dimorphism, perceived sex-typicality and perceived attractiveness, we ran path analyses with ‘sem()’ function from the lavaan package for R (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). We conducted a separate path analysis for each stimulus sex/sample combination (e.g. Iranian female stimuli). Perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness were set as dependent variables (see Figure 2), while directionality of mutual interdependence between the two was not decided and we treated it as a correlation; for a detailed description of the model development, see Section S.2.5 in the Supplementary Material. Paths designated in this study were based on evidence from literature, formal logic and our decisions (see also Kane & Ashbaugh, Reference Kane and Ashbaugh2017). The aim of the study was to explore the proposed paths, not to confirm a pattern as an objective causality. Because the number of observations per estimated parameter was relatively low, robust p-values were obtained using a permutation test with 10,000 iterations, where the full models were estimated on randomised datasets.

Figure 2. A visualisation of path analyses (multiple regression models) between the rated facial attributes (perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness) and facial measures ordered by the sex of the stimuli. Arrows represent the direction of the association. Non-significant paths are omitted. Association between perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness was treated as a correlation (i.e. the direction was not specified). Numbers next to the paths indicate the estimate of regression or correlation coefficient in a corresponding model with standardised variables. Red colour denotes a negative coefficient. The graph shows to what extent is the observed within-sample variability of each variable explained by other variable(s). In every sample, perceived femininity and attractiveness are closely mutually associated in the women's samples. In most population samples, perceived masculinity was not associated with perceived men's attractiveness. The significant paths mostly replicate significant Pearson's correlations (see Figure S4) (+ p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001).

3. Results

3.1. Correlational analyses

In women, we found a significant positive correlation between perceived attractiveness and perceived femininity in all five samples: Cameroonian, r(98) = 0.77; 95% CI [0.68, 0.84], p < 0.001; Colombian, r(64) = 0.84 [0.76, 0.90], p < 0.001; Czech, r(48) = 0.87 [0.78, 0.92], p < 0.001; Iranian, r(41) = 0.93 [0.88, 0.96], p < 0.001; and Turkish, r(92) = 0.89 [0.84, 0.93], p < 0.001. Furthermore, in the sample of Cameroonian women, both perceived attractiveness and perceived femininity were significantly and positively correlated with skin lightness (r(98) = 0.31 [0.12, 0.47], p = 0.014; and r(98) = 0.36 [0.17, 0.52], p = 0.004, respectively). In the rest of women's samples, the correlation between skin lightness and perceived femininity or attractiveness was not significant (p > 0.05, p-value after Benjamini–Hochberg correction; see Figure S4).

In men, perceived masculinity and attractiveness were significantly and positively correlated only in the Colombian and Czech sample (r(70) = 0.44 [0.23, 0.61], p = 0.004; r(48) = 0.48 [0.23, 0.67], p = 0.013, respectively). In samples of the Cameroonian, Iranian and Turkish male faces, perceived masculinity was significantly negatively associated with facial skin lightness (r(98) = −0.38 [−0.54, −0.20], p = 0.001; r(42) = −0.63 [−0.78, −0.40], p < 0.001; r(89) = 0.32 [−0.49, −0.12], p = 0.009, respectively), meaning that darker men were perceived as more masculine.

Morphological averageness was significantly correlated with perceived attractiveness in Iranian men (r(42) = −0.41 [−0.63, −0.13], p = 0.028), with perceived femininity (r(48) = −0.44 [−0.64, −0.19], p = 0.011) and attractiveness (r(48) = −0.52 [−0.70, −0.28], p = 0.002) in Czech women, and with perceived femininity (r(92) = −0.29 [−0.46, −0.09], p = 0.03) and attractiveness (r(92) = −0.34 [−0.51, −0.14], p = 0.007) in Turkish women. In these samples, more average faces (i.e. ‘averageness’ values closer to zero) were thus perceived as more sex-typical and/or attractive.

Finally, sexual shape dimorphism was significantly correlated with perceived masculinity in the sample of Cameroonian men (r(98) = 0.33 [0.15, 0.50], p = 0.005), meaning that men with more male-like facial shape were perceived as more masculine. Sexual shape dimorphism also correlated with perceived femininity in Colombian women (r(64) = −0.33 [−0.53, −0.10], p = 0.045), implying that women with more female-like facial shape were perceived as more feminine, with perceived femininity (r(41) = −0.45 [−0.66, −0.18], p = 0.016) and attractiveness (r(41) = −0.43 [−0.65, −0.15], p = 0.022) in Iranian women, and with perceived masculinity of Turkish men (r(89) = 0.44 [0.26, 0.59], p < 0.001). In the rest of the samples, the association between sexual shape dimorphism and perceived sex-typicality was not significant after Benjamini–Hochberg correction (see Figure S4). Figure 1 presents forest plots for correlations between perceived sex-typicality, sexual shape dimorphism and perceived attractiveness for each population sample and sex. All correlation coefficients are reported in detail in Figure S4 and Table S2–S5 in the Supplementary Material.

3.2. Path analyses

3.2.1. Women's faces

Significant paths among variables mostly replicated the significant correlations reported above. In all women's samples, there were significant positive residual correlations between perceived femininity and attractiveness (r = 0.75; 95% CI [0.56, 0.94] for Cameroonian, 0.82 [0.57, 1.00] for Colombian, 0.80 [0.59, 1.00] for Czech, 0.91 [0.65, 1.00] for Iranian and 0.89 [0.66, 1.00] for Turkish women, p < 0.001 in all cases).

In Cameroonian women, there was a significant positive association (partial regression) between facial skin lightness and both perceived femininity and attractiveness (β = 0.35 [0.18, 0.52], p = 0.001; β = 0.29 [0.11, 0.47], p = 0.004, respectively).

Concerning correlations between facial morphology and perceived traits, we found a significant association between sexual shape dimorphism and perceived femininity in the Colombian and Iranian women's sample (β = −0.30 [−0.53, −0.07], p = 0.019; β = −0.50 [−0.74, −0.25], p = 0.001, respectively), while in the Iranian women's sample, sexual shape dimorphism was significantly associated with perceived attractiveness (β = −0.47 [−0.71, −0.22], p = 0.002). More feminine facial morphology had a more negative value of sexual shape dimorphism, which indicates that feminine facial shape was perceived as more attractive in Iran and as more feminine in the Iranian and Colombian women's sample. In Colombia, partial regression between sexual shape dimorphism and perceived attractiveness was not significant (as noted above, we report the ‘robust p-values’ based on bootstrapping) but was of a similar magnitude and direction (β = −0.25 [−0.49, −0.02], p = 0.051). In the Czech women's sample, morphological facial averageness was significantly associated with both perceived femininity and attractiveness (more average faces were perceived as more feminine and attractive, β = −0.47 [−0.70, −0.25], p = 0.001; β = −0.53 [−0.74, −0.33], p < 0.001, respectively). The same held true for the Turkish female sample (β = −0.30 [−0.49, −0.11], p = 0.002 for the association between morphological averageness and perceived femininity; β = −0.34 [−0.53, −0.15], p < 0.001 between morphological averageness and perceived attractiveness).

