Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T01:24:26.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Depression and executive functioning bidirectionally impair one another across 9 years: Evidence from within-person latent change and cross-lagged models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 June 2021

Nur Hani Zainal*
Affiliation:
National University of Singapore, Kent Ridge Campus, Singapore
Michelle G. Newman
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University, State College, Pennsylvania, USA
*
*Author for correspondence: Nur Hani Zainal, E-mail [email protected]

Abstract

Background

Scar and vulnerability models assert that increased psychopathology may predict subsequent executive functioning (EF) deficits (and vice versa) over protracted timescales, yet most prior work on this topic has been cross-sectional. Thus, we tested the within- and between-person relations between EF, depression, and anxiety.

Methods

Older adult participants (n = 856) were assessed across four waves, approximately 2 years apart. Performance-based EF and caregiver-rated symptom measures were administered. Bivariate latent change score and random-intercept cross-lagged panel models were conducted.

Results

Within persons, random-intercept cross-lagged panel models revealed that prior greater depression forecasted lower subsequent EF, and vice versa (d = −0.292 vs. −0.292). Bivariate dual latent change score models showed that within-person rise in depression predicted EF decreases, and vice versa (d = −0.245 vs. −0.245). No within-person, cross-lagged, EF-anxiety relations emerged. Further, significant negative between-person EF-symptom relations were observed (d = −0.264 to −0.395).

Conclusion

Prospective, within-person findings offer some evidence for developmental scar and vulnerability models.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Psychiatric Association

In daily life, most of us depend on our global executive functioning (EF) capacity to effectively accomplish tasks, communicate, handle emotions, make choices, prioritize goals, and solve problems [Reference Nigg1,Reference McClelland and Cameron2]. Global EF is defined as a group of multidomain cognitive control systems entwined with attention, information processing, and other cognitive abilities [Reference Karr, Areshenkoff, Rast, Hofer, Iverson and Garcia-Barrera3,Reference Baggetta and Alexander4]. Our global EF systems comprise facets of inhibition (capacity to abstain from autopilot actions), working memory (WM; ability to alter cognitive representations with incoming data in real-time), shifting (adeptness to flexibly switch from one mental set to another) [Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt5], and verbal fluency [Reference Aita, Beach, Taylor, Borgogna, Harrell and Hill6]. Relatedly, evidence has shown consistently that language-based, temporal lobe-mediated verbal fluency ability (marked by scores on diverse time-limited word generation on animal- and phonemic-cued tests) had strong and unique relations with common EF variance (i.e., global EF capacity) in diverse youth and adult samples [Reference Gustavson, Panizzon, Franz, Reynolds, Corley and Hewitt7Reference Gustavson, Elman, Panizzon, Franz, Zuber and Sanderson-Cimino10]. Given its importance, EF problems have been linked to issues with career, social relationships, diet, nutrition, and health [Reference Favieri, Forte and Casagrande11,Reference Pineda-Alhucema, Aristizabal, Escudero-Cabarcas, Acosta-López and Vélez12]. Executive dysfunction-related health problems include cardiorespiratory, metabolic, neuroendocrine, and psychiatric disorders [Reference Berryman, Stanton, Bowering, Tabor, McFarlane and Moseley13,Reference Hofmann, Schmeichel and Baddeley14]. Thus, understanding the risk factors and consequences of EF decrements is essential.

Scar theories propose that increases in psychiatric symptoms can precede and predict future EF decline. Specifically, scar models posit that chronic increased depression and anxiety may build up oxidative and inflammatory-stress, thereby adversely impacting EF-related brain regions over protracted durations [Reference Gałecki, Talarowska, Anderson, Berk and Maes15Reference Copeland, Shanahan, Worthman, Angold and Costello17]. Relatedly, scar models such as the vascular- [Reference Alexopoulos, Meyers, Young, Campbell, Silbersweig and Charlson18] and executive dysfunction syndrome-depression [Reference Alexopoulos19] hypotheses assert that increased depression and anxiety could impair future EF via buildup of tissue injury (e.g., lacunes, microinfarcts, and white matter hyperintensities) in cardiovascular systems, cognitive control-, and reward processing-related brain regions, over long timescales [Reference Alexopoulos20]. These brain areas might include frontal–striatal pathways (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, and anterior cingulate cortex) [Reference Alexopoulos, Hoptman, Yuen, Kanellopoulos, Seirup and Lim21,Reference Alexopoulos, Hoptman, Kanellopoulos, Murphy, Lim and Gunning22].

Thus far, 47 longitudinal studies have offered support for scar models. For instance, higher depression severity during adolescence was associated with lower vocabulary score in early adulthood 8 years later [Reference Beaujean, Parker and Qiu23]; however, whether such pattern applied to various stages in adulthood could not be inferred from that study. Other studies suggested such a possibility. Swedish and American adults with (vs. without) major depression displayed worsened episodic memory, EF, or verbal fluency after 6 months to 5 years despite symptom remission [Reference Hammar, Sørensen, Årdal, Oedegaard, Kroken and Roness24,Reference Wilson, Mendes de Leon, Bennett, Bienias and Evans25]. Likewise, among mid-life and older community adults, increased anxiety was related to reduced immediate and delayed auditory memory abilities following 12 years [Reference Gulpers, Oude Voshaar, van Boxtel, Verhey and Köhler26]. Similarly, 2 meta-analyses of 43 studies showed that heightened anxiety and depression dovetailed with larger EF decline and incidence of major neurocognitive disorders in diverse community and clinical samples across 1–17 years [Reference Gulpers, Ramakers, Hamel, Köhler, Oude Voshaar and Verhey27,Reference Diniz, Butters, Albert, Dew and Reynolds28].

Simultaneously, vulnerability models argue that EF decline can function as a precursor of later heightened depression and anxiety. Vulnerability models assert that poorer EF may forecast future anxiety and depression across prolonged periods due to chronic problems with disengaging from negative self-referential perseverative thinking (e.g., worry and rumination) [Reference Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan and De Raedt29,Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer30]. Likewise, EF deficits can make it perpetually hard to detach from threats, leading to excessive focus on anxiety-inducing factors in one’s surroundings and risk for increased anxiety [Reference Cabrera, Brugos and Montorio31,Reference Barry, Vervliet and Hermans32]. Moreover, it has been thought that poorer EF, especially WM, can predict increased depression and anxiety across long durations, in part due to difficulties with adjusting to various changing emotion-eliciting contexts in versatile and optimal ways [Reference Hantke, Gyurak, Van Moorleghem, Waring, Adamson and O’Hara33]. In sum, vulnerability theories argue that worse EF may forecast increased depression and anxiety over long durations.

To date, 31 prospective investigations have empirically supported vulnerability theories. For example, an earlier study demonstrated that poorer WM was related to future chronic course of increased depression [Reference Harvey, Le Bastard, Pochon, Levy, Allilaire and Dubois34]. Likewise, reduced inhibition, WM, shifting, verbal fluency, and other cognitive functioning indices were connected with pathological worry dimensionally and categorically 9 years later in community adults [Reference Zainal and Newman35]. More recently, meta-analytic data on 29 studies (n = 121,749) showed that cognitive deficits were associated with increased major depression severity following several months to 45 years in diverse clinical and community-dwelling samples [Reference Scult, Paulli, Mazure, Moffitt, Hariri and Strauman36].

However, the mostly two-time-point, between-person, regression studies testing the prospective relations between mental health symptoms and global EF to date introduce shortcomings to clinical science. Such methods do not account for the nesting of repeated assessments within persons to capture change-to-future change trajectories across time [Reference Molenaar, Huizenga, Nesselroade, Staudinger and Lindenberger37]. Mounting global pressures related to neuropsychiatric illnesses, increasing life expectancy, and aging [Reference Lang, Clifford, Wei, Zhang, Leung and Augustine38Reference Cohen, Edmondson and Kronish40] make it crucial to explore whether change in global EF over long durations may be related to future change in mental illness during adulthood development. Further, between-person differences across time may be due to stable variations observed across the lifespan [Reference Gustavson, Panizzon, Elman, Franz, Reynolds and Jacobson41], or to individual differences in aging-associated rate of EF decrements [Reference Braun, Schmukle and Kunzmann42Reference Yu, Yang, Geldhof, Tyler, Gansert and Chase45]. The latter possibility can only be captured by using within-person methods that also capture change. Moreover, the foregoing scar and vulnerability theories posit that EF-symptom relations unfold within persons across long durations [Reference Hülür, Ram, Willis, Schaie and Gerstorf46Reference Allerhand, Gale and Deary51]. Awareness of within-person prolonged trajectories of increased depression or anxiety, EF decrements, and their covariation may guide the design of personalized prevention, diagnostic, and treatment efforts that rely on idiographic (or within-person) more than between-person data, as part of precision psychiatry [Reference Lenze, Nicol, Barbour, Kannampallil, Wong and Piccirillo52Reference Zuidersma, Riese, Snippe, Booij, Wichers and Bos54]. It is also important to note that observations of between- and within-person differences in EF and psychopathological symptoms do not always align with each other [Reference Moore, Campbell, Delgadillo, Paolillo, Sundermann and Holden55Reference Zainal and Newman57]. To broaden and deepen comprehension of EF and mental health in mid-life and older adulthood, within-person (co)variations and change must be considered. Tethering within-person data analytic approaches with longitudinal study designs is thus important to comprehend the bidirectional within-person changes in EF and subsequent changes in symptoms (and vice versa).

