People worry. Central features of human lives such as relationships, finances and careers all involve a degree of uncertainty. Many sorts of real and potential problems can arise; things can and do go wrong. One of the defining traits of humanity is the ability to consider the future and imagine various possible outcomes. An upshot of this special ability, however, is that at times we focus on the negative. Worry is a focus on undesirable outcomes that becomes extended over time. For most, a degree of worry is normal and largely adaptive (Esbjorn et al., Reference Esbjørn, Lønfeldt, Nielsen, Reinholdt-Dunne, Sømhovd and Cartwright-Hatton2015). It is hard to imagine anyone experiencing a full range of emotions without sometimes feeling worried or anxious about the future. Most can use worry as a means of preparing for, or avoiding, unwanted outcomes (Borkovec, Ray, & Stöber, Reference Borkovec, Ray and Stober1998; Stöber & Joormann, Reference Stöber and Joormann2001a). Some, however, worry to a degree that becomes overwhelming and debilitating, and unchecked, this can have serious consequences. Excessive worry is, in fact, a core symptom of generalized anxiety disorder and relates to interpersonal problems, academic difficulties, self-harm and thoughts of suicide (Dugas, Schwartz, & Francis, Reference Dugas, Schwartz and Francis2004; Tan, Bonn, & Tam, Reference Tan, Bonn and Tam2018). Excessive worry perpetuates and exacerbates emotional distress rather than facilitating planning or coping (Kertz, Bigda-Peyton, Rosmarin, & Björgvinsson, Reference Kertz, Bigda-Peyton, Rosmarin and Björgvinsson2012). Excessive worry can take on a life of its own. Those suffering from generalized anxiety disorder, for example, can experience worry itself as a topic of worry (e.g., “I have too many worries; I can’t stop worrying”; Wells, Reference Wells1995). Again, such maladaptive levels of worry have been linked to a variety of psychopathologies (Barlow, Reference Barlow1988; National Institute of Mental Health, 2016; Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski, & Castonguay, Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013).
A large body of research findings suggest that social support may be a key factor in alleviating or protecting against excessive worry, as well as promoting general psychological well-being (e.g., Cohen, Reference Cohen2004; Seeman, Lusignolo, Albert, & Berkman, Reference Seeman, Lusignolo, Albert and Berkman2001). Social support is thought to dampen or moderate the negative effects of stress on psychological well-being (e.g. Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Liu, Schootman and Jeffe2013), as well as promote proactive coping and improved quality of life in general (Kawachi & Berkman, Reference Kawachi and Berkman2001; Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock, & Coyle, Reference Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock and Coyle2016). Social support can be thought of as providing a sense of security in the face of uncertainty or, in attachment terms, a secure base to which an individual is able to retreat when experiencing stress or anxiety (Feeney & Collins, Reference Feeney and Collins2015). The role of social support as a secure base is thought to be important because it provides a safe space for the individual to calm negative emotions and refocus their attention away from abstract fears and towards concrete challenges that can be addressed. However, even knowing the important benefits of social support, it is not always clear how it is best provided. Thus, the primary purpose of this study was to look in some depth at the experience of social support in relation to worry among a group of Malaysian adults. This was hoped to provide insight into how, and in what forms, social support can best help to alleviate worry.
Normal and pathological worry
Again, although worrying is normal for humans to a degree, when it persists over a long period of time and does not aid with coping, it becomes problematic, even pathological (Borkovec et al., Reference Borkovec, Ray and Stober1998; Esbjorn et al., Reference Esbjørn, Lønfeldt, Nielsen, Reinholdt-Dunne, Sømhovd and Cartwright-Hatton2015). Excessive worry becomes a source of stress and a hindrance to coping in and of itself. The study of worry thus encompasses two types of worry: normal, or adaptive forms of worry; and excessive, or potentially pathological, worry. Pathological worry, commonly measured using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990), refers to persistent and excessive worry that can interfere with functioning. Normal worry, commonly measured using the Worry Domains Questionnaire (WDQ; Stöber & Joormann, Reference Stöber and Joormann2001a), refers to worries experienced, at times, by most individuals in various day-to-day life domains, such as relationships and work. To ensure that the construct of worry is examined in a comprehensive manner, Stöber and Joormann (Reference Stöber and Joormann2001b) recommend measuring both types.