Perceived facial attractiveness and perceived femininity in women's samples were also related to other variables: in Colombia, women with a higher BMI were perceived as significantly less feminine (β = −0.28 [−0.52, −0.05], p = 0.026) and less attractive (β = −0.25 [−0.50, −0.01], p = 0.041), while in the Cameroonian women's sample, age was significantly negatively associated with perceived femininity (β = −0.26 [−0.44, −0.08], p = 0.017).

Concerning associations between the predictors themselves, in Cameroonian (β = 0.28 [0.08, 0.47], p = 0.005), Colombian (β = 0.33 [0.10, 0.56], p = 0.010) and Turkish women's samples (β = 0.23 [0.04, 0.42], p = 0.029), fWHR was significantly positively associated with BMI. In Cameroon (β = 0.27 [0.08, 0.46], p = 0.008) and Turkey (β = 0.39 [0.21, 0.58], p < 0.001), women with a higher fWHR exhibited lower levels of female-like sexual shape dimorphism, while in Czechia, women with higher fWHR exhibited higher levels of female-like SShD (β = −0.33 [−0.59, −0.07], p = 0.025). Age was significantly associated with BMI (β = 0.24 [0.05, 0.43], p = 0.025) only in the Cameroonian women's sample and with fWHR in the Iranian (β = 0.46 [0.19, 0.72], p = 0.002) and Turkish women's sample (β = −0.27 [−0.46, −0.08], p = 0.007).

3.2.2. Men's faces

Perceived masculinity of Colombian and Czech male faces was significantly positively correlated with their perceived attractiveness (r = 0.46 [0.24, 0.67], p < 0.001 and r = 0.62 [0.36, 0.88], p < 0.001, respectively). In samples from the other populations, the association between perceived attractiveness and perceived masculinity was not significant (r = −0.07 [−0.22, 0.07], p = 0.57; r = 0.16 [−0.02, 0.33], p = 0.53; and r = 0.19 [0.03, 0.36], p = 0.14 for Cameroonian, Iranian and Turkish male faces, respectively). Facial skin lightness was significantly negatively associated with perceived masculinity in the Cameroonian (β = −0.33 [−0.49, −0.18], p = 0.001), Colombian (β = −0.24 [−0.44, −0.03], p = 0.041) and Iranian (β = −0.50 [−0.70, −0.30], p = 0.001) male samples.

Concerning morphological and perceived facial traits, the following significant associations were observed: in the Cameroonian (β = 0.36 [0.19, 0.54], p < 0.001), Colombian (β = 0.30 [0.07, 0.53], p = 0.014) and Turkish sample (β = 0.45 [0.24, 0.66], p < 0.001) more male-like facial shapes (sexual shape dimorphism) were perceived as more masculine (perceived masculinity). In the Iranian (β = −0.42 [−0.68, −0.15], p = 0.007) and Turkish samples (β = −0.23 [−0.43, −0.04], p = 0.024), facial configurations closer to the average (morphological averageness) were perceived as more attractive.

Body mass index was significantly positively associated with fWHR in Cameroonian (β = 0.26 [0.07, 0.45], p = 0.009), Colombian (β = 0.23 [0.01, 0.45], p = 0.048), Czech (β = 0.29 [0.01, 0.57], p = 0.041) and Turkish (β = 0.36 [0.16, 0.55], p < 0.001) men, meaning that relatively heavier men had relatively wider faces (in the Iranian stimuli, we did not measure weight and height and thus could not compute this association). In Cameroonian men, fWHR was significantly associated with morphological averageness (β = −0.24 [−0.43, −0.04], p = 0.019) and sexual shape dimorphism (β = 0.39 [0.21, 0.58], p < 0.001). The fWHR was also significantly positively related to sexual shape dimorphism in Iranian (β = 0.57 [0.33, 0.81], p < 0.001) and Turkish men (β = 0.40 [0.22, 0.59], p < 0.001).

See also Figures S5 and S6 in the Supplementary Material for path analyses with alternative variables and datasets. Table S6 in the Supplementary Material reports the full ‘lavaan’ output for all fitted path analyses.

4. Discussion

In this study, we investigated the associations between perceived attractiveness, perceived sex-typicality and facial sexual shape dimorphism (morphological sexual dimorphism) in samples from five distant populations, namely Cameroon, Colombia, Czechia, Iran and Turkey. We also explored whether these associations were affected by other factors (morphological facial averageness, skin lightness, relative facial width, body mass and age).

As predicted (see Table 3), raters strongly preferred women who were perceived as more feminine in all samples. On the other hand, raters did not agree on the preferred degree of perceived male sex-typicality. In fact, only raters from the Czech and Colombian samples preferred men who were perceived as more masculine. Morphometric variables (facial morphological averageness, sexual shape dimorphism), relative facial width and measured skin lightness were inconsistent predictors of perceived scales: they predicted perceived characteristics only in a subset of the samples.

Table 3. Outline of predictions (a) and significant results (b)

1 Predictions within a given sample (e.g. darker/lighter within an Iranian male sample).

2 Predictions and results based on age and BMI are not further discussed because they go beyond the scope of the current study. For a more detailed review and anticipated effects of variables which were not discussed in the Introduction, see Sections S1 and S2.5 in Supplementary Material.

3 Significant result for Attractiveness|Perceived Sex-typicality; N out of five female and five male samples, from four samples for each sex for BMI.

4.1. Association between perceived sex-typicality and perceived attractiveness

The hypothesis according to which preferred visual facial traits should be invariant across samples from distant populations (Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Kalakanis, Rubenstein, Larson, Hallam and Smoot2000) found support in our results only in part. In all five female samples, perceived attractiveness and perceived women's femininity were positively associated. Concerning preference for femininity, our results thus converge with previous evidence to the effect that perceived female femininity is preferred across populations (Little et al., Reference Little, Jones and DeBruine2011), except for cases where the sampled populations are distant, visually distinctive and/or inhabit various environments (cf. Marcinkowska et al., Reference Marcinkowska, Kozlov, Cai, Contreras-Garduño, Dixson, Oana and Rantala2014).