Two cutting-edge techniques that attain these aims are random-intercept cross-lagged panel models (RI-CLPM) [Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman58] and bivariate dual latent change score models (BLCS) [Reference Grimm and Ram59]; two forms of longitudinal structural equation modeling (SEM). These longitudinal SEM approaches benefit researchers by accounting for prior lagged relations and regression to the mean, minimizing measurement unreliability, and using all available values instead of listwise deletion [Reference Zainal and Newman57]. Further, by adjusting for temporally stable between-person differences and autoregressive effects, these models can test if change in one variable across a previous time-period or time-lag is associated with change in another variable at the next time-period or time-lag within persons. Accordingly, by evaluating lead–lag change-to-future change connections, RI-CLPM and BLCS models move us toward the ability to draw causal inferences [Reference Mund and Nestler60]; inquiries essential to clinical science.

Thus far, three studies of adult participants have tested the longitudinal, dynamic, within-person relations between EF and anxiety, depression, or pertinent concepts with BLCS. Using BLCS, increase in anxiety was related to cognitive functioning decline in older adults [Reference Zainal and Newman57]; despite that, the two-wave study prevented understanding of how symptom change predicted subsequent EF change (and conversely). Relatedly, BLCS models across three waves showed that 9-year growth in excessive worry dovetailed with future 9-year decline in global and unique EF facets [Reference Zainal and Newman49]; however, whether change in EF forecasted later change in worry was not examined. Another five time-point study demonstrated that rise in trait neuroticism at one time-lag preceded and linked to reductions in spatial processing, WM, and processing speed at the next time-lag [Reference Zainal and Newman57]; nonetheless, one-item assessments of cognitive functioning were used in the study. To our knowledge, no studies have tested EF-psychological symptom relations in older adults with RI-CLPM. However, a recent study in youths that utilized RI-CLPM [Reference Oh, Greenberg, Willoughby, Vernon-Feagans, Greenberg and Blair61] suggested the possibility of EF problems serving as risk factors for later increased depression and anxiety.

Building on this literature, this study aimed to examine the within-person associations between a global EF composite (formed via a latent composite of five measures) and depression or anxiety severity using RI-CLPM and BLCS in older adults. Based on scar theories, we hypothesized that within persons, higher anxiety or depression severity would reliably precede and relate to greater future EF decline at the next time-point and time-lag. Moreover, based on vulnerability models, we hypothesized that within persons, lower EF would forecast subsequent rise or increased depression or anxiety severity at the next time-point or time-lag. Last, using a SEM-based model comparison approach [Reference Widaman, Ferrer and Conger62], we aimed to directly juxtapose the effect sizes indicating the scar (vs. vulnerability) hypothesis to determine if any differences emerged.

Method

Participants

The present study was a secondary analysis of the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study (ADAMS) publicly available and restricted-use datasets [Reference Steven, Gwenith, Michael, Kenneth, Mary Beth and Brenda63]. Ethical approval was provided by the University of Michigan and Duke University Medical Center, and all participants voluntarily consented to enroll. Participants (n = 856) averaged 81.59 years of age (SD = 7.10, range = 70–110), 58.53% were female, and 76.87% identified as White, compared to the other 23.13% who identified African American or other ethnicities. In addition, 28.62% (n = 245) of the participants needed support for dressing, feeding, or bathing based on caregiver report, or were diagnosed with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-Fourth Edition-Text Revised (DSM-IV-TR) [64,65]—defined dementia, major depressive disorder, stroke, or other neurological condition. These dementia syndromes included probable and possible Alzheimer’s disease, cardiovascular, and other causes. All DSM-IV-TR diagnoses were attained through expert consensus with a multidisciplinary team of neurologists, psychiatrists, geriatric psychologists, and other healthcare professionals [Reference Langa, Plassman, Wallace, Herzog, Heeringa and Ofstedal66]. The online supplementary material (OSM) offers more details on the sample characteristics.

Procedures

Participants completed performance-based EF measures and had a significant other caregiver (e.g., spouse and children) who could reliably report on their behavioral symptoms across multiple time-points. Data were collected across four waves in 2004 (Time 1; T1), 2006 (Time 2; T2), 2008 (Time 3; T3), and 2010 (Time 4; T4) [Reference Langa, Plassman, Wallace, Herzog, Heeringa and Ofstedal66]. The following caregiver-rated symptom assessment and EF tests were administered.

Measures

Mental health symptoms

The widely used caregiver-rated Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) [Reference Cummings, Mega, Gray, Rosenberg-Thompson, Carusi and Gornbein67]—depression and anxiety domains were utilized to assess past-month depression and anxiety severity in the form of a structured interview. Caregiver ratings were used in this study as self-reported mental health symptom severity data was only available at T1 and T2 [Reference Steven, Gwenith, Michael, Kenneth, Mary Beth and Brenda63,Reference Mehta, Stewart, Langa, Yaffe, Moody-Ayers and Williams68,Reference Fisher, Franks, Plassman, Brown, Potter and Llewellyn69], and due to the reliable nature of caregiver-reported data that tends to align with self-rated symptom measures [Reference Steffens, Fisher, Langa, Potter and Plassman70]. To measure depression, caregivers were asked about the presence and duration of depression symptoms (e.g., sadness, irritability, feels worthless, and suicidal thoughts) the participant may have exhibited. To assess for anxiety, caregivers were inquired on the presence of any anxiety symptoms (e.g., excessive worry, breathlessness, and behavioral avoidance). Also, for each participant’s symptom domain, the informants reported on the degree of the following four facets: severity (3-point scale; 1 = mild to 3 = marked); change from past typical behaviors (3-point scale; 0 = no; 1 = yes; 2 = exaggeration of previous problems); distress (6-point scale; 0 = not at all to 5 = very severely or extremely). Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 in the OSM show that these four manifest indicators for the depression and anxiety scales had excellent model fit using a series of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) at distinct time-points. Further, the NPI has reliably shown strong internal consistency, as well as convergent and discriminant validity [Reference Cummings, Mega, Gray, Rosenberg-Thompson, Carusi and Gornbein67,Reference de Medeiros, Robert, Gauthier, Stella, Politis and Leoutsakos71]. In this study, internal consistencies were high for the depression severity (Cronbach’s αs = 0.93, 0.94, 0.92, and 0.93 at T1, T2, T3, and T4, respectively) and anxiety severity scales (αs = 0.93, 0.96, 0.93, and 0.90 at T1, T2, T3, and T4, respectively).

Executive functioning

The following five measures of EF were used to create a composite latent global EF composite: (a) controlled oral word association (a verbal fluency assessment that captures unplanned generation of words within a time limit that start with some assigned letter) [Reference Borkowski, Benton and Spreen72]; (b) animal fluency (another time-limited verbal fluency test based on the animal category) [Reference Ardila, Ostrosky‐Solís and Bernal73]; (c) serial 7 subtraction (extent of accuracy of counting down from 100 by 7 within a time limit) [Reference Manning74]; (d) backward digit span (degree of accuracy of recall in reverse order of integer strings of increasing length) [Reference Wechsler75]; and (e) symbol digit modality test (level of accuracy of replacing a single-digit integer for randomized displays of geometric patterns) [Reference Smith76]. These EF assessments have been shown to have good internal consistency, strong convergent, and discriminant validity [Reference Oliveira, Mograbi, Gabrig and Charchat-Fichman77Reference Hinton-Bayre and Geffen79]. In this study, the αs for the global EF composite were strong across all time-points (αs = 0.92, 0.83, 0.87, and 0.88 at T1, T2, T3, and T4, respectively). Moreover, at each time-point, a composite global EF index was created by standardizing each EF measure and averaging the scores across EF measures. Further, exploratory factor analysis and a series of CFA demonstrated that a one-factor latent global EF composite had good model fit across waves of assessment (refer to page 4 and Supplementary Table S3 of the OSM). In addition, these global EF scores have been normed based on age and education, and appropriate adjustments were made for participants with hearing impairments [Reference Fong, Fearing, Jones, Shi, Marcantonio and Rudolph80Reference Welsh, Breitner and Magruder-Habib83]. Also, Supplementary Table S4 in the OSM shows the descriptive statistics of the study variables based on SEM analyses.

