Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T21:33:43.495Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comparative clinical trials in psychotherapy: Have large effects been replicated?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2020

Nickolas D. Frost*
Affiliation:
Department of Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA
Thomas W. Baskin
Affiliation:
University of California, College of Biological Sciences, Davis, CA, USA
Bruce E. Wampold
Affiliation:
Department of Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA Modum Bad Psychiatric Center, Vikersund, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Nickolas D. Frost, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

The purpose of this review is to examine the replication attempts of psychotherapy clinical trials for depression and anxiety. We focus specifically on replications of trials that exhibit large differences between psychotherapies. The replicability of these trials is especially important for meta-analysis, where the inclusion of false-positive trials can lead to erroneous conclusions about treatment efficacy.

Methods

Standard replication criteria were developed to distinguish direct from conceptual replication methodologies. Next, an exhaustive literature search was conducted for published meta-analyses of psychotherapy comparisons. Trials that exhibited large effects (d > 0.8) were culled from these meta-analyses. For each trial, a cited replication was conducted to determine if the trial had been subsequently replicated by either ‘direct’ or ‘conceptual’ methods. Finally, a broader search was conducted to examine the extent of replication efforts in the psychotherapy literature overall.

Results

In the meta-analytic search, a total of N = 10 meta-analyses met the inclusion criteria. From these meta-analyses, N = 12 distinct trials exhibited large effect sizes. The meta-analyses containing more than two large effect trials reported evidence for treatment superiority. A cited replication search yielded no direct replication attempts (N = 0) for the trials with large effects, and N = 4 conceptual replication attempts of average or above average quality. However, of these four attempts, only two partially corroborated the results from their original trial.

Conclusion

Meta-analytic reviews are influenced by trials with large effects, and it is not uncommon for these reviews to contain several such trials. Since we find no evidence that trials with such large effects are directly replicable, treatment superiority conclusions from these reviews are highly questionable. To enhance the quality of clinical science, the development of authoritative replication criteria for clinical trials is needed. Moreover, quality benchmarks should be considered before trials are included in a meta-analysis, or replications are attempted.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

There are few concepts more vital to the integrity of a scientific discipline than replication. Put simply, replication involves re-testing a hypothesis to corroborate a scientific result (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt2009). Replication functions as the final arbiter of scientific knowledge – forcing scientists to refine (or discard) flawed theories that cannot precisely predict the outcome of successive experiments (Francis, Reference Francis2012).

Replication is especially important in clinical sciences, where failure to reproduce scientific results can lead to the dissemination of ineffective clinical practices (Prasad et al., Reference Prasad, Vandross, Toomey, Cheung, Rho, Quinn, Chacko, Borkar, Gall, Selvaraj, Ho and Cifu2013). In both medicine and psychiatry, reproducibility in clinical science has been investigated (Ioannidis, Reference Ioannidis2005; Tajika et al., Reference Tajika, Ogawa, Takeshima, Hayasaka and Furukawa2015) but unfortunately, not in psychotherapy research (Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Brandes, King and Markon2019).

Based on thousands of clinical trials and hundreds of reviews of those trials, it is incontrovertible that psychotherapy is an effective intervention across a wide range of mental health problems (Lambert, Reference Lambert2013; Wampold and Imel, Reference Wampold and Imel2015; Munder et al., Reference Munder, Fluckiger, Leichsenring, Abbass, Hilsenroth, Luyten, Rabung, Steinert and Wampold2019; Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Karyotaki, Reijnders and Ebert2019a), and replications to address the question of absolute efficacy would merely provide redundant information. However, there are ongoing questions about the superiority of particular psychotherapies (Wampold, Reference Wampold, Norcross, Beutler and Levant2005; Tolin, Reference Tolin2014; Wampold et al., Reference Wampold, Flückiger, Del Re, Yulish, Frost, Pace, Goldberg, Miller, Baardseth, Laska and Hilsenroth2017; Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Reijnders and Huibers2019b).

There is little scientific consensus about what constitutes a meaningful superiority result in psychotherapy research. Occasionally, however, a single psychotherapy comparison or a few of these comparisons produce large effects (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). These trials can influence meta-analytic conclusions regarding treatment superiority, and in turn, treatment guidelines (Cottraux et al., Reference Cottraux, Note, Albuisson, Yao, Note, Mollard, Bonasse, Jalenques, Guérin and Coudert2000; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Ehlers, Hackmann, McManus, Fennell, Grey, Waddington and Wild2006; Wampold et al., Reference Wampold, Flückiger, Del Re, Yulish, Frost, Pace, Goldberg, Miller, Baardseth, Laska and Hilsenroth2017). The replicability of these trials is important because large effects might well be false-positive results.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the replications of psychotherapy trials demonstrating large treatment differences. We focused on trials for depression and anxiety-related disorders because they are among the most prevalent (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005a, Reference Kessler, Demler, Frank, Olfson, Pincus, Walters, Wang, Wells and Zaslavsky2005b), and many psychotherapies for these disorders are classified as having strong empirical support (see https://www.div12.org/psychological-treatments/frequently-asked-questions/).