Such high correlation between perceived femininity and attractiveness conforms to the evolutionary model and its implicit assumptions. It has been proposed, mostly based on indirect evidence, that femininity in women's faces presents an honest cue to proper hormone-based development (Probst et al., Reference Probst, Bobst and Lobmaier2016; Thornhill & Grammer, Reference Thornhill and Grammer1999), fertility and reproductive health (Law Smith et al., Reference Law Smith, Perrett, Jones, Cornwell, Moore, Feinberg and Hillier2006) and should be therefore preferred. Moreover, our results across samples replicate findings of recent studies which identified a strong association between facial femininity and attractiveness using various research design (Foo, Simmons et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; Mogilski & Welling, Reference Mogilski and Welling2017; Muñoz-Reyes et al., Reference Muñoz-Reyes, Iglesias-Julios, Pita and Turiegano2015; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jones, Debruine and Little2009). Contrary to this assumption of universal preference of female femininity, other scholars suggested that in harsh environments (De Barra et al., Reference De Barra, DeBruine, Jones, Mahmud and Curtis2013; Penton-Voak et al., Reference Penton-Voak, Jacobson and Trivers2004) and small rural populations (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Clark, Josephson, Boyette, Cuthill, Fried and Penton-Voak2014), preference for female sex-typicality should be weaker or absent altogether. In our study, however, the magnitude of femininity preference was relatively stable across the samples from distant populations. Accordingly, female femininity, eventually pointing to sexual maturity and reproductive health, may present a women's characteristic that is universally preferred.

For men, we observed a substantial variation across samples in the magnitude of association between perceived masculinity and attractiveness. The results therefore do not support unequivocal conclusions that either masculine (Foo, Simmons, et al., Reference Foo, Simmons and Rhodes2017; Skrinda et al., Reference Skrinda, Krama, Kecko, Moore, Kaasik, Meija and Krams2014) or relatively more feminine (Perrett et al., Reference Perrett, Lee, Penton-Voak, Rowland, Yoshikawa, Burt and Akamatsu1998; Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Hickford and Jeffery2000) facial traits are universally preferred in male faces. Perceived masculinity was considered attractive only in two of the samples, the Czech and the Colombian one. In the Iranian and Turkish sample, the association was also positive but not statistically significant.

Preference for masculinity might be the result of different adaptive processes in Czech and Colombian society but it is also possible that the results converge owing to similarities between our Czech and Colombian sample (not the whole populations). In two studies, Borras-Guevara et al. (Reference Borras-Guevara, Batres and Perrett2017a, Reference Borras-Guevara, Batres and Perrettb) found a negative association between Colombian women's masculinity preference in manipulated male faces and Colombian women's fear of domestic violence. Importantly, raters in these studies represented various social strata of the Colombian society. Our targets and raters, on the other hand, were for the most part university students from Bogota. It is thus likely that they represented a relatively affluent social group where preference for masculine male partners is not counteracted by fear of domestic violence. The issue of within-populations variance of preferences could, however, be addressed only in a study that would measure and compare women's relative safety from male violence, wealth distribution, and the perception of these environmental factors in both the Czech and Colombian societies.

Women's preferences in Middle Eastern countries in our sample (Iran and Turkey) were predicted neither by sexual shape dimorphism nor by perceived sex-typicality. Both Iranian and Turkish female raters preferred morphological averageness of facial shape in the male stimuli.

Future studies should explore this observed absence of preference for sex-typicality in Middle East countries with regard to locally important cultural factors. In particular, it should be taken into account that in Middle Eastern countries, the tradition of arranged marriages is still widespread, as is women's subordinate social role and strong interpersonal bonds (Cindoglu et al., Reference Cindoglu, Çemrek, Toktas, Zencirci, Hennon and Wilson2011; Friedland et al., Reference Friedland, Afary, Gardinali and Naslund2016). Parents and other relatives who negotiate and arrange a marriage may not perceive sex-typicality as highly important for their choice. Nonetheless, recent evidence shows that people in both countries choose their partners substantially more freely than in the past (Hart, Reference Hart2007; Honarvar et al., Reference Honarvar, Salehi, Barfi, Asadi, Honarvar, Odoomi and Lankarani2016; Atari & Jamali, Reference Atari and Jamali2016a, Reference Atari and Jamalib), which might imply that the preference for average facial configurations displayed by our local raters could mirror local adaptations in actual mate choice, unbiased by arranged marriages.

4.2. Association between shape variables and perceived characteristics

Previous studies revealed a weak to moderate association between perceived and morphological sexual dimorphism (Komori et al., Reference Komori, Kawamura and Ishihara2011; Mitteroecker et al., Reference Mitteroecker, Windhager, Møller and Schaefer2015). In the current study, neither perceived femininity nor perceived masculinity and attractiveness were associated with sexual shape dimorphism (morphological sexual dimorphism) universally across all samples. When restricted only to significant effects in path analyses, more female-like shape indicated more attractive ratings only in one of the five women's samples (Iranian). In two populations (Colombian and Iranian), women with more female-like facial shape were also perceived as more feminine. In men, a more male-like facial configuration was predictive of higher perceived masculinity in three of the five samples (Cameroonian, Colombian, and Turkish). All in all, we thus found no universal association between perceived and measured sex-typicality in either sex, although in some of the samples, the association ran in the predicted direction.

Facial morphological averageness did not predict perceived characteristics consistently either. In three of the women's samples (Czech, Iranian and Turkish) and one men's sample (Turkish), more average facial configurations were perceived as more attractive. In the Czech and Turkish women sample, more average facial configurations were perceived as more feminine. As suggested by current research, it is possible that facial averageness is a trait that is relatively less important for the perception of facial characteristics than previously thought (Foo, Simmons, et al., Reference Foo, Nakagawa, Rhodes and Simmons2017; Holzleitner et al., Reference Holzleitner, Lee, Hahn, Kandrik, Bovet, Renoult and Jones2019; Jones & Jaeger, Reference Jones and Jaeger2019). It seems, therefore, that morphological variables beyond actual dimorphism are not good predictors of the perceived facial attractiveness and perceived sex-typicality.