Data analyses

All longitudinal SEM analyses were performed with the lavaan package [Reference Rosseel84] in R Version 3.6.3. Model fit was assessed utilizing practical fit indices and heuristic cut-offs: confirmatory fit index (CFI; CFI ≥ 0.90) [Reference Bentler85] and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; RMSEA ≤ 0.09) [Reference Steiger86]. To maximize all available data points, we used full information maximum likelihood, the gold standard [Reference Graham87], to manage missing data. In total, 16.29% of the data were missing. Further, the data were missing completely at random (χ2[df = 113] = 134.32, p = 0.084).

Next, we established longitudinal measurement invariance; a prerequisite for longitudinal SEM [Reference McArdle88]. We progressively evaluated for configural invariance (equivalence of factor structure), metric invariance (equal factor structure and item loadings [λs], freely estimated item intercepts [τs], and item error variances [εs] across the time-points), scalar invariance (equal factor structure, λs, and τs, but freely estimated εs across time-points), and strict invariance (equal factor structure, λs, τs, and εs, across time-points) [Reference Widaman, Ferrer and Conger62]. To test for measurement invariance, we conducted a Δχ2 difference test. A statistically significant Δχ2 meant that the more (vs. less) restricted model had poorer fit [Reference Cheung and Rensvold89]. However, as Δχ2 is affected by sample size despite negligible misfit changes, the following change in practical fit indices, ΔCFI ≤ −0.01 or ΔRMSEA < +0.015 [Reference Chen90], from the less restricted to more restricted models signaled measurement nonequivalence.

The RI-CLPM was used to manage interdependent repeated assessments nested within persons, and to distinguish between within-person (dynamic state) variance and between-person (trait) variance [Reference Mulder and Hamaker91]. RI-CLPM procedures permitted us to test these within-person reciprocal cross-lagged relations (γs) accounting for within-person autoregressive effects (βs; level of one variable forecasting its subsequent level), trait variances (αs), and regression to the mean [Reference Falkenström, Finkel, Sandell, Rubel and Holmqvist92,Reference Usami93]. Of primary interest were the within-person cross-lagged associations between level of EF at a prior time-point (T − 1) and level of depression or anxiety symptom severity (SYM) at the next adjacent time-point (T) following about 2 years (γs) (and vice versa), as shown in Equations (1) and (2).

(1) $$ \unicode{x03BC} {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[ T\right]}={\unicode{x03B1}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B2}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B3}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]} $$
(2) $$ \unicode{x03BC} {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[ T\right]}={\unicode{x03B1}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B2}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B3}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[ T\hbox{-} 1\right]} $$

Concurrently, BLCS approaches were utilized to test if within-person change in depression or anxiety symptom severity at a previous time-lag (ΔT − 1) would be related to change in EF at the next successive time-lag (ΔT) (and vice versa). BLCS is a cutting-edge method that empowers researchers to test within-person change-to-future change associations (coupling effects; δs) after accounting for trait-level initial status, trait-level constant change parameters (αs), and within-person autoregressive paths (proportional effects; change in a variable predicting subsequent change in itself; βs) [Reference Grimm and Ram59]. The BLCS models relevant to our research question can be denoted in Equations (3) and (4) as follows.

(3) $$ \Delta {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T\right]}={\unicode{x03B1}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T- 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B2}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T- 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B4}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T- 1\right]} $$
(4) $$ \Delta {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T\right]}={\unicode{x03B1}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B2}}_{\mathrm{X}}\times {\left(\mathrm{SYM}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T\hbox{-} 1\right]}+{\unicode{x03B4}}_{\mathrm{Y}}\times {\left(\mathrm{EF}\right)}_{\left[\varDelta T\hbox{-} 1\right]} $$

As recommended, the within-person cross-lagged associations (γs) in the RI-CLPM and within-person coupling effects in the BLCS (δs) were constrained to be equal across waves of assessments to reduce SEs in parameter estimates (refer to Supplementary Figure S1 shows a BLCS model in [Reference McArdle94]). Also, baseline psychopathology and EF were controlled for in all models.

As we aimed to directly compare the scar and vulnerability hypotheses, we contrasted a model that freely estimated the cross-lagged or coupling effects (EF predicting future SYM and conversely) to a model that constrained the cross-lagged or coupling effects to equality. A statistically significant change (Δ) in χ2 value in comparing these two models would indicate notable differences in the strength of effect sizes for one hypothesis versus the other [Reference Widaman, Ferrer and Conger62]. If the Δχ2 test was not significant, the more parsimonious model with equality constraints on cross-lagged or coupling effects was chosen as the final model. Effect sizes were calculated using the formula, Cohen’s $ d=\frac{\beta }{SD\left(\beta \right)}\times \sqrt{\left(\frac{2}{N}\right)} $ [Reference Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow and Burke95], where β is the unstandardized regression estimate, SD(β) its standard deviation, and N is the sample size. Cohen’s d values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 indicated small, moderate, and large effects, respectively.

Power analysis

Following best practices [Reference Arend and Schafer96], an a priori Monte Carlo power analysis based on a conservative effect size of d = 0.20 for the cross-lagged effects in the RI-CLPM and coupling effects (bidirectional change-to-future change EF-symptom relations) in the BLCS was performed using the RAMpath R package [Reference Zhang, Hamagami, Grimm and McArdle97]. After 1,000 replications per condition, we observed 90.6–100.0% power to detect significant within-person cross-lagged or coupling effects. Further, there was 90.0–100.0% power to identify other significant parameter estimates.

Results

Longitudinal measurement invariance

Supplementary Table S5 in the OSM shows the longitudinal measurement invariance analyses for the constructs of interest. Overall, strict level of equivalence (equal λs, τs, εs) was observed for the constructs of depression severity, anxiety severity, and EF. Therefore, conducting analyses using RI-CLPM and BLCS approaches were appropriate.

Lagged relations between depression severity and executive function

Table 1 displays all of the parameter estimates for the RI-CLPM testing the cross-lagged relations between depression severity and EF. The model with equality constraints on the cross-lagged effects did not significantly differ from the model that freely estimated those parameters (Δχ2[df = 1] = 0.017, p = 0.895). The parsimonious model with equality constraints showed good model fit (χ2[df = 24] = 45.160, p = 0.006, CFI = 0.984, RMSEA = 0.032). Within persons, higher prior depression severity substantially predicted lower EF at the subsequent time-point (β = −0.073, 95% CI [−0.119, −0.026], d = −0.292). Likewise, lower previous EF significantly predicted greater depression severity at the next time-point within persons (β = −0.073, 95% CI [−0.119, −0.026], d = −0.292). Also, between persons, higher random intercept depression severity was significantly correlated with lower random intercept EF (β = −0.055, 95% CI [−0.094, −0.016], d = −0.264).

Table 1. Random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model of DEP and EF across four time-points.

Note: Model fit indices: χ2(df = 24) = 45.160, p = 0.006, CFI = 0.984, RMSEA = 0.032, 95% CI [0.017, 0.046]. Within-person cross-lagged effects refer to level in DEP at a prior time-point (T-1) predicting (➔) future Δ in EF at the next adjacent time-point (T) (and vice versa). Within-person coupling effects and proportional effects, residual covariances between DEP and EF, as well as variances of DEP and EF were each uniquely fixed to be equal across all three time-lags.