Worry and demographics
Although to some degree worrying is a universal human phenomenon, previous research has looked at differences in worry proneness across various groups. In general, these results are far from conclusive, but one consistent finding relates to age: Worry tends to be more prevalent in younger adults compared to older adults (Basevitz, Pushkar, Chaikelson, Conway, & Dalton, Reference Basevitz, Pushkar, Chaikelson, Conway and Dalton2008; Golden et al., Reference Golden, Conroy, Bruce, Denihan, Greene, Kirby and Lawlor2011; Gonçalves & Byrne, Reference Gonçalves and Byrne2013; Lindesay et al., Reference Lindesay, Baillon, Brugha, Dennis, Stewart, Araya and Meltzer2006). Mature adults, it is suggested, have more experience in coping with various problems (Valliant, Reference Valliant1977) and are more likely to feel established or secure in their situations, leaving them less likely to feel threatened about the future (Armstrong, Wuthrich, Knight, & Joiner, Reference Armstrong, Wuthrich, Knight and Joiner2014; Borkovec, Reference Borkovec1988). This is not true for everyone though. Those who tend towards chronic or pathological worry tend to report more intense worries as they get older (e.g. Wisocki, Hunt, & Souza, Reference Wisocki, Hunt and Souza1998).
Other demographic factors generally show inconsistent results. With regard to gender, for example, although many intuitively see women as more prone to worry, research does not support the idea of consistent gender differences. Some studies have found a greater tendency towards worry among women compared to men (e.g. Golden et al., Reference Golden, Conroy, Bruce, Denihan, Greene, Kirby and Lawlor2011; Robichaud, Dugas, & Conway, Reference Robichaud, Dugas and Conway2003) Others, however, have found no gender differences (e.g., Brown, Antony, & Barlow, Reference Brown, Antony and Barlow1992; Tallis, Davey, & Bond, Reference Tallis, Davey, Bond, Davey and Tallis1994). Similarly, with regard to ethnicity, although results are somewhat mixed, the majority of studies show no clear differences in pathological worry between ethnic groups (Gillis, Haaga, & Ford, Reference Gillis, Haaga and Ford1995; Scott, Eng, & Heimberg, Reference Scott, Eng and Heimberg2002).
Specifically looking at Malaysia, which is the focus of this research, there are three major ethnic groups that are clearly defined by law (Malays, Malaysian-Chinese, and Malaysian-Indian). To our knowledge, previous studies have not specifically looked at ethnic differences for worry. However, large-scale public health studies (Institute for Public Health, 2011) have suggested that generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is more prevalent among ethnic Indians as compared to Malays and ethnic Chinese. Also, the same data appear to show greater tendencies towards GAD among Malaysian females and young adults. For this reason, this study also took a preliminary look at the relationships between age, gender, ethnicity and worry among Malaysians. Identifying whether differences in pathological worry exist between Malaysian ethnic groups, it was thought, could provide clues regarding this observed prevalence of GAD and thus assist in focusing intervention efforts.
Anxiety and worry
As mentioned, there appears to be a qualitative difference between moderate or normal worry and more severe forms of worry that are associated with GAD. Individuals with GAD tend to exhibit a disproportionate fear of negative events, along with low self-efficacy. Compared to others, they foresee worse consequences when things go wrong and feel less capable of solving problems (Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston, & Dugas, Reference Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston and Dugas1998; Wong et al., Reference Wong, Sultan Shah, Teng, Lin, Majeed and Chan2016). GAD patients also tend to be hypersensitive to changes in their emotional states, particularly fearing shifts in mood from positive to negative. Because of this, positive emotions can, paradoxically, become a source of anxiety: The anxious person knows that good feelings are certain to end at some point and they fear the transition back to negativity (Llera & Newman, Reference Llera and Newman2011). Chronic worry, for the anxious person, thus becomes a form of self-protection. They avoid the pain of negative emotional shifts by convincing themselves that positive feelings are not real, thereby minimizing shock and pain of what they see as inevitable adversity and loss (Newman et al., Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013). Relatedly, GAD and pathological worry are linked to traits such as indecisiveness, intolerance of uncertainty, and perfectionism (Koerner, Mejia, & Kusec, Reference Koerner, Mejia and Kusec2017). The highly anxious person is much more afraid of failure or negative events than most; they see negative consequences as being much more severe. Thus, unconsciously, they use worry as a buffer against making decisions or commitments. In the process, by obsessing over seemingly minor issues, they achieve a level of predictability. They avoid the pain of disappointment and failure in the real world by continually playing out negative scenarios in their mind (Rassin, Reference Rassin2007; Stöber & Joormann, Reference Stöber and Joormann2001b).