Measured morphometric variables used in this study express facial shape variance as a single number. Human perception is not, however, a computational device that processes shape in that way. As suggested by plastic surgery, cosmetics, and related fields, some parameters of facial shape are more important for perceived attractiveness than others. Such features include relative lip size, lower face size (Penna et al., Reference Penna, Fricke, Iblher, Eisenhardt and Stark2015) and eyebrow size and shape (Schreiber et al., Reference Schreiber, Singh and Klatsky2005). Moreover, preferred traits may be a combination of mature, neotenous and expressive traits (Borelli & Bernerburg, Reference Borelli and Berneburg2010; Cunningham et al., Reference Cunningham, Barbee and Pike1990), not just juvenile/submissive traits (in women) and mature/dominant traits (in men). Taken together, it is unlikely that the complex phenomenon of human facial trait assessment could be fully captured by a single morphometric measure.

4.3. Sexual dimorphism in the fWHR

The fWHR was positively associated with sexual shape dimorphism in Cameroonian and Turkish faces of both sexes and in Czech female and Iranian male faces. Such positive association implies that Cameroonian and Turkish men and women with more masculine facial configurations had relatively wider faces. Past studies identified a slight sexual dimorphism in fWHR (with men having relatively wider faces; Carré & McCormick, Reference Carre and McCormick2008), but a more recent study cast doubt on sexual dimorphism in fWHR when it ran analyses that controlled for sexual dimorphism in body size (Kramer, Reference Kramer2015). Özener (Reference Özener2012) found no sexual dimorphism in fWHR in a Turkish university student sample. Although fWHR is no longer considered a sexually dimorphic measure, our data suggest that fWHR is associated with more male sex-typical facial shape at least in some of our samples. Except for the sample of Czech women, fWHR was also significantly positively associated with BMI in all samples for which BMI was available. In short, it thus turned out that relatively heavier people also have wider faces regardless of their sex.

4.4. Association between skin lightness and perceived characteristics

We further predicted that skin lightness, which has been associated with perceived sex-typicality and healthiness in previous research (Carrito & Semin, Reference Carrito and Semin2019; Stephen & Perrett, Reference Stephen, Perrett, Elliot, Fairchild and Franklin2016), should be positively related to perceived attractiveness and sex-typicality (with darker men being perceived as more masculine) in all the five population samples. In the samples of Cameroonian, Colombian and Iranian men, faces with a darker skin were perceived as more masculine. In the Czech and Turkish men's sample, facial skin lightness was not associated with perceived masculinity but the statistically non-significant associations for both Czech (β = −0.15) and Turkish (β = −0.13) men were in the same direction as in the rest of the samples. Owing to a low statistical power of our study, we cannot decide whether this association is ecologically irrelevant in these two countries or whether there indeed exists a stable association between darker facial skin and perceived sex-typicality across populations.

Regarding women, skin lightness was significantly associated with perceived facial characteristics only in the Cameroonian female sample. Raters perceived Cameroonian women with a lighter complexion as being both more feminine (perceived femininity) and more attractive. This observation is in line with previous studies which identified facial skin colouration as a more important cue to the perception of facial attributes in African than in non-African populations (Coetzee et al., Reference Coetzee, Greeff, Stephen and Perrett2014; Kleisner et al., Reference Kleisner, Kočnar, Tureček, Stella, Akoko, Třebický and Havlíček2017; Strom et al., Reference Strom, Zebrowitz, Zhang, Bronstad and Lee2012).

4.5. Limitations of the study

The aim of the current study was to explore the association between perceived and measured facial traits in samples from five distant countries. To acquire comparable data and results from each investigated country, we tried to keep the methodology of data collection as similar as possible. Despite these efforts, we did not manage to maintain all photo acquisition parameters identical throughout (e.g. camera sensor size or camera-to-subject distance), nor were we able to secure identical lighting conditions in all samples. Within each sample, however, photo acquisition standards were identical and maintained.

The questionnaire for Colombian raters differed in the granularity of the rating scale: in Colombia, the scale ranged from 1.0 to 10.0 with one decimal place, while elsewhere raters responded using seven-point Likert scales. On the other hand, we analysed all data within-sample rather than aggregating them across the samples and report standardised coefficients. There is, therefore, no reason to suspect that scale variability affected our results.

Electronic devices used by raters may have caused some differences in ratings owing to, for instance, differences in the screen size (Třebický et al., Reference Třebický, Fialová, Stella, Štěrbová, Kleisner and Havlíček2018). Future studies should either control for the type of electronic device used or conduct rating sessions under controlled laboratory conditions.

On an individual level, preference for sexual dimorphism in men's faces may have been influenced by current fertility status and hormonal regulation (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Hahn and DeBruine2019 and citations therein) or raters’ relationship status (Little et al., Reference Little, Jones, Penton-Voak, Burt and Perrett2002, Reference Little, Cohen, Jones and Belsky2007). Unfortunately, we did not collect these data about participating raters. Concerning other factors on a subpopulation scale, in the Turkish and Cameroonian samples we explored whether some raters’ attributes and some stimuli affected the ratings and significant paths (e.g. the method of skin lightness measurement, combining and separating subsamples, raters’ sex; see Table S4 and Figure S5 in the Supplementary Material). Unfortunately, in the Cameroonian, Czech and Iranian sample, only tens of individual ratings were available: the subsets they yielded are thus unlikely to form a reliable base for a comparison of stability of perception across various social and ethnic groups within the sampled populations.

Although we measured and controlled for several variables (age, BMI, fWHR) and checked for some aspects of raters’ identity, we certainly did not address all possible confounding variables. Other facial attributes, such as skin colouration with respect to redness and yellowness (Carrito et al., Reference Carrito, Santos, Lefevre, Whitehead, Silva and Perrett2016), contrast between facial features and skin (Stephen & McKeeganh, Reference Stephen and McKeeganh2010), or facial hair (Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Sidari, Sains, Alexander, Harrison, Mefodeva and Dixson2020) might likewise affect perception of sex-typicality and attractiveness. Future studies should also account for cross-cultural differences in characteristics that people value in potential mates, local beliefs and customs, and for the prevailing type of spousal choice.