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; DEP, depression severity; EF, executive functioning; RI, random intercept.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

*** p < 0.001.

Table 2 presents the parameter estimates for the BLCS models examining the change-to-future change associations between depression severity and EF. The freely estimated (vs. constrained) models were not significantly different from each other (Δχ2[df = 1] = 0.235, p = 0.628). The final model with equality constraints on the coupling effects showed acceptable model fit (χ2[df = 25] = 47.000, p = 0.005, CFI = 0.974, RMSEA = 0.039, 95% CI [0.021, 0.057]). Within persons, greater growth in depression severity at a prior time-lag significantly predicted EF decrement at the next time-lag (β = −0.540, 95% CI [−0.955, −0.124], d = −0.245). Likewise, within persons, EF decline at a previous time-lag was significantly associated with larger increase in depression severity at the subsequent time-lag (β = −0.540, 95% CI [−0.955, −0.124], d = −0.245). Figures 1 and 2 summarize the analyses of the lagged relations between depression severity and EF.

Table 2. Bivariate dual latent change score model of DEP and EF across four time-points.

Note: Model fit indices: χ 2(df = 25) = 47.000, p = 0.005, CFI = 0.974, RMSEA = 0.039, 95% CI [0.021, 0.057]. Within-person coupling effects refer to change (Δ) in DEP at a prior time-lag (ΔT-1) predicting (➔) future Δ in EF at the next adjacent time-lag (ΔT; and vice versa). Within-person coupling effects and proportional effects, residual covariances between DEP and EF, as well as variances of DEP and EF were each uniquely fixed to be equal across all three time-lags.

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; DEP, depression severity; EF, executive functioning.

* p < 0.05;

** p < 0.01;

*** p < 0.001.

Figure 1. Random-Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Models Between EF and Depression Severity.

Note. **p < .01; ***p < .001. Δ = within-person change in construct from a time-lag to the next adjacent time-lag; DEP = depression severity; EF = executive functioning.

Figure 2. Bivariate Dual Latent Change Score Models Between EF and Depression Severity.

Note. **p < .01; ***p < .001. Δ = within-person change in construct from a time-lag to the next adjacent time-lag; DEP = depression severity; EF = executive functioning.

Lagged relations between anxiety severity and executive function

Table 3 shows the model parameter estimates for the RI-CLPM evaluating the cross-lagged relations between anxiety severity and EF. The freely estimated model was not significantly different from the constrained model (Δχ2[df = 1] = 0.069, p = 0.792). The final model with equality constraints on the cross-lagged effects demonstrated good model fit (χ2[df = 23] = 86.84, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.952, RMSEA = 0.057). Within persons, no cross-lagged relations were observed between prior anxiety severity and EF at the subsequent time-point (β = −0.025, 95% CI [−0.101, 0.051], d = −0.062). Likewise, no within-person cross-lagged relations were found between previous EF and anxiety severity at the next time-point (β = −0.025, 95% CI [−0.101, 0.051], d = −0.062). However, between persons, higher random intercept anxiety severity was significantly related to lower random intercept EF (β = −0.070, 95% CI [−0.104, −0.036], d = −0.395).

Table 3. Random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model of ANX and EF across four time-points.

Note: Model fit indices: χ2(df = 24) = 33.102, p = 0.102, CFI = 0.982, RMSEA = 0.021, 95% CI [0.008, 0.031]. Within-person cross-lagged effects refer to level in ANX at a prior time-point (T-1) predicting (➔) future Δ in EF at the next adjacent time-point (T) (and vice versa). Within-person coupling effects and proportional effects, residual covariances between ANX and EF, as well as variances of ANX and EF were each uniquely fixed to be equal across all three time-lags.

Abbreviations: ANX, anxiety severity; CI, confidence interval; EF, executive functioning; RI, random intercept.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

*** p < 0.001.

Table 4 shows the parameter estimates for the BLCS models testing the change-to-future change relations between anxiety severity and EF. The constrained (vs. freely estimated) models were not significantly different (Δχ2[df = 1] = 0.005, p = 0.943). The final model with equality constraints on the coupling parameters showed acceptable model fit (χ2[df = 25] = 46.996, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.966, RMSEA = 0.057). Within persons, prior change in anxiety severity at a previous time-lag was not significantly associated with change in EF at the subsequent time-lag (β = −0.254, 95% CI [−0.951, 0.444], d = −0.068) and vice versa (β = −0.254, 95% CI [−0.951, 0.444], d = −0.068).Footnote 1

Table 4. Bivariate dual latent change score model of ANX and EF across four time-points.

Note: Model fit indices: χ2(df = 25) = 46.996, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.966, RMSEA = 0.057, 95% CI [0.044, 0.071]. Within-person coupling effects refer to change (Δ) in ANX at a prior time-lag (ΔT-1) predicting (➔) future Δ in EF at the next adjacent time-lag (ΔT; and vice versa). Within-person coupling effects and proportional effects, residual covariances between ANX and EF, as well as variances of ANX and EF were each uniquely fixed to be equal across all three time-lags.

Abbreviations: ANX, anxiety severity; CI, confidence interval; EF, executive functioning.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

*** p < 0.001.

Discussion

Partially supporting scar and vulnerability hypotheses, robust RI-CLPM and BLCS methods showed that within persons, higher prior level and change in depression (but not anxiety) severity predicted greater reduced EF at the next time-point and subsequent time-lag, and conversely. Simultaneously, these models demonstrated stronger between-person, cross-sectional magnitude between EF and anxiety compared to EF and depression severity. Overall, findings concurred with up-to-date, cross-sectional, between-person evidence from recent meta-analytic data (e.g., [Reference Scult, Paulli, Mazure, Moffitt, Hariri and Strauman36]). Results also extended an early seminal cross-sectional study [Reference Christopher and MacDonald98] which observed that whereas patients with (vs. without) depression performed poorly on auditory and visual WM tasks, patients with anxiety disorders attained scores comparable to healthy controls. Findings also built on hierarchical linear modeling results that whereas inverse EF-depression relations tended to predominate within persons, negative EF-anxiety associations tended to be larger between persons [Reference Laukka, Dykiert, Allerhand, Starr and Deary99]. The divergence between within- and between-person findings for anxiety is likely due to the fact that between-person analyses do not account for individual differences in person-specific changes across time. Whereas between-person differences across time could be due to group differences in stable variations observed across the lifespan, they may not be capturing individual differences in aging-associated rate of EF or mental health deterioration. In fact, whereas prior between-person findings were interpreted to suggest that moderate levels of anxiety (but not depression) could facilitate performance on EF tests up to a certain point, this relation has not held up when examined at the within-person level [Reference Bierman, Comijs, Jonker and Beekman100Reference Sari, Koster and Derakshan102]. Another potential explanation pertains to the fact that anxiety (vs. depression) severity tends to be more stable across the lifespan, as illustrated by prospective [Reference Wetherell, Gatz and Pedersen103] and gene–environment studies [Reference Lee, Gatz, Pedersen and Prescott104]. Accordingly, higher stability and lower variability in anxiety severity across the lifespan could translate to stronger predominance of between-person, as opposed to within-person, effects on EF over long durations. Clearly, more longitudinal work is needed to test these notions.

Why did rise in depression severity consistently predict future EF decline at the next time-point and time-lag within persons? Overall, our findings offered support for scar theories. Conceivably, recurrent depression episodes might be a factor in cognitive functioning decline and diseased neurological aging processes (e.g., shrinkage in learning- and EF-linked brain regions and white matter hyperintensities) over the years [Reference Taylor, McQuoid, Payne, Zannas, MacFall and Steffens105,Reference Elbejjani, Fuhrer, Abrahamowicz, Mazoyer, Crivello and Tzourio106]. Biologically, elevated depression might have this adverse effect on EF across prolonged durations via chronic wear-and-tear of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function, such as buildup of glucocorticoids and proinflammatory cytokines (e.g., C-reactive protein) [Reference Zainal and Newman107,Reference Lebedeva, Sundström, Lindgren, Stomby, Aarsland and Westman108]. On that note, elevated depression might precede or speed up the onset of dementia, possibly via the accumulation of neurofibrillary plaques and tangles in emotion modulation-, EF-, and learning-related brain areas [Reference Butters, Klunk, Mathis, Price, Ziolko and Hoge109Reference Butters, Sweet, Mulsant, Ilyas Kamboh, Pollock and Begley111]. Equally tenable are scar models centering on behavioral, environmental, and lifestyle factors observed for extended durations in depression (e.g., decreased physical exercise, suboptimal sleep, diet, and nutrition), that could impact proinflammatory and cardiovascular processes [Reference Wang, Zhou, Chen, Jing, He and Sun112,Reference Alvaro, Roberts and Harris113]. Future longitudinal studies using RI-CLPM and BLCS models can further examine the “neurotoxic” scar effect of increased depression.