Perceived Social Support, Worry, and Anxiety
Social support can assume a variety of forms. Thus, it has been operationalized and studied in several ways. Past research has conceived of social support variously as social embeddedness or connectedness, enacted support or actual support received, and perceived social support (see Barrera, Reference Barrera1986, for a review). Of these three operational constructs, perceived social support (PSS), or the belief that adequate support is available when needed, has been found to have the greatest impact on mental health (Barrera, Reference Barrera1986; Calvete & Connor-Smith, Reference Calvete and Connor-Smith2006; Lakey & Orehek, Reference Lakey and Orehek2011). PSS has been shown to relate to the individual’s specific appraisal of the support available to them (Antonucci & Israel, Reference Antonucci and Israel1986) as well as to feelings of relationship satisfaction (Sarason, Sarason, Shearin, & Pierce, Reference Sarason, Sarason, Shearin and Pierce1987). Thus, aside from being relatively easy to measure through self-report, the construct of PSS includes elements of both embeddedness, and enacted support.
Studies consistently indicate that PSS relates to better mental health outcomes. Those reporting higher levels of PSS, for example, have lower rates of clinical depression (Lakey & Cronin, Reference Lakey, Cronin, Dobson and Dozois2008) and fewer symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Brewin, Andrews, & Valentine, Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000). PSS also relates to lower anxiety levels (Holt & Espelage, Reference Holt and Espelage2005) and stress-inducing situations and events being perceived as less important (Duman & Kocak, 2013). In neurological terms, PSS appears to lessen the expression of anxiety in the amygdala, thus acting as a protective factor against the development of psychopathologies such as PTSD and severe depression (Hyde, Gorka, Manuck, & Hariri, Reference Hyde, Gorka, Manuck and Hariri2011).
Specifically, with regard to worry, studies have indicated that PSS benefits those with more situation-specific worries. Caregivers of disabled children (Ma & Mak, Reference Ma and Mak2016) and breast cancer survivors (Waters, Liu, Schootman, & Jeffe, Reference Waters, Liu, Schootman and Jeffe2013), for example, reported palliative effects on mental health when receiving adequate social support. Studies of the relationship between social support and more generalized or pathological worry, however, are relatively rare. Also, importantly, it is not known exactly how social support helps to alleviate worry or what the most effective types of social support are. Many studies, both quantitative and qualitative (e.g. Dam, Boot, Van Boxtel, Verhey, & De Vugt, Reference Dam, Boots, Van Boxtel, Verhey and De Vugt2018; O’Connor, Longman, White, & Obst, Reference O’Connor, Longman, White and Obst2015), have focused more on the perceived presence of social support than on the forms it takes and how it is beneficial. Qualitative studies on the nature of social support have found somewhat mixed results: Some types of social support seem to be experienced as particularly beneficial while others are not. Previous results have cited the perceived value of emotional support and expert knowledge (Dwarswaard, Bakker, Staa, & Boeije, Reference Dwarswaard, Bakker, Staa and Boeije2016). However, support that is seen as too directive or that limits individual autonomy is sometimes perceived negatively (e.g. Feeney & Collins, Reference Feeney and Collins2015; Potvin, Brown, & Cobigo, Reference Potvin, Brown and Cobigo2016). Thus, a better understanding of the qualitative nature of social support, what works and what does not may be an important key to understanding its relationship to worry and other aspects of mental health.
Returning to the context of this study in Malaysia, we can see that despite an increasing prevalence of anxiety disorders over the past 20 years, as well as an established link between worry and anxiety, there has been little research on worry (Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, MuhdYusoff, Ratnasingam, Mohamed, Nasir, MohdSallehuddin and Aris2015). Similarly, looking specifically at the Malaysian setting, there is no known research on the nature and effectiveness of available social support. Qualitative investigations of social support in other settings have indicated that social support can improve feelings of worth or self-esteem (Lakey & Cohen, Reference Lakey, Cohen, Cohen, Underwood and Gottlieb2000; Thoits, Reference Thoits1982) as well as lessen anxiety and support proactive coping (Casale, Wild, & Kuo, Reference Casale, Wild and Kuo2013). There is, however, no known qualitative research examining the subjective aspects of social support in Malaysia.