5. Conclusions

Current studies on the association between facial attractiveness and sexual dimorphism frequently use a combination of manipulated stimuli and a forced-choice paradigm that dichotomises participants’ decision-making. It is therefore appropriate to investigate whether conclusions yielded by these setups can be replicated using different methods. Our results, which were based on non-manipulated facial photographs, provide limited support to the hypothesis that raters prefer sex-typical features across populations from distant countries. While perceived femininity of women was preferred in all population samples included in our study and the strength of perceived attractiveness–femininity association was relatively stable, thus suggesting a potential universality of the association, men's perceived facial masculinity was preferred only in the Czech and Colombian sample, that is, in two distant populations. Our raters from urbanised Iranian and Turkish populations did not prefer facial masculinity in men and the same applied to raters from the relatively more rural Cameroonian society. Our study therefore points to a population-specific association of perceived male sex-typicality and attractiveness based on natural facial stimuli of both sexes.

Further, we showed that morphometric variables (sexual shape dimorphism and facial averageness) and measured skin lightness were only moderate and inconsistent predictors of perceived sex-typicality. Presumably owing to this weak predictive power, these variables also did not predict perceived attractiveness across the samples. Measured and perceived sex-typicality tell a different story with respect to human preference. Ideally, different terms should be applied to measured and perceived traits associated with sex-typicality and averageness and researchers ought to bear in mind that some traits (e.g. skin lightness) may be population-specific predictors of perceived attributes, in this case sex-typicality.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Oscar R. Sánchez, Eugenio Valderrama and Andrés Castellanos-Chacón for their help with data collection in Colombia. We also thank Tomáš Kočnar for helping with data collection in Cameroon. We are grateful to Anna Pilátová for proofreading of the manuscript.

Authors’ contributions

KK, VF and PT developed the experimental design of the study. VT, JDL, FP, SAS, RMA and KK collected the facial stimuli and took the facial measurements. VF, JDL, FP, SAS, RMA and KK collected stimuli ratings. PT and KK developed the statistical analyses. VF conducted the statistical analyses and drew diagrams. VF, VT and KK wrote the initial draft, and VF, VT, JDL, PT, FP, SAS, RMA and KK critically revised the manuscript.

Financial support

This study was supported by the Czech Science Foundation (grant number 21-10527S) and by the Charles University, project GA UK no. 1169120. Data collection in Colombia was supported by Universidad El Bosque, Vice-rectory of Research (grant number PCI.2016-8835).

Conflicts of interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Data availability statement

Original data and other supplementary materials to this article are available online at https://osf.io/va8pg/?view_only=021e11321855463f82fc64e6ceb5716a.