Findings suggested that reduced EF functioned as a risk factor for subsequent heightened depression (but not anxiety) within persons. This could be because poorer EF may have compromised abilities to harness “top-down” cognitive control over depressed mood (but not necessarily anxiety symptoms), and to refocus thoughts and actions to create and sustain more positive emotions (e.g., via engaging in mood-lifting activities or searching for suitable social support). However, the result that change in EF deficits did not forecast change in future anxiety within persons was inconsistent with prior longitudinal, between-person studies that found evidence supporting the vulnerability hypothesis. For instance, two studies showed that EF deficits were risk factors for generalized anxiety disorder symptoms across time (e.g., [Reference Zainal and Newman35,Reference Zhang, Norton, Carriere, Ritchie, Chaudieu and Ancelin114]). Also, using BLCS, two studies observed that within-person rise in anxiety or trait neuroticism at a time-lag predicted worsened cognitive functioning at the next time-lag [Reference Zainal and Newman57,Reference Petkus, Reynolds, Wetherell, Kremen and Gatz115] in community-dwelling Swedish adults. Similarly, another recent BLCS study found that within persons, worse cognitive functioning forecasted increased anxiety and depression across 4 years in patients with Parkinson’s disease [Reference Petkus, Filoteo, Schiehser, Gomez and Petzinger116]. Differences in data analysis (e.g., linear regression vs. SEM), sample characteristics (e.g., age and data collection site), anxiety measures (e.g., worry vs. anxiety symptoms), and study design (e.g., time-lags) might account for such variability in findings.

In addition, between persons, cross-sectional relations between lower EF and greater depression or anxiety severity were reliably observed. Observations at the between-person level are concordant with several community-based studies. For example, poorer EF facets (e.g., shifting and inhibition) or global cognition have been shown to consistently forecast increased worry, anxiety, and depression at a later time in children [Reference Nelson, Kidwell, Nelson, Tomaso, Hankey and Espy117], adolescents [Reference Kertz, Belden, Tillman and Luby118], mid-life adults [Reference Zainal and Newman35], and older adults [Reference Zhang, Norton, Carriere, Ritchie, Chaudieu and Ancelin114], across 3–12 years. Our study extended those findings by bolstering arguments that the strength and sign of magnitudes between within- and between-person associations might not coincide [Reference Fisher, Medaglia and Jeronimus56]. The field can benefit from using prospective designs (e.g., cross-panel and ecological momentary assessment) and SEM to clarify the between- and within-person relations among EF, depression, and anxiety severity across years and smaller timescales (e.g., within-day and day-to-day fluctuations) [Reference Sliwinski, Mogle, Hyun, Munoz, Smyth and Lipton48,Reference McNeish and Hamaker119].

Relatedly, the cross-sectional, between-person negative associations between anxiety or depression and EF in this study may be accounted for by the attentional control theory [Reference Eysenck and Derakshan120] and attentional scope model of rumination [Reference Whitmer and Gotlib121]. Note that these theories are inappropriate for explaining the within-person, cross-lagged, and long-term change-to-future change relations between EF and depression severity found herein as they argue that symptom-EF perturbation relations occur across brief durations or at one time-point [Reference Zainal and Newman49,Reference Beckwé, Deroost, Koster, De Lissnyder and De Raedt122]. Further, these models assert that elevated symptoms could deplete finite EF resources for task-pertinent processing and increased anxiety and depression would be reliably linked to greater cognitive rigidity (i.e., difficulty disengaging from threat or distractions) at a single time-point. Such mechanisms may unfold through excessive repetitive negative thinking, such as worry, brooding, and obsessions, as consistently evidenced by cross-sectional or experimental meta-analytic data [Reference Yang, Cao, Shields, Teng and Liu123Reference Snyder, Kaiser, Warren and Heller125].

Findings must be interpreted in light of study strengths and limitations. Unmeasured third variables (e.g., genetics) [Reference Friedman, Miyake, Altamirano, Corley, Young and Rhea126] may have contributed to observed outcomes. Additionally, although other studies have observed within- or between-person relations between depression and EF domains of shifting and inhibition [Reference Zainal and Newman49], consistent with theory and neuroanatomical evidence [Reference Xi, Liu, Zhong, Yang, Peng and Yi127], these EF facets were not measured herein. Also, as no structured diagnostic interviews were included, future studies that include such diagnostic instruments could determine if the results would be similar. In addition, given the predominantly White sample, subsequent investigations can determine if outcomes extend to culturally diverse populations by conducting multiple-group SEM (e.g., [Reference Zainal, Newman and Hong128]). Limitations notwithstanding, study strengths included the large and well-powered sample size, administration of behavioral EF and caregiver-rated symptom assessments, four-wave cross-panel longitudinal dataset, and use of potent SEM approaches.

If the pattern of results herein was replicated, some clinical implications deserve consideration. Offering preventive interventions at early signals of increased depression might assist with remediating depression, but would also probably benefit EF capacities. Moreover, the field could benefit from continuing to test EF indices as reliable predictors or markers of treatment response for depression and anxiety, as suggested by various studies (e.g., [Reference Douglas and Porter129,Reference Mohlman130]). Relatedly, based on recent evidence, such efforts might be augmented by investigating if cognitive-behavioral therapies (CBTs) (e.g., behavioral activation, cognitive remediation, problem-solving therapy, personalized, environment-focused, and technology-facilitated CBTs) [Reference Chen, Etnier, Chan, Chiu, Hung and Chang131Reference Alexopoulos, Raue, Kiosses, Mackin, Kanellopoulos and McCulloch136], mindfulness-based interventions [Reference Zainal and Newman137], EF training [Reference Karbach and Verhaeghen138], and pharmacological treatments [Reference Bastos, Pinto Guimarães and Trentini139], could simultaneously alleviate depression and enhance EF capacities.

Financial Support

The ADAMS study has been funded by the following grants and agencies: the National Institute on Aging (grant numbers U01 AG09740 and K08 AG19180); the New Investigator Research Grant from the Alzheimer’s Association; the Paul Beeson Physician Faculty Scholars in Aging Research award; National Institutes of Health (grant number K12 HD01438); and the National Institute of Mental Health (grant number K24 70027). The original investigators and funding agency are not responsible for the analyses or interpretations presented here.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from Health and Retirement Study (HRS)—ADAMS—website. Data are available at https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/publications/biblio/5761 with the permission of the study team principal investigators and team members, G. H. Steven, G. F. Gwenith, D. H. Michael, M. L. Kenneth, O. Mary Beth, L. P. Brenda, R. W. David, and colleagues.

Authorship Contributions

The research team, Michelle G. Newman, and Nur Hani Zainal took full responsibility for the data, the accuracy of analyses and interpretation, as well as conduct of the research. All authors have made substantial contributions to analysis and interpretation of the study and its findings; drafted and revised the article for intellectual content; gave their final approval of the version to be submitted; and read and approved the manuscript.

Ethical Statement

This study was conducted in compliance with the American Psychological Association (APA) and Declaration of Helsinki ethical standards in the treatment of human participants and approved by the institutional review board (IRB). Informed consent was obtained from participants as per IRB requirements at the University of Michigan at Ann Arbor and Duke University. Since this study used a publicly available dataset, it was exempt from additional IRB approval.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.2217.

Footnotes

1 As part of a sensitivity analysis, we determined that the results were similar when analyses were restricted to a sample without baseline major depression and cognitive or physical disabilities (n = 611), as shown on page 9 of the OSM.