Research aims and significance
The aims of this study were thus to provide a preliminary look at the prevalence of worry and social support in Malaysia (i.e. Which major groups are most affected by worry?). And, more importantly, to take an in-depth qualitative look at the roles that social support plays in managing worry for Malaysian adults (i.e. How do they experience social support and what do they perceive to be most helpful or beneficial?).
Due to the small sample size, quantitative measures were intended merely to provide some general guidance for future studies. The more important contributions of this study were expected to lie in a qualitative analysis of how social support is experienced. Looking at how Malaysians experience social support in general and the more specific role which it plays in alleviating worry could, it was hoped, contribute to our overall knowledge of social support’s importance in mental health and well-being in general.
Research questions
The primary research questions are listed below.
Research Question 1: How does the prevalence of normal, pathological worry, and perceived social support vary across demographic groups in Malaysia?
Research Question 2: Do higher levels of perceived social support relate to lower levels of normal and pathological worry?
Research Question 3: What forms of social support do Malaysians benefit from?
Research Question 4: How does social support assist Malaysians in managing worry? (i.e. What roles does social support fill, and how is it beneficial?)
Method
Participants
This study used a convenience sample of participants (N = 136) recruited through social media advertisements and subsequent snowball sampling (i.e. peer referrals). Any Malaysian citizen over the age of 18 years was eligible to participate. The final sample included 96 females (71%) and 40 males (29%), ranging from 21 to 59 years old (M = 33.99, SD = 7.65). The majority of the participants were of Malay ethnicity (61%), followed by Malaysian Chinese (26%), Malaysian Indians (8%) and others (5%). In terms of marital status, 53 participants were single (39%), 77 were married (57%) and 6 were divorced or separated (4%). As for employment status, 54% were employed full-time, 6% were employed part-time, 12% were self-employed, 12% were unemployed and 17% were not in the workforce (either retired or studying full-time). In terms of education, most of the participants either had a bachelor’s degree (65%) or a postgraduate degree (31%). Only 4% had just a high school diploma or equivalent.
Ethics
This research project was approved by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (Project Number 10409).
Design
This research project used a mixed-method, non-experimental approach. Both quantitative (for the first two research questions) and qualitative data (for the third research question) were collected through an online survey. The quantitative portion of the study was correlational in nature. The dependent variables were pathological worry and normal worry. The independent variables were perceived social support, age, gender, ethnicity and marital status. The qualitative portion consisted of a thematic analysis of open-ended responses regarding the roles and value of social support in managing worry.
Measures
Demographics
The demographics section asked for information about age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, educational status and employment status.
Pathological worry
The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990) is a 16-item, self-rated scale that is used to measure the severity of pathological worry. It has robust psychometric properties with high internal consistency (α = .93), well-established test–retest reliability (r = .87) and good validity (Molina & Borkovec, Reference Molina, Borkovec, Davey and Tallis1994; Stöber, Reference Stöber1998). Respondents rate the degree to which items describe their worry-related experiences on a 5-point scale, from 1 (not at all typical of me) to 5 (very typical of me). Higher scores indicate more severe levels of pathological worry. The possible scoring range for the PSWQ is from 16 to 80. Scores from 16 to 39 represent low levels of worry, 40 to 59 represent moderate levels of worry, and scores above 60 are considered to be high, potentially indicative of an anxiety disorder (Salzer, Stiller, Tacke-Pook, Jacobi, & Leibing, Reference Salzer, Stiller, Tacke-Pook, Jacobi and Leibing2009).
Normal worry
The Worry Domains Questionnaire — Short Form (WDQ-SF; Stöber & Joormann, Reference Stöber and Joormann2001a) is an abbreviated version of the 25-item WDQ (Tallis, Eysenck, & Mathews, Reference Tallis, Eysenck and Mathews1992). It measures the amount of normal worry in five domains of day-to-day concern, namely aimless future, work, relationship, financial issues, and lack of confidence. WDQ-SF displayed a near-perfect correlation with the original WDQ (r = .97) and a high internal consistency (α = .88). Each item describes the worry content of a person, and clients are asked to rate how much they experience that type of worry on a scale of 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). A total score is obtained by summing the ratings of each item. Higher scores indicate greater amounts of normal worry. As the WDQ-SF is designed to measure domains of normal worry, it does not have a standard clinical cut-off point.