Ethical statement

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Adams, D. C., & Otárola-Castillo, E. (2013). Geomorph: An R package for the collection and analysis of geometric morphometric shape data. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4(4), 393399. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2013). Developmental origins of the other-race effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 173178. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412474459CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aoki, K. (2002). Sexual selection as a cause of human skin colour variation: Darwin's hypothesis revisited. Annals of Human Biology, 29(6), 589608. https://doi.org/10.1080/0301446021000019144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apicella, C. L., Little, A. C., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter–gatherers. Perception, 36, 18131820. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atari, M., & Jamali, R. (2016a). Dimensions of women's mate preferences: Validation of a mate preference scale in Iran. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(2), 110. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916651443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atari, M., & Jamali, R. (2016b). Mate preferences in young Iranian women: Cultural and individual difference correlates. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 2(4), 247253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-016-0060-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badaruddoza (2007). A paradox of human mate preferences and natural selection. Journal of Human Ecology, 21(3), 195197. https://doi.org/10.1080/09709274.2007.11905972CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. https://cran.r-project.org/package=forestplotCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, A., Dabbs, J. M., & Dabbs, J. M. Sr (1993). Testosterone and men's marriages. Social Forces, 72(2), 463477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boothroyd, Lynda G., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Partner characteristics associated with masculinity, health and maturity in male faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(5), 11611173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boothroyd, Linda G., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 211218. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borelli, C., & Berneburg, M. (2010). ‘Beauty lies in the eye of the beholder’? Aspects of beauty and attractiveness. JDDG – Journal of the German Society of Dermatology, 8(5), 326330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1610-0387.2009.07318.xGoogle Scholar
Borras-Guevara, M. L., Batres, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2017a). Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 481489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borras-Guevara, M. L., Batres, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2017b). Domestic violence shapes Colombian women's partner choices. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 71(175), 1314. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2405-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronstad, P. M., & Russell, R. (2007). Beauty is in the ‘we’ of the beholder: Greater agreement on facial attractiveness among close relations. Perception, 36(11), 16741681. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5793CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R., Scott, I. M., Maklakov, A. A., Kasumovic, M. M., Clark, A. P., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2011). National income inequality predicts women's preferences for masculinized faces better than health does. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 810812. https://doi.org/10.1038/29772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, D., Nolan, C., Hayward, W. G., Russell, R., & Sulikowski, D. (2013). Is there an own-race preference in attractiveness? Evolutionary Psychology, 11(4), 855872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypothesis tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00023992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carre, J. M., & McCormick, C. M. (2008). In your face: Facial metrics predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in varsity and professional hockey players. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1651), 26512656. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0873CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrito, M. de L., Santos, I. M. B. dos, Lefevre, C. E., Whitehead, R. D., Silva, C. F. da, & Perrett, D. I. (2016). The role of sexually dimorphic skin colour and shape in attractiveness of male faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(2), 125133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrito, M. L., & Semin, G. R. (2019). When we don't know what we know – Sex and skin color. Cognition, 191(June), 103972. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cindoglu, D., Çemrek, M., Toktas, S., & Zencirci, G. (2011). The family in Turkey: The battleground of the modern and the traditional. In Hennon, C. B. & Wilson, S. M. (Eds.), Families in a global context (pp. 235263). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203836941Google Scholar
Clarkson, T. R., Sidari, M. J., Sains, R., Alexander, M., Harrison, M., Mefodeva, V., … Dixson, B. J. W. (2020). A multivariate analysis of women's mating strategies and sexual selection on men's facial morphology. Royal Society Open Science, 7, 191209. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coetzee, V., Faerber, S. J., Greeff, J. M., Lefevre, C. E., Re, D. E., & Perrett, D. I. (2012). African perceptions of female attractiveness. PLoS ONE, 7(10), 38. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coetzee, V., Greeff, J. M., Stephen, I. D., & Perrett, D. I. (2014). Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: The role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS ONE, 9(7). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, M. R., Barbee, A. P., & Pike, C. L. (1990). What do women want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male facial physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 6172. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Barra, M., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Mahmud, Z. H., & Curtis, V. A. (2013). Illness in childhood predicts face preferences in adulthood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 384389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Crawford, J. R., Welling, L. L. M., & Little, A. C. (2010). The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: Cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces. Proceedings. Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 277(1692), 24052410. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Crawford, J. R., & Welling, L. L. M. (2011). Further evidence for regional variation in women's masculinity preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278(1707), 813814. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2200Google Scholar
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Smith, F. G., & Little, A. C. (2010). Are attractive men's faces masculine or feminine? The importance of controlling confounds in face stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(3), 751758. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016457Google ScholarPubMed
Deffenbacher, K. A., Vetter, T., Johanson, J., & O'Toole, A. J. (1998). Facial aging, attractiveness, and distinctiveness. Perception, 27(10), 12331243. https://doi.org/10.1068/p271233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixson, Barnaby J., Dixson, A. F., Bishop, P. J., & Parish, A. (2010). Human physique and sexual attractiveness in men and women: A New Zealand–U.S. comparative study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(3), 798806. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9441-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixson, Barnaby J., Little, A. C., Dixson, H. G., & Brooks, R. C. (2017). Do prevailing environmental factors influence human preferences for facial morphology? Behavioral Ecology, 28(5), 12171227. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durante, K. M., & Li, N. P. (2009). Oestradiol level and opportunistic mating in women. Biology Letters, 5(2), 179182. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foo, Y. Z., Nakagawa, S., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2017). The effects of sex hormones on immune function: A meta-analysis. Biological Reviews, 92(1), 551571. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foo, Y. Z., Simmons, L. W., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Predictors of facial attractiveness and health in humans. Scientific Reports, 7(November 2016), 39731. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep39731CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedland, R., Afary, J., Gardinali, P., & Naslund, C. (2016). Love in the Middle East: The contradictions of romance in the Facebook World. Critical Research on Religion, 4(3), 229258. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050303216676523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annual Review of Anthropology, 34, 523548. https://doi.org/10.1146/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLoS ONE, 10(7), 118. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Germine, L., Russell, R., Bronstad, P. M., Blokland, G. A. M., Smoller, J. W., Kwok, H., … Wilmer, J. B. (2015). Individual aesthetic preferences for faces are shaped mostly by environments, not genes. Current Biology, 25(20), 26842689. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, M., & Lumley, T. (2020). forestplot: Advanced Forest Plot Using ‘grid’ Graphics. https://rdrr.io/cran/forestplot/Google Scholar
Gu, Z., Eils, R., & Schlesner, M. (2016). Complex heatmaps reveal patterns and correlations in multidimensional genomic data. Bioinformatics, 32(18), 28472849. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btw313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gu, Z., Gu, L., Eils, R., Schlesner, M., & Brors, B. (2014). Circlize implements and enhances circular visualization in R. Bioinformatics, 30(19), 28112812. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btu393CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, K. (2007). Love by arrangement: The ambiguity of ‘spousal choice’ in a Turkish village. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 13(2), 345362. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9655.2007.00438.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausman, B. L. (1999). Ovaries to estrogen: Sex hormones and chemical femininity in the 20th century. Journal of Medical Humanities, 20(3), 165176. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022926928578CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, A. J., Holzleitner, I. J., Talamas, S. N., & Perrett, D. I. (2016). Perception of health from facial cues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1693). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzleitner, I. J., Lee, A. J., Hahn, A. C., Kandrik, M., Bovet, J., Renoult, J. P., … Jones, B. C. (2019). Comparing theory-driven and data-driven attractiveness models using images of real women's faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 45(12), 15891595. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honarvar, B., Salehi, F., Barfi, R., Asadi, Z., Honarvar, H., Odoomi, N., … Lankarani, K. B. (2016). Attitudes toward and experience of singles with premarital sex: A population-based study in Shiraz, southern Iran. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(2), 395402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0577-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hönekopp, J. (2006). Once more: Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Relative contributions of private and shared taste to judgments of facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 199209. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.199Google ScholarPubMed
Hunter, R. S. (1958). Photoelectric color difference meter. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 48(12), 985. https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.48.000985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 251267. https://doi.org/10.1145/3011286.3011307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, B. C., Hahn, A. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2019). Ovulation, sex hormones, and women's mating psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(1), 5162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, D., & Hill, K. (1993). Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations. Human Nature, 4(3), 271296. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, A. L., & Jaeger, B. (2019). Biological bases of beauty revisited: The effect of symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism on female facial attractiveness. Symmetry, 11(2), 279. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym11020279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, L., & Ashbaugh, A. R. (2017). Simple and parallel mediation: A tutorial exploring anxiety sensitivity, sensation seeking, and gender. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 13(3), 148165. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.13.3.p148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleisner, K., Kočnar, T., Tureček, P., Stella, D., Akoko, R. M., Třebický, V., & Havlíček, J. (2017). African and European perception of African female attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(6), 744755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleisner, K., Pokorný, Š., & Saribay, S. A. (2019). Toward a new approach to cross-cultural distinctiveness and typicality of human faces: The cross-group typicality/distinctiveness metric. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koehler, N., Simmons, L. W., Rhodes, G., & Peters, M. (2004). The relationship between sexual dimorphism in human faces and fluctuating asymmetry. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(suppl. 4), S233S236. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2009). Averageness or symmetry: Which is more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychologica, 131(2), 136142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2011). Multiple mechanisms in the perception of face gender: Effect of sex-irrelevant features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 626633. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020369Google ScholarPubMed