References

Nigg, JT. On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:361–83. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, MM, Cameron, CE. Developing together: the role of executive function and motor skills in children’s early academic lives. Early Child Res Q. 2019;46:142–51. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karr, JE, Areshenkoff, CN, Rast, P, Hofer, SM, Iverson, GL, Garcia-Barrera, MA. The unity and diversity of executive functions: a systematic review and re-analysis of latent variable studies. Psychol Bull. 2018;144:1147–85. doi:10.1037/bul0000160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baggetta, P, Alexander, PA. Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 2016;10:1033. doi:10.1111/mbe.12100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, NP, Miyake, A, Young, SE, DeFries, JC, Corley, RP, Hewitt, JK. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2008;137:201–25. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aita, SL, Beach, JD, Taylor, SE, Borgogna, NC, Harrell, MN, Hill, BD. Executive, language, or both? An examination of the construct validity of verbal fluency measures. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26:441–51. doi:10.1080/23279095.2018.1439830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, DE, Panizzon, MS, Franz, CE, Reynolds, CA, Corley, RP, Hewitt, JK, et al. Integrating verbal fluency with executive functions: evidence from twin studies in adolescence and middle age. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2019;148:2104–19. doi:10.1037/xge0000589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, DC, Kaplan, E, Kramer, JH. The Delis–Kaplan executive function system: examiner’s manual. San Antonio, NM: The Psychological Corporation; 2001.Google Scholar
Friesen, DC, Edwards, K, Lamoureux, C. Predictors of verbal fluency performance in monolingual and bilingual children: the interactive role of English receptive vocabulary and fluid intelligence. J Commun Disord. 2021;89:106074 doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.106074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, DE, Elman, JA, Panizzon, MS, Franz, CE, Zuber, J, Sanderson-Cimino, M, et al. Association of baseline semantic fluency and progression to mild cognitive impairment in middle-aged men. Neurology. 2020;95:e973–83. doi:10.1212/wnl.0000000000010130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Favieri, F, Forte, G, Casagrande, M. The executive functions in overweight and obesity: a systematic review of neuropsychological cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2126 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pineda-Alhucema, W, Aristizabal, E, Escudero-Cabarcas, J, Acosta-López, JE, Vélez, JI. Executive function and theory of mind in children with ADHD: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28:341–58. doi:10.1007/s11065-018-9381-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berryman, C, Stanton, TR, Bowering, KJ, Tabor, A, McFarlane, A, Moseley, GL. Do people with chronic pain have impaired executive function? A meta-analytical review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34:563–79. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W, Schmeichel, BJ, Baddeley, AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:174–80. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gałecki, P, Talarowska, M, Anderson, G, Berk, M, Maes, M. Mechanisms underlying neurocognitive dysfunctions in recurrent major depression. Med Sci Monit. 2015;21:1535–47. doi:10.12659/MSM.893176.Google ScholarPubMed
Juruena, MF, Bocharova, M, Agustini, B, Young, AH. Atypical depression and non-atypical depression: Is HPA axis function a biomarker? A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2018;233:4567. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, WE, Shanahan, L, Worthman, C, Angold, A, Costello, EJ. Generalized anxiety and C-reactive protein levels: a prospective, longitudinal analysis. Psychol Med. 2012;42:2641–50. doi:10.1017/S0033291712000554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Meyers, BS, Young, RC, Campbell, S, Silbersweig, D, Charlson, M. ‘Vascular depression’ hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:915–22. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220033006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS. Mechanisms and treatment of late-life depression. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9:188–8. doi:10.1038/s41398-019-0514-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS. Depression and cerebrovascular disease: What is to be done? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;25:129–30. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2016.11.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexopoulos, GS, Hoptman, MJ, Yuen, G, Kanellopoulos, D, Seirup, JK, Lim, KO, et al. Functional connectivity in apathy of late-life depression: a preliminary study. J Affect Disord. 2013;149:398405. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Hoptman, MJ, Kanellopoulos, D, Murphy, CF, Lim, KO, Gunning, FM. Functional connectivity in the cognitive control network and the default mode network in late-life depression. J Affect Disord. 2012;139:5665. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaujean, AA, Parker, S, Qiu, X. The relationship between cognitive ability and depression: a longitudinal data analysis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2013;48:1983–92. doi:10.1007/s00127-013-0668-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammar, Å, Sørensen, L, Årdal, G, Oedegaard, KJ, Kroken, R, Roness, A, et al. Enduring cognitive dysfunction in unipolar major depression: a test–retest study using the Stroop paradigm. Scand J Psychol. 2010;51:304–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00765.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, RS, Mendes de Leon, CF, Bennett, DA, Bienias, JL, Evans, DA. Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in a community population of older persons. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:126.Google Scholar
Gulpers, BJA, Oude Voshaar, RC, van Boxtel, MPJ, Verhey, FRJ, Köhler, S. Anxiety as a risk factor for cognitive decline: a 12-year follow-up cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;27:4252. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2018.09.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gulpers, B, Ramakers, I, Hamel, R, Köhler, S, Oude Voshaar, R, Verhey, F. Anxiety as a predictor for cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;24:823–42. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2016.05.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diniz, BS, Butters, MA, Albert, SM, Dew, MA, Reynolds, CF. Late-life depression and risk of vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis of community-based cohort studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;202:329–35. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.112.118307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koster, EHW, De Lissnyder, E, Derakshan, N, De Raedt, R. Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:138–45. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A, Nolen-Hoeksema, S, Schweizer, S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:217–37. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabrera, I, Brugos, D, Montorio, I. Attentional biases in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;71:102207. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, TJ, Vervliet, B, Hermans, D. An integrative review of attention biases and their contribution to treatment for anxiety disorders. Front Psychol. 2015;6:968. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hantke, NC, Gyurak, A, Van Moorleghem, K, Waring, JD, Adamson, MM, O’Hara, R, et al. Disentangling cognition and emotion in older adults: the role of cognitive control and mental health in emotional conflict adaptation. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;32:840–8. doi:10.1002/gps.4535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, PO, Le Bastard, G, Pochon, JB, Levy, R, Allilaire, JF, Dubois, B, et al. Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38:567–76. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG. Executive function and other cognitive deficits are distal risk factors of generalized anxiety disorder 9 years later. Psychol Med. 2018;48:2045–53. doi:10.1017/S0033291717003579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scult, MA, Paulli, AR, Mazure, ES, Moffitt, TE, Hariri, AR, Strauman, TJ. The association between cognitive function and subsequent depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2017;47:1–17. doi:10.1017/S0033291716002075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molenaar, PCM, Huizenga, HM, Nesselroade, JR. The relationship between the structure of interindividual and intraindividual variability: a theoretical and empirical vindication of developmental systems theory. In: Staudinger, UM, Lindenberger, U, editors. Understanding human development: dialogues with lifespan psychology. Boston, MA: Springer US; 2003, p. 339–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, L, Clifford, A, Wei, L, Zhang, D, Leung, D, Augustine, G, et al. Prevalence and determinants of undetected dementia in the community: a systematic literature review and a meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e011146 doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y-T, Brayne, C, Matthews, FE. Prevalence of dementia in East Asia: a synthetic review of time trends. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;30:793801. doi:10.1002/gps.4297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, BE, Edmondson, D, Kronish, IM. State of the art review: depression, stress, anxiety, and cardiovascular disease. Am J Hypertens. 2015;28:1295–302. doi:10.1093/ajh/hpv047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gustavson, DE, Panizzon, MS, Elman, JA, Franz, CE, Reynolds, CA, Jacobson, KC, et al. Stability of genetic and environmental influences on executive functions in midlife. Psychol Aging. 2018;33:219–31. doi:10.1037/pag0000230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, T, Schmukle, SC, Kunzmann, U. Stability and change in subjective well-being: the role of performance-based and self-rated cognition. Psychol Aging. 2017;32:105–17. doi:10.1037/pag0000153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, JK, Gross, AL, Pa, J, McLaren, DG, Park, LQ, Manly, JJ, et al. Longitudinal change in neuropsychological performance using latent growth models: a study of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6:540–50. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9161-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, BJ, Dixon, RA, McArdle, JJ. Tracking cognition–health changes from 55 to 95 years of age. J Gerontol B. 2011;66B:i153–61. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, D, Yang, PJ, Geldhof, GJ, Tyler, CP, Gansert, PK, Chase, PA, et al. Exploring idiographic approaches to children’s executive function performance: an intensive longitudinal study. J Pers Oriented Res. 2020;6:7387. doi:10.17505/jpor.2020.22401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hülür, G, Ram, N, Willis, SL, Schaie, KW, Gerstorf, D. Cognitive aging in the Seattle longitudinal study: within-person associations of primary mental abilities with psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility. J Intelligence. 2016;4:12. doi:10.3390/jintelligence4030012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kievit, RA, Brandmaier, AM, Ziegler, G, van Harmelen, A-L, de Mooij, SMM, Moutoussis, M, et al. Developmental cognitive neuroscience using latent change score models: a tutorial and applications. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;33:99117. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sliwinski, MJ, Mogle, JA, Hyun, J, Munoz, E, Smyth, JM, Lipton, RB. Reliability and validity of ambulatory cognitive assessments. Assessment. 2018;25:1430. doi:10.1177/1073191116643164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG. Within-person increase in pathological worry predicts future depletion of unique executive functioning domains. Psychol Med. 2020;111. doi:10.1017/S0033291720000422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brose, A, Schmiedek, F, Lövdén, M, Lindenberger, U. Daily variability in working memory is coupled with negative affect: the role of attention and motivation. Emotion. 2012;12:605–17. doi:10.1037/a0024436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allerhand, M, Gale, CR, Deary, IJ. The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and positive well-being in older people: a prospective study using the English longitudinal study of aging. Psychol Aging. 2014;29:306–18. doi:10.1037/a0036551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenze, EJ, Nicol, GE, Barbour, DL, Kannampallil, T, Wong, AWK, Piccirillo, J, et al. Precision clinical trials: a framework for getting to precision medicine for neurobehavioural disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2021;46:e97e110. doi:10.1503/jpn.200042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marzano, L, Bardill, A, Fields, B, Herd, K, Veale, D, Grey, N, et al. The application of mHealth to mental health: opportunities and challenges. Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2:942–8. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00268-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuidersma, M, Riese, H, Snippe, E, Booij, SH, Wichers, M, Bos, EH. Single-subject research in psychiatry: facts and fictions. Front Psych. 2020;11:539777 doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.539777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, RC, Campbell, LM, Delgadillo, JD, Paolillo, EW, Sundermann, EE, Holden, J, et al. Smartphone-based measurement of executive function in older adults with and without HIV. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2020;35:347–57. doi:10.1093/arclin/acz084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, AJ, Medaglia, JD, Jeronimus, BF. Lack of group-to-individual generalizability is a threat to human subjects research. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115:E6106–15. doi:10.1073/pnas.1711978115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG. Larger increase in trait negative affect is associated with greater future cognitive decline and vice versa across 23 years. Depress Anxiety. 2021;38:146–60. doi:10.1002/da.23093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, EL, Kuiper, RM, Grasman, RPPP. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol Methods. 2015;20:102–16. doi:10.1037/a0038889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, KJ, Ram, N. Latent growth and dynamic structural equation models. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2018;14:5589. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mund, M, Nestler, S. Beyond the cross-lagged panel model: next-generation statistical tools for analyzing interdependencies across the life course. Adv Life Course Res. 2019;41:100249. doi:10.1016/j.alcr.2018.10.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, Y, Greenberg, MT, Willoughby, MT, Vernon-Feagans, L, Greenberg, MT, Blair, CB, et al. Examining longitudinal associations between externalizing and internalizing behavior problems at within- and between-child levels. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48:467–80. doi:10.1007/s10802-019-00614-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widaman, KF, Ferrer, E, Conger, RD. Factorial invariance within longitudinal structural equation models: measuring the same construct across time. Child Dev Perspect. 2010;4:10–8. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00110.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steven, GH, Gwenith, GF, Michael, DH, Kenneth, ML, Mary Beth, O, Brenda, LP, et al. Aging, demographics and memory study (ADAMS): sample design, weighting and analysis for ADAMS. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan; 2009.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatry Press; 1994.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.Google Scholar
Langa, KM, Plassman, BL, Wallace, RB, Herzog, AR, Heeringa, SG, Ofstedal, MB, et al. The aging, demographics, and memory study (ADAMS): study design and methods. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25:181–91. doi:10.1159/000087448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, JL, Mega, M, Gray, K, Rosenberg-Thompson, S, Carusi, DA, Gornbein, J. The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–14. doi:10.1212/wnl.44.12.2308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, KM, Stewart, AL, Langa, KM, Yaffe, K, Moody-Ayers, S, Williams, BA, et al. “Below average” self-assessed school performance and Alzheimer’s disease in the aging, demographics, and memory study. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5:380–7. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2009.07.039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, GG, Franks, MM, Plassman, BL, Brown, SL, Potter, GG, Llewellyn, D, et al. Caring for individuals with dementia and cognitive impairment, not dementia: findings from the aging, demographics, and memory study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:488–94. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03304.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffens, DC, Fisher, GG, Langa, KM, Potter, GG, Plassman, BL. Prevalence of depression among older Americans: the aging, demographics and memory study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21:879–88. doi:10.1017/S1041610209990044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Medeiros, K, Robert, P, Gauthier, S, Stella, F, Politis, A, Leoutsakos, J, et al. The neuropsychiatric inventory-clinician rating scale (NPI-C): reliability and validity of a revised assessment of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22:984–94. doi:10.1017/S1041610210000876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkowski, JG, Benton, AL, Spreen, O. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1967;5:135–40. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(67)90015-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardila, A, Ostrosky‐Solís, F, Bernal, B. Cognitive testing toward the future: the example of semantic verbal fluency (ANIMALS). Int J Psychol. 2006;41:324–32. doi:10.1080/00207590500345542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manning, RT. The serial sevens test. Arch Intern Med. 1982;142:1192. doi:10.1001/archinte.1982.00340190148022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III (WAIS- III) manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.Google Scholar
Smith, A. Symbol digit modalities test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1982.Google Scholar
Oliveira, RM, Mograbi, DC, Gabrig, IA, Charchat-Fichman, H. Normative data and evidence of validity for the Rey auditory verbal learning test, verbal fluency test, and Stroop test with Brazilian children. Psychol Neurosci. 2016;9:5467. doi:10.1037/pne0000041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, SM, Buschkuehl, M, Perrig, WJ, Meier, B. The concurrent validity of the N-back task as a working memory measure. Memory. 2010;18:394412. doi:10.1080/09658211003702171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton-Bayre, A, Geffen, G. Comparability, reliability, and practice effects on alternate forms of the digit symbol substitution and symbol digit modalities tests. Psychol Assess. 2005;17:237–41. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.17.2.237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fong, TG, Fearing, MA, Jones, RN, Shi, P, Marcantonio, ER, Rudolph, JL, et al. Telephone interview for cognitive status: creating a crosswalk with the mini-mental state examination. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5:492–7. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2009.02.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, AR, Wallace, RB. Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997;52B:3748. doi:10.1093/geronb/52B.Special_Issue.37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plassman, BL, Newman, TT, Welsh, KA, Helms, M, JCS, B. Properties of the telephone interview for cognitive status: application in epidemiological and longitudinal studies. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1994;7:235–41.Google Scholar
Welsh, KA, Breitner, JC, Magruder-Habib, KM. Detection of dementia in the elderly using telephone screening of cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1993;6:103–10.Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:136. doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:238–46. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiger, JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: an interval estimation approach. Multivar Behav Res. 1990;25:173–80. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, JW. Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:549–76. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, JJ. Longitudinal dynamic analyses of cognition in the health and retirement study panel. Adv Stat Anal. 2011;95:453–80. doi:10.1007/s10182-011-0168-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, GW, Rensvold, RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2002;9:233–55. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14:464504. doi:10.1080/10705510701301834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, JD, Hamaker, EL. Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Struct Equ Modeling. 2020;111. doi:10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738.Google Scholar