Perceived social support
The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet, Stiller Tacke-Pook, Jacobi, & Leibing, 1988) contains 12 items and is designed to assess the degree to which social support is perceived to be available from significant others, family and friends. The MSPSS has been shown to be valid and reliable across cultures and age groups, with good internal consistency (α = .88), test–retest reliability (α = .85) and factorial validity (Stanley, Beck, & Zebb, Reference Stanley, Beck and Zebb1998; Wongpakaran, Wongpakaran, & Ruktrakul, Reference Wongpakaran, Wongpakaran and Ruktrakul2011). The items are rated on a scale of 1 (very strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree). A total score is calculated by adding the ratings for each item. Total scores from 12 to 35 are considered low social support. Scores of 36 to 60 indicate moderate levels of support, and scores above 60 are considered high levels of support.
Open-ended essay question
Participants were asked to describe in their own words, and in as much detail as possible, the ways in which support from family, friends and others has played a role in helping them manage worry in their lives.
Procedure
A mass email advertisement was sent first to the researchers’ social media contacts and subsequently disseminated to other eligible participants via snowball sampling. All eligible parties were informed of the criteria for participation and the general purpose of the study. Interested individuals followed a link to the online questionnaire and were subsequently presented with an explanatory statement/informed consent agreement. This statement provided further information about the study and clearly explained that participation was voluntary and that participants were free to withdraw at any time. Participants were provided with contact information for a 24-hour counselling hotline, as well as the primary investigator, in case they felt uneasy or required counselling during or following the survey. Participation was completely voluntary. No participants received compensation of any sort. Measures were presented to participants in the same order as listed in the measures section above.
Results
Following a 3-week collection period, questionnaire data were downloaded into spreadsheet format and prepared for analysis. Quantitative data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 22. Qualitative coding and textual organization processes were facilitated through the use of QSR NVivo 12 Plus. Results are outlined below.
Quantitative Assumptions
There were no missing values or outliers identified in the data (e.g. Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013). For the parametric tests, data on other ethnic groups and the divorced were excluded due to small sample sizes. In addition, data on employment status and educational level were excluded as these variables are not the focus of this research. An assumptions check revealed that the data for participants’ age, normal worry levels and perceived social support levels were not normally distributed. Similarly, the normal worry scores for females and those in the age group 40–59 were also not normally distributed. However, according to the central limit theorem, since the sample size was large (N > 30), the violation of normality was not an issue (Field, Reference Field2013). Homogeneity of variance in worry levels were equal across demographic variables.
Worry and demographics
Means and standard deviations for pathological worry (PSWQ), normal worry (WDQ-SF) and perceived social support (MSPSS) for all demographic variables are presented in Table 1. Internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was good for all three measures (PSWQ: α = .84; WDQ-SF: α = .89; MSPSS: α = .86).
Note: PSS = perceived social support.
Age was found to have a significant negative correlation to normal worry r(134) = -.295, p < .01, but not pathological worry, r(134) = -.127, p > .05. Older participants reported significantly less normal worry, but pathological worry was not related to age.
Other demographic variables revealed no significant relationships to worry.
For gender, no significant differences between women and men were found for pathological worry, t(134) = 1.60, p > .05, or normal worry, t(134) = -.223, p > .05.
For ethnicity, no significant differences between ethnic groups were found for pathological worry, F(2, 126) = .017, p > .05, or normal worry, F(2, 126) = .413, p > .05.
For marital status, no significant differences were found between single and married participants for pathological worry, t(128) = -.124, p > .05, or for normal worry, t(128) = .460, p > .05.
For social support and worry, PSS was negatively correlated to both normal worry, r(134) = -.209, p < .01, and pathological worry, r(134) = -.277, p < .01. Both measures of worry decreased with higher levels of social support.