Kościński, K. (2007). Facial attractiveness: General patterns of facial preferences. Anthropological Review, 70(1), 4579. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10044-008-0001-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kościński, K. (2008). Facial attractiveness: Variation, adaptiveness and consequences of facial preferences. Anthropological Review, 71(1), 77105. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10044-008-0012-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kowner, R., & Ogawa, T. (1995). The role of rater's sex, personality, and appearance in judgments of facial beauty. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 339349. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.81.1.339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R. S. S. (2015). Facial width-to-height ratio in a large sample of commonwealth games athletes. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(1), 197209. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491501300112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R. S. S., Mileva, M., & Ritchie, K. L. (2018). Inter-rater agreement in trait judgements from faces. PLoS ONE, 13(8), e0202655. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0202655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langlois, J., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 123(3), 390423. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.126.3.390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Law Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. R., … Hillier, S. G. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proceedings. Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 273(1583), 135140. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, A. J., & Zietsch, B. P. (2011). Experimental evidence that women's mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity. Biology Letters, 7(6), 892895. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.0454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (1996). Comparison of salivary steroid profiles in naturally occurring conception and non-conception cycles. Human Reproduction, 11(10), 20902096. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.humrep.a019055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, A. C., Cohen, D. L., Jones, B. C., & Belsky, J. (2007). Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61(6), 967973. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0325-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, A. C, DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2013). Environment contingent preferences: Exposure to visual cues of direct male–male competition and wealth increase women's preferences for masculinity in male faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 193200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, A. C., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2002). The role of masculinity and distinctiveness in judgments of human male facial attractiveness. British Journal of Psychology, 93(4), 451464. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712602761381349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 16381659. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269(1496), 10951100. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.1984CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestripieri, D., Klimczuk, A. C. E., Traficonte, D. M., & Wilson, M. C. (2014). A greater decline in female facial attractiveness during middle age reflects women's loss of reproductive value. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcinkowska, U. M., Kozlov, M. V., Cai, H., Contreras-Garduño, J., Dixson, B. J., Oana, G. A., … Rantala, M. J. (2014). Cross-cultural variation in men's preference for sexual dimorphism in women's faces. Biology Letters, 10(4), 47. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcinkowska, U. M., Rantala, M. J., Lee, A. J., Kozlov, M. V, Aavik, T., Cai, H., … Dixson, B. J. W. (2019). Women's preferences for men's facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions. Scientific Reports, 9(3387), 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-39350-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marečková, K., Weinbrand, Z., Chakravarty, M. M., Lawrence, C., Aleong, R., Leonard, G., … Paus, T. (2011). Testosterone-mediated sex differences in the face shape during adolescence: Subjective impressions and objective features. Hormones and Behavior, 60(5), 681690. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazur, A., & Michalek, J. (1998). Marriage, divorce, and male testosterone. Social Forces, 77(1), 315330. https://doi.org/10.2307/3006019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLellan, B., & McKelvie, S. J. (1993). Effects of age and gender on perceived facial attractiveness. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 25(1), 135142. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0078790CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitteroecker, P., Windhager, S., Møller, G. B., & Schaefer, K. (2015). The morphometrics of ‘masculinity’ in human faces. PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0118374. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118374CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogilski, J. K., & Welling, L. L. M. (2017). The relative importance of sexual dimorphism, fluctuating asymmetry, and color cues to health during evaluation of potential partners’ facial photographs: A conjoint analysis study. Human Nature, 28(1), 5375. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9277-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooradian, A. D., Morley, J. E., & Korenman, S. G. (1987). Biological actions of androgens. Endocrine Reviews, 8 (1)(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, F. R., Cornwell, R. E., Law Smith, M. J., Al Dujaili, E. A. S., Sharp, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2011). Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in human male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1706), 774780. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1678CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Iglesias-Julios, M., Pita, M., & Turiegano, E. (2015). Facial features: What women perceive as attractive and what men consider attractive. PLoS ONE, 10(7), 117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132979CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, K., & Watanabe, K. (2019). Data-driven mathematical model of East-Asian facial attractiveness: The relative contributions of shape and reflectance to attractiveness judgements. Royal Society Open Science, 6(5). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.182189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, J., Pawłowski, B., Borkowska, B., Augustyniak, D., & Drulis-Kawa, Z. (2018). No evidence for the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in male humans. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25694-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özener, B. (2012). Facial width-to-height ratio in a Turkish population is not sexually dimorphic and is unrelated to aggressive behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(3), 169173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penna, V., Fricke, A., Iblher, N., Eisenhardt, S. U., & Stark, G. B. (2015). The attractive lip: A photomorphometric analysis. Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, 68(7), 920929. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjps.2015.03.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(4), 229241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penton-Voak, I. S., Jacobson, A., & Trivers, R. (2004). Populational differences in attractiveness judgements of male and female faces: Comparing British and Jamaican samples. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(6), 355370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I. S., Rowland, D. A., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., … Akamatsu, S. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394(6696), 884887. https://doi.org/10.1038/29772CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrett, D. I., May, K. A., & Yoshikawa, S. (1994). Facial shape and judgements of female attractiveness. Nature, 368(6468), 239242. https://doi.org/10.1038/368239a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, M., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2008). Does attractiveness in men provide clues to semen quality? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21(2), 572579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01477.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polo, P., Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Pita, M., Shackelford, T. K., & Fink, B. (2019). Testosterone-dependent facial and body traits predict men's sociosexual attitudes and behaviors. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(3), 110. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Probst, F., Bobst, C., & Lobmaier, J. S. (2016). Testosterone-to-estradiol ratio is associated with female facial attractiveness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(1), 8999. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1024696CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rantala, M. J., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kivleniece, I., Kecko, S., & Krams, I. (2012). Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in humans. Nature Communications, 3(394), 15. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1696CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation forStatistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rennels, J. L., Bronstad, P. M., & Langlois, J. H. (2008). Are attractive men's faces masculine or feminine? The importance of type of facial stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 884893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.884CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W. (2018). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Northwestern University. https://cran.r-project.org/package=psychVersion=1.8.12.Google Scholar
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57(1), 199226. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proceedings. Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 270(suppl.), S93S95. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, G., Hickford, C., & Jeffery, L. (2000). Sex-typicality and attractiveness: Are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive? British Journal of Psychology, 91(1), 125140. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712600161718CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Kieran, L., McKay, R., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Attractiveness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-western cultures: In search of biologically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30(5), 611625. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohlf, F. J. (2015). The tps series of software. Hystrix, 26(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.4404/hystrix-26.1-11264Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. R package version 0.5-15. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. https://econpapers.repec.org/article/jssjstsof/v_3a048_3ai02.htm%0Ahttp://www.jstatsoft.org/v48/i02/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryder, H., Maltby, J., Rai, L., Jones, P., & Flowe, H. D. (2016). Women's fear of crime and preference for formidable mates: How specific are the underlying psychological mechanisms? Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 293302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saribay, S. A., Biten, A. F., Meral, E. O., Aldan, P., Trebicky, V., & Kleisner, K. (2018). The Bogazici face database: Standardized photographs of Turkish faces with supporting materials. PLoS ONE, 13(2). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0192018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheel, A. M., Tiokhin, L., Isager, P. M., & Lakens, D. (2020). Why hypothesis testers should spend less time testing hypotheses. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966795Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, C. A., Rasband, W. S., & Eliceiri, K. W. (2012). NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nature Methods, 9(7), 671675. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiber, J. E., Singh, N. K., & Klatsky, S. A. (2005). Beauty lies in the ‘eyebrow’ of the beholder: A public survey of eyebrow aesthetics. Aesthetic Surgery Journal, 25(4), 348352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asj.2005.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Josephson, S. C., Boyette, A. H., Cuthill, I. C., Fried, R. L., … Penton-Voak, I. S. (2014). Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(40), 1438814393. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1409643111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, I. M. L., Pound, N., Stephen, I. D., Clark, A. P., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2010). Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans. PLoS ONE, 5(10), e13585. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013585CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420428. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18839484CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kecko, S., Moore, F. R., Kaasik, A., Meija, L., … Krams, I. (2014). Body height, immunity, facial and vocal attractiveness in young men. Naturwissenschaften, 101(12), 10171025. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-014-1241-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, F. G., Jones, B. C., Debruine, L. M., & Little, A. C. (2009). Interactions between masculinity–femininity and apparent health in face preferences. Behavioral Ecology, 20(2), 441445. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorokowski, P., Kościński, K., & Sorokowska, A. (2013). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder but ugliness culturally universal? Facial preferences of Polish and Yali (papua) people. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(4), 907925. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., & Kras, D. (2013). Face color and sexual attractiveness: Preferences of Yali people of Papua. Cross-Cultural Research, 47(4), 415427. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397113485540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephen, I. D., & McKeeganh, A. M. (2010). Lip colour affects perceived sex typicality and attractiveness of human faces. Perception, 39(8), 11041110. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephen, I. D., & Perrett, D. I. (2016). Color and face perception. In Elliot, A. J., Fairchild, M. D., & Franklin, A. (Eds.), Handbook of Color Psychology (pp. 585602). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781107337930.029Google Scholar
Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. M. L., Coetzee, V., Pound, N., Perrett, D. I., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men's faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(4), 260267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stirrat, M. R., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 21(3), 349354. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610362647CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strom, M. A., Zebrowitz, L. A., Zhang, S., Bronstad, P. M., & Lee, H. K. (2012). Skin and bones: The contribution of skin tone and facial structure to racial prototypicality ratings. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e41193. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strzałko, J., & Kaszycka, K. A. (1992). Physical attractiveness: Interpersonal and intrapersonal variability of assessments. Biodemography and Social Biology, 39(1–2), 170176. https://doi.org/10.1080/19485565.1992.9988813CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaddle, J. P., & Reierson, G. W. (2002). Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269(1507), 22852289. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Congnitive Sciences, 3(12), 452460. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01403-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(2), 131144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Grammer, K. (1999). The body and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(2), 105120. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00044-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Třebický, V., Fialová, J., Kleisner, K., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., & Havlíček, J. (2015). Further evidence for links between facial width-to-height ratio and fighting success: Commentary on Zilioli et al. (2014). Aggressive Behavior, 41(4), 331334. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Třebický, V., Fialová, J., Stella, D., Štěrbová, Z., Kleisner, K., & Havlíček, J. (2018). 360 degrees of facial perception: Congruence in perception of frontal portrait, profile, and rotation photographs. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2405), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Třebický, V., Stirrat, M., & Havlíček, J. (2019). Fighting assessment. Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science, 111. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2738-1Google Scholar
van Bokhoven, I., van Goozen, S. H. M., van Engeland, H., Schaal, B., Arseneault, L., Séguin, J. R., … Tremblay, R. E. (2006). Salivary testosterone and aggression, delinquency, and social dominance in a population-based longitudinal study of adolescent males. Hormones and Behavior, 50(1), 118125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Berghe, P. L., & Frost, P. (1986). Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: A case of gene culture co-evolution? Ethnic and Racial Studies, 9(1), 87113. https://doi.org/10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitzthum, V. J. (2009). The ecology and evolutionary endocrinology of reproduction in the human female. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 140(S49), 95136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.v140.49sCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitzthum, V. J., Bentley, G. R., Spielvogel, H., Caceres, E., Thornburg, J., Jones, L., … Chatterton, R. T. (2002). Salivary progesterone levels and rate of ovulation are significantly lower in poorer than in better-off urban-dwelling Bolivian women. Human Reproduction, 17(7), 19061913. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/humrep/17.7.1906CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagatsuma, H. (1967). The social perception of skin color in Japan. Daedalus, 96(2), 407443. https://www.jstor.org/stable/20027045Google Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Gilani, S. Z., Shafait, F., Mian, A., Tan, D. W., Maybery, M. T., … Eastwood, P. (2015). Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1816). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1351Google ScholarPubMed
Worthman, C. M. (1995). Hormones sex gender. Annual Review of Anthropology, 24, 593617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., Wang, R., Bronstad, P. M., Eisenberg, D. T. A., Undurraga, E. A., Reyes-García, V., & Godoy, R. A. (2012). First impressions from faces among U.S. and culturally isolated Tsimane’ people in the Bolivian rainforest. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 119134. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111411386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the stimuli sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the rater sample