Falkenström, F, Finkel, S, Sandell, R, Rubel, JA, Holmqvist, R. Dynamic models of individual change in psychotherapy process research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2017;85:537–49. doi:10.1037/ccp0000203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Usami, S. On the differences between general cross-lagged panel model and random-intercept cross-lagged panel model: interpretation of cross-lagged parameters and model choice. Struct Equ Modeling. 2021;28:331–44. doi:10.1080/10705511.2020.1821690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArdle, JJ. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:577605. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunlap, WP, Cortina, JM, Vaslow, JB, Burke, MJ. Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychol Methods. 1996;1:170–7. doi:10.1037/1082-989x.1.2.170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arend, MG, Schafer, T. Statistical power in two-level models: a tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychol Methods. 2019;24:119. doi:10.1037/met0000195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Z, Hamagami, F, Grimm, KJ, McArdle, JJ. Using R package RAMpath for tracing SEM path diagrams and conducting complex longitudinal data analysis. Struct Equ Modeling. 2015;22:132–47. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.935257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christopher, G, MacDonald, J. The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10:379–99. doi:10.1080/13546800444000128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukka, EJ, Dykiert, D, Allerhand, M, Starr, JM, Deary, IJ. Effects of between-person differences and within-person changes in symptoms of anxiety and depression on older age cognitive performance. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1350–8. doi:10.1017/S0033291717002896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierman, EJM, Comijs, HC, Jonker, C, Beekman, ATF. Effects of anxiety versus depression on cognition in later life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:686–93. doi:10.1097/00019442-200508000-00007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierman, EJ, Comijs, HC, Rijmen, F, Jonker, C, Beekman, AT. Anxiety symptoms and cognitive performance in later life: results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Aging Ment Health. 2008;12:517–23. doi:10.1080/13607860802224276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sari, BA, Koster, EHW, Derakshan, N. The effects of active worrying on working memory capacity. Cogn Emot. 2017;31:9951003. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1170668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherell, JL, Gatz, M, Pedersen, NL. A longitudinal analysis of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Psychol Aging. 2001;16:187–95. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, LO, Gatz, M, Pedersen, NL, Prescott, CA. Anxiety trajectories in the second half of life: genetic and environmental contributions over age. Psychol Aging. 2016;31:101–13. doi:10.1037/pag0000063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, WD, McQuoid, DR, Payne, ME, Zannas, AS, MacFall, JR, Steffens, DC. Hippocampus atrophy and the longitudinal course of late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;22:1504–12. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2013.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbejjani, M, Fuhrer, R, Abrahamowicz, M, Mazoyer, B, Crivello, F, Tzourio, C, et al. Depression, depressive symptoms, and rate of hippocampal atrophy in a longitudinal cohort of older men and women. Psychol Med. 2015;45:1931–44. doi:10.1017/S0033291714003055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG. Depression and worry symptoms predict future executive functioning impairment via inflammation. Psychol Med. 2021;111. doi:10.1017/s0033291721000398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebedeva, A, Sundström, A, Lindgren, L, Stomby, A, Aarsland, D, Westman, E, et al. Longitudinal relationships among depressive symptoms, cortisol, and brain atrophy in the neocortex and the hippocampus. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2018;137:491502. doi:10.1111/acps.12860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, MA, Klunk, WE, Mathis, CA, Price, JC, Ziolko, SK, Hoge, JA, et al. Imaging Alzheimer pathology in late-life depression with PET and Pittsburgh compound-B. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Dis. 2008;22:261–8. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e31816c92bf.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, MA, Whyte, EM, Nebes, RD, Begley, AE, Dew, MA, Mulsant, BH, et al. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:587–95. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, MA, Sweet, RA, Mulsant, BH, Ilyas Kamboh, M, Pollock, BG, Begley, AE, et al. APOE is associated with age-of-onset, but not cognitive functioning, in late-life depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;18:1075–81. doi:10.1002/gps.1006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J, Zhou, Y, Chen, K, Jing, Y, He, J, Sun, H, et al. Dietary inflammatory index and depression: a meta-analysis. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22:654–60. doi:10.1017/S1368980018002628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alvaro, PK, Roberts, RM, Harris, JK. A systematic review assessing bidirectionality between sleep disturbances, anxiety, and depression. Sleep. 2013;36:1059–68. doi:10.5665/sleep.2810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, X, Norton, J, Carriere, I, Ritchie, K, Chaudieu, I, Ancelin, ML. Risk factors for late-onset generalized anxiety disorder: results from a 12-year prospective cohort (the ESPRIT study). Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e536. doi:10.1038/tp.2015.31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petkus, AJ, Reynolds, CA, Wetherell, JL, Kremen, WS, Gatz, M. Temporal dynamics of cognitive performance and anxiety across older adulthood. Psychol Aging. 2017;32:278–92. doi:10.1037/pag0000164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petkus, AJ, Filoteo, JV, Schiehser, DM, Gomez, ME, Petzinger, G. Worse cognitive performance predicts increased anxiety and depressive symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a bidirectional analysis. Neuropsychology. 2019;33:3546. doi:10.1037/neu0000498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, TD, Kidwell, KM, Nelson, JM, Tomaso, CC, Hankey, M, Espy, KA. Preschool executive control and internalizing symptoms in elementary school. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46:1509–20. doi:10.1007/s10802-017-0395-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kertz, SJ, Belden, AC, Tillman, R, Luby, J. Cognitive control deficits in shifting and inhibition in preschool age children are associated with increased depression and anxiety over 7.5 years of development. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:1185–96. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0101-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D, Hamaker, EL. A primer on two-level dynamic structural equation models for intensive longitudinal data in Mplus. Psychol Methods. 2020;25:610–35. doi:10.1037/met0000250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, MW, Derakshan, N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;50:955–60. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitmer, AJ, Gotlib, IH. An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:1036–61. doi:10.1037/a0030923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckwé, M, Deroost, N, Koster, EHW, De Lissnyder, E, De Raedt, R. Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychol Res. 2014;78:651–60. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y, Cao, S, Shields, GS, Teng, Z, Liu, Y. The relationships between rumination and core executive functions: a meta-analysis. Depress Anxiety. 2017;34:3750. doi:10.1002/da.22539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, TP. Anxiety and working memory capacity: a meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol Bull. 2016;142:831–64. doi:10.1037/bul0000051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, HR, Kaiser, RH, Warren, SL, Heller, W. Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3:301–30. doi:10.1177/2167702614534210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, NP, Miyake, A, Altamirano, LJ, Corley, RP, Young, SE, Rhea, SA, et al. Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal twin study. Dev Psychol. 2016;52:326–40. doi:10.1037/dev0000075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xi, C, Liu, Z, Zhong, M, Yang, H, Peng, W, Yi, J. Impaired set-shifting in drug-naïve patients with borderline personality disorder: an event-related potentials study. J Affect Disord. 2021;280:6471. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG, Hong, RY. Cross-cultural and gender invariance of transdiagnostic processes in the United States and Singapore. Assessment. 2021;28:485502. doi:10.1177/1073191119869832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, KM, Porter, RJ. Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychological function in major depression. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009;43:1105–17. doi:10.3109/00048670903279887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohlman, J. Does executive dysfunction affect treatment outcome in late-life mood and anxiety disorders? J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2005;18:97108. doi:10.1177/0891988705276061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F-T, Etnier, JL, Chan, K-H, Chiu, P-K, Hung, T-M, Chang, Y-K. Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2020;50:1451–67. doi:10.1007/s40279-020-01292-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Raue, PJ, Banerjee, S, Marino, P, Renn, BN, Solomonov, N, et al. Comparing the streamlined psychotherapy “Engage” with problem-solving therapy in late-life major depression. A randomized clinical trial. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;110. doi:10.1038/s41380-020-0832-3.Google ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS. Digital mental health for older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020;28:191–3. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2019.06.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Raue, PJ, Gunning, F, Kiosses, DN, Kanellopoulos, D, Pollari, C, et al. “Engage” therapy: behavioral activation and improvement of late-life major depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;24:320–6. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2015.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Wilkins, VM, Marino, P, Kanellopoulos, D, Reding, M, Sirey, JA, et al. Ecosystem focused therapy in poststroke depression: a preliminary study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27:1053–60. doi:10.1002/gps.2822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Raue, PJ, Kiosses, DN, Mackin, RS, Kanellopoulos, D, McCulloch, C, et al. Problem-solving therapy and supportive therapy in older adults with major depression and executive dysfunction: effect on disability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:3341. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zainal, NH, Newman, MG. Mindfulness enhances cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis of 100 randomized controlled trials. 2021; 1165. doi:10.31234/osf.io/vzxw7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karbach, J, Verhaeghen, P. Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:2027–37. doi:10.1177/0956797614548725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastos, AG, Pinto Guimarães, LS, Trentini, CM. Neurocognitive changes in depressed patients in psychodynamic psychotherapy, therapy with fluoxetine and combination therapy. J Affect Disord. 2013;151:1066–75. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.08.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model of DEP and EF across four time-points.

Figure 1

Table 2. Bivariate dual latent change score model of DEP and EF across four time-points.

Figure 2

Figure 1. Random-Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Models Between EF and Depression Severity.Note. **p < .01; ***p < .001. Δ = within-person change in construct from a time-lag to the next adjacent time-lag; DEP = depression severity; EF = executive functioning.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Bivariate Dual Latent Change Score Models Between EF and Depression Severity.Note. **p < .01; ***p < .001. Δ = within-person change in construct from a time-lag to the next adjacent time-lag; DEP = depression severity; EF = executive functioning.

Figure 4

Table 3. Random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model of ANX and EF across four time-points.

Figure 5

Table 4. Bivariate dual latent change score model of ANX and EF across four time-points.

Supplementary material: File

Zainal and Newman supplementary material

Zainal and Newman supplementary material

Download Zainal and Newman supplementary material(File)
File 181.8 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.