Open-ended responses
Participants’ open-ended descriptions of their experiences with social support were systematically examined by means of thematic analysis. In the last portion of the questionnaire, participants described in their own words their experiences regarding social support and the management of worry. Responses ranged between 83 and 326 words, with an average response of about 180 words (M = 178, SD = 24). Our analysis, as stated in Research Questions 3 and 4, addressed two separate issues: the experienced benefits of social support and the specific types of support they found helpful.
Coding was conducted by the primary investigator and two graduate assistants. All had extensive training in qualitative analysis as well as backgrounds in psychological counseling. Analysis was done in iterative phases over the course of six weeks, with coders meeting twice per week to compare coding notes and perform reliability checks.
In analyzing the roles played by social support, the following procedure was used. First, all participant responses were read through line by line to gain an overall familiarity with the data. Next, during a second pass through the data, “concepts” were identified by noting key points that were relevant to each issue in question (i.e. benefits of social support and/or types of social support). Each coder worked separately to identify concepts, which were then combined through discussions and a process of consensus into a common vocabulary. After the initial concepts were identified, their labels were reviewed, rearranged and grouped based on similarity. Throughout this coding process, agreement between coders was very high (K = .92). Over several iterations of this process, a set of higher-order “themes” was agreed to be representative of the manner in which lower order “concepts” could be meaningfully grouped. These “themes” were in turn examined for similarities and connecting patterns, eventually resulting in a set of four overarching “categories” that represent the major roles for social support described by the participants. Results are shown in Tables 2 and 3.
In analyzing the types of support participants found most beneficial, the first two stages of coding – those related to “concepts” and “themes” – were essentially the same, but for the final “category” stage, the themes identified within our Malaysian sample were matched with categories of social support (i.e. emotional, tangible and informational) borrowed from the literature (e.g. Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985; House & Kahn, Reference House, Kahn, Cohen and Syme1985). See Table 4 for these categories, along with concepts and themes derived from the Malaysian data.
Note: Categories taken from House and Kahn (Reference House, Kahn, Cohen and Syme1985).
Discussion
Overall, the quantitative data from this Malaysian sample echo previous studies from other countries. These results, discussed briefly under the headings “Worry and demographic groups in Malaysia” and “Worry and perceived social support”, suggest that broad patterns of worry and how it interacts with social support are not meaningfully different in Malaysia as compared to other countries. More noteworthy from a theoretical perspective, our qualitative analyses describe from the participants’ standpoint the experience of receiving social support and the roles that it serves in their lives. These roles represent the subjective, largely emotional benefits that participants experience when receiving social support. Understanding these roles, which are discussed under the heading “The role of social support in managing worry”, should be of use in providing direction for future research and better understanding of the nature of social support’s salutary effects. The next subsection, “Helpful actions by support providers”, discusses more specifically the behaviors that participants find most valuable when in need of support.
Worry and demographic groups in Malaysia
The results of our demographic comparisons were essentially in line with those of previous studies. No significant differences in normal or pathological worry were found for gender, marital status and ethnicity. These results are in agreement with previous research, which has been largely inconclusive with regard to the relationship of worry and most demographic categories (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Antony and Barlow1992; Golden et al., Reference Golden, Conroy, Bruce, Denihan, Greene, Kirby and Lawlor2011; Robichaud et al., Reference Robichaud, Dugas and Conway2003; Tallis et al., Reference Tallis, Davey, Bond, Davey and Tallis1994). Furthermore, in line with previous research, there was a significant decrease in normal worry with age, but no difference in age for pathological worry (e.g. Babcock, Malonebeach, Hou, & Smith, Reference Babcock, Malonebeach, Hou and Smith2012; Powers, Wisocki, & Whitbourne, Reference Powers, Wisocki and Whitbourne1992).
Due to the limited size and general non-representativeness of these data (see limitations), it would not be appropriate to generalize much from these quantitative results. Overall, however, 9.1% of the total variance in normal worry levels in this study could be accounted for by age, indicating a moderately strong relationship. Previous research has suggested that young people, in general, have more “day-to-day” worries, or more concerns related to domains such as work, relationships, and future plans. It is easy to imagine 20-somethings being less settled in these areas compared to those in their 40s and 50s (e.g. Borkovec, Reference Borkovec1988; Valliant, Reference Valliant1977). Those prone to pathological worry, on the other hand, seem to be more anxious by disposition. So, rather than using worry as an adaptive tool in response to relevant challenges, they may be more prone to worry regardless of their situation (Newman et al., Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013). As a result, their tendency to worry may be less likely to decrease with age.