Figure 2

Figure 1. Forest plots displaying the relative strength of Pearson's correlations between perceived attractiveness and perceived sex-typicality (a), perceived attractiveness and sexual shape dimorphism (b) and between sexual shape dimorphism and perceived sex-typicality (c), with confidence intervals of each coefficient. Each row corresponds to a single sample (women from all five samples, men from all five samples, with sampled countries in alphabetical order). Blue circles represent Pearson's correlation coefficients (mean estimate on the given sample), while black lines stand for error bars defined as 95% confidence intervals around each mean correlation. A vertical line in zero (‘0’) enables us to inspect whether the confidence interval for a given correlation contains zero. The columns on the right side of the diagrams show coefficients of the associations. This figure facilitates a comparison of bivariate associations among the population samples.

Figure 3

Figure 2. A visualisation of path analyses (multiple regression models) between the rated facial attributes (perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness) and facial measures ordered by the sex of the stimuli. Arrows represent the direction of the association. Non-significant paths are omitted. Association between perceived sex-typicality and attractiveness was treated as a correlation (i.e. the direction was not specified). Numbers next to the paths indicate the estimate of regression or correlation coefficient in a corresponding model with standardised variables. Red colour denotes a negative coefficient. The graph shows to what extent is the observed within-sample variability of each variable explained by other variable(s). In every sample, perceived femininity and attractiveness are closely mutually associated in the women's samples. In most population samples, perceived masculinity was not associated with perceived men's attractiveness. The significant paths mostly replicate significant Pearson's correlations (see Figure S4) (+ p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001).

Figure 4

Table 3. Outline of predictions (a) and significant results (b)