Worry and perceived social support
As expected, a significant negative relationship was evident between PSS and both pathological worry and normal worry. This finding is consistent with a large body of research demonstrating connections between social support, better mental health, and improved overall well-being (e.g. Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985; Hobfoll & Vaux, Reference Hobfoll, Vaux, Goldberger and Breznitz1993; Kawachi & Berkman, Reference Kawachi and Berkman2001; Wills, Reference Wills and Clark1991).
Considering that worry-related cognitive processes are central to many emotional disorders (e.g. Newman et al., Reference Newman, Llera, Erickson, Przeworski and Castonguay2013) and are symptomatic of a wide range of mental illnesses (Kertz et al., Reference Kertz, Bigda-Peyton, Rosmarin and Björgvinsson2012), this suggests that, if it in fact can reduce worry, social support might play an important buffering role between stressors and negative health outcomes. Again, the limited size and correlational nature of these data mean that the current findings cannot imply causality and are not generalizable. However, the large body of previous research connecting social support with improved outcomes supports the idea that social support can be both salutary and protective.
The role of social support in managing worry
The conception of social support as protective led to the final two research questions: How, and in what ways, does social support alleviate the experience of worry? These questions were approached through examining the content of long-form, open-ended written responses. Throughout our analysis of written responses, the most prominent, recurring theme was social support as a source of belonging and security. Social support reassures us that others care about us and thus has an intrinsic calming effect. Worry is akin to a feeling of potential danger or threat (de Jong-Meyer et al., Reference De Jong-Meyer, Beck and Riede2009; Dugas & Koerner, Reference Dugas and Koerner2005), often accompanied by concerns about one’s ability to deal with problems if they arise (Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston and Dugas1998) and a fear of failure (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990). One of the greatest perceived benefits of social support thus appears to simply be the knowledge that others are there to help, that one is not alone, and that others care and “have your back”, so to speak. Similarly, the simple knowledge or feeling that one is accepted and loved unconditionally, even in the case of failure, can go a long way towards alleviating worry. This finding is consistent with Feeney and Collins’ (Reference Feeney and Collins2015) conception of social support as a secure base or safe haven that provides feelings of safety, security and calm in the face of threat.
A second, closely related theme was a reduction in fear and a feeling of emotional relief. Fear is in many ways the underlying emotion behind worry (e.g. de Jong-Meyer et al., Reference De Jong-Meyer, Beck and Riede2009) Unallayed fear leads to chronic stress and many related problems. Support from others, it was reported, allowed participants to lessen their fears and relax. This relief comes partly from having an outlet to express emotions and worries and partly from the aforementioned knowledge that help, acceptance and forgiveness are available if needed. Again, the presence of an emotional secure base provides the individual with a safe haven to which they can retreat when threatened. It provides a protected harbour within which the individual can recover and regroup. Such emotional recovery, or returning to non-threatened emotional states, is a key to regaining perspective and maintaining effective engagement with the world.
The third major theme that arose in our analysis was the role of social support in helping to reappraise situations, or to reorient thought patterns in a realistic and constructive way. Those who are prone to worry tend to focus on the negative, or on worst case scenarios (Rassin, Reference Rassin2007). Moreover, worry-related thoughts tend to be abstract and less tangible in nature (Stöber, Reference Stöber1997), sometimes making it difficult for worriers to clearly identify and address the source of their worry (Borkovec et al., Reference Borkovec, Ray and Stober1998). Participants reported that social support often helped them think in more practical terms and provided realistic perspectives. By grounding thought in concrete facts rather than possibilities, social support can help the individual break out of ruminative cycles and focus their attention on matters that can be dealt with.
The final major theme was similar to the previous point, that social support can facilitate effective problem-solving and decision-making. An important component of worry is, as mentioned earlier, a feeling that one will not be able to cope adequately with problems; that one lacks the capability to solve problems if they arise (Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston and Dugas1998). This lack of confidence is often accompanied by a fear of failure and an inability to make decisions. Those prone to worry often delay making decisions or dealing with problems out of fear of failure or making mistakes, thus leaving problems unaddressed and, paradoxically, leading to more stress and worry (Rassin, Reference Rassin2007; Stöber & Joormann, Reference Stöber and Joormann2001b). Participants in this study reported that social support helped them feel more confident and motivated them to actively face challenges. In some cases, participants also reported receiving concrete assistance with problem-solving and decision-making (Lakey & Cohen, Reference Lakey, Cohen, Cohen, Underwood and Gottlieb2000; Thoits, Reference Thoits1982).
Helpful actions by support providers
The final objective of this study was to identify specific social support related behaviors that participants found helpful in alleviating their worry. Again, this involved a thematic analysis of open-ended responses. Support behaviors were classified in the same way as described earlier; first as lower-order concepts, then into higher order themes, and finally into broad categories (Table 4). In general, it was found that the support behaviors described by participants fell within three broad categories often described in the literature: emotional support, tangible support and informational support (House & Kahn, Reference House, Kahn, Cohen and Syme1985). Thus, these same categories were used to classify the themes and concepts listed in Table 4. In a broad sense, the types of actions reported as helpful by these Malaysian participants and those from other cultural contexts were similar, indicating a certain universality in the experience of social support. Generally speaking, social support helps the individual process and regulate emotions; it provides practical help with specific problems, and it helps one to better understand the issues involved.
There also appear to be, based on the results from this study, aspects of support that are particularly relevant to the management of worry; in particular, the idea of showing unconditional acceptance and care for the person in distress often arose. Worry is, at its root, an expression of anxiety or fear of the unknown. Often, more than anything, the person who is worried wants to feel safe. They want to be reassured that things will be all right and that help is available if it is needed. Advice, ideas and guidance, and clarity of thinking have their time and place of course, but more than anything, participants in this study wanted to have their fears allayed; they wanted to feel safe and accepted.
Limitations and future studies
This study had numerous limitations. Although the ethnic composition of this sample was similar to Malaysia as a whole, in most other ways it was not representative. Due to limited resources, participants were recruited online using social media advertisements and snowballing techniques. This resulted in a sample that was much more highly educated than the population of Malaysia as a whole (e.g. 96% of this sample were college educated or above, compared to 42% of the broader population). They were also overwhelmingly female (71% female compared to 49% in the general population). A representative sample would need to include a broader swathe of educational/socioeconomic backgrounds as well as more men. To achieve this, data would need to be collected in several languages. The participants here were highly educated, so the use of English did not pose a problem. However, a representative sampling process, in order to be more balanced, would need to recruit participants offline in a variety of geographical regions. This would require collecting data in Bahasa Melayu as well as other regional dialects.
These findings also suggest additional research. Given that anxiety has specifically been identified as a growing problem in Malaysia, future studies should investigate how social support specifically relates to anxiety. For example, studies could attempt to quantify the roles of social support identified here (e.g. emotional safe-haven, unconditional acceptance, support in cognitive reappraisal) and look at their correlations with standard anxiety measures. Measuring the degree to which each component role of social support specifically relates to mental health outcomes could help clarify the mechanisms involved in social support’s salutary effects. Similarly, measuring these component roles of social support could be of use in developing and targeting future interventions.
Conclusion
This study, by looking at open-ended descriptions of participants’ experiences with social support provides important insight into the ways in which social support can help the individual address life’s challenges. For these participants, the greatest perceived benefits of social support were emotional. Feelings of security, comfort and being cared about were consistently reported as primary benefits of social support. Second to this was help in reassessing situations – seeing problems in a more practical way and separating facts from feelings.
Those who engage in worry are often insecure about themselves and their capability to handle threatening situations (Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston and Dugas1998). They want assurance that even if they fail, things will be okay and that they will not be abandoned. After this, of course, participants reported increases in confidence when given advice and guidance. And, they appreciated it when they were given practical advice about their strengths and weaknesses or errors in their thinking. Most important, however, most of our participants reported wanting to feel safe (e.g. Bonn, Reference Bonn2015; Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1988). Just as a child, when afraid, might return to her caregiver for assurance and support, our participants, when feeling worried or anxious, experienced social support as a secure base to which they could return for comfort, safety, and reassurance.
Acknowledgements
This project received support from the Global Asia in the 21st Century Research Platform of Monash University. Project Code: E/EE/LTg_01/2018/01.