Ingredients of replication

Schmidt (Reference Schmidt2009) distinguished two broad categories of scientific replication: direct and conceptual. Generally, a direct replication involves procedural duplication of an earlier experiment, whereas a conceptual replication involves testing the central hypothesis of the earlier study through alternative experimental arrangements (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt2009). As the ‘gold standard’ of experimental designs, randomised clinical trials (RCTs) are well suited for direct or conceptual replication, but a direct replication offers the strongest corroboratory evidence because it most closely resembles the original trial (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt2009). Alternatively, conceptual replication may well elucidate other aspects of the phenomenon, such as generalizability.

The experimental logic of a clinical trial is standard across scientific disciplines. In the case of psychotherapy comparisons, a sample of patients suffering from a particular disorder are recruited, randomly assigned to two or more treatment conditions, receive the respective treatments, and are evaluated at termination to compare outcomes between treatment groups (Heppner et al., Reference Heppner, Kivlighan and Wampold2008). Better outcomes for the patients in one treatment condition vis-à-vis the outcomes for the other treatment, beyond what is expected by chance, are evidence for the superiority of that treatment (Heppner et al., Reference Heppner, Kivlighan and Wampold2008).

Despite this sound logic, inferences regarding treatment superiority (i.e. Treatment A is superior to B) in psychotherapy trials are more complicated. Blinding is not possible in psychotherapy, and researcher allegiance to a particular treatment can influence outcomes in direct comparisons (McLeod, Reference McLeod2009; Munder et al., Reference Munder, Gerger, Trelle and Barth2011, Reference Munder, Brütsch, Leonhart, Gerger and Barth2013). Moreover, unlike medicine, psychotherapies cannot be completely standardised across patients or administrations. In psychotherapy, the therapist delivering the treatment, regardless of the degree to which the treatment is standardised, can make a difference; that is, therapists are not interchangeable (Baldwin and Imel, Reference Baldwin, Imel and Lambert2013). Consequently, therapist characteristics (training, experience, etc.) and representativeness should be considerations for replication.

Despite these complexities, enough methodological features can be controlled to discriminate direct and conceptual replication of a psychotherapy trial. In a direct replication, nearly all trial features must be the same between the original trial and replication attempt (denoted by ‘ + ’ in Table 1). These features broadly include duplication of treatment delivery, therapist expertise, characteristics of the patient sample and outcome assessment.

Table 1. Direct replication criteria

‘+’ = This is a feature that must remain the same in the primary trial and replication; ‘−’ = this is a feature that can be different in the replication.

Some trial features, however, can be altered so that clinical trial could qualify as a conceptual replication when the researcher is interested in examining parametric effects or generalizability across contexts. The number of psychotherapy sessions delivered (treatment dose), format of treatment (i.e. individual or group; see Table 1) and context (e.g. university speciality clinic v. community clinic) are examples of features that may vary between the primary trial and replication attempt in these situations. A characteristic of a conceptual replication is that specific features of the original study are intentionally varied to examine a particular conjecture. A case could be made, however, that to accept any superiority result as an established scientific finding, at least one direct replication is required (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt2009).

Method

This replication investigation employed the following steps. First, meta-analyses of comparative psychotherapy treatments for depression and anxiety were identified. Second, from those meta-analyses, trials with large effects (d > 0.8) were culled from the meta-analyses. Third, all citations to the identified trials were examined to determine whether they qualified as direct or conceptual replications according to the components listed in Table 1. These processes are different from canonical reviews and therefore various standards (e.g. PRISMA) were not applicable. Although these procedures were determined a priori, the research was not registered.

Identification of meta-analyses

Meta-analyses were only included in this study if they met the following criteria: (a) they included RCTs that directly compared at least two active psychosocial treatments, (b) patients were adults who suffered from a diagnosable anxiety or depressive psychiatric condition as defined by any edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders or other accepted diagnostic nosologies, and (c) they were published in a peer-reviewed journal.

The search for meta-analytic reviews included an exhaustive search of the database PsycINFO using the search engine ProQuest by two of the authors (BLIND). Primary search terms included ‘Depression’, ‘Anxiety’ and ‘Meta-analysis’. Filtering options for adult population and peer-reviewed journal were applied to limit search results. Potential meta-analyses were evaluated by all the authors to determine whether they met the eligibility criteria until full consensus was reached.

Large effect trials

A clinical trial database was developed that included all the comparative trials contained in the meta-analyses. Some meta-analyses included only comparative trials; for other meta-analyses, comparative trials formed a subset of trials. In the latter situation, only the comparative trials were included in this study. Some trials were contained in more than one meta-analysis and these duplicate trials were noted and then excluded so that a trial only appeared once in the trial database.

Effect size conventions (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988) stipulate that small effect sizes include those in the range of 0.2, medium effect sizes are in the range of 0.5 and large effects are in the range of 0.8. To identify the largest effect trials from our database, we used a cut-off value of d = 0.80. Trials that exhibited absolute effect sizes equal to or greater than this cut-off were included in a new dataset that only contained large effect trials. Only targeted outcomes that measured diagnosis-specific symptomology were considered in this investigation, as reported in the published meta-analysis or supplied from the meta-analytic authors upon our request in two cases (Tolin, Reference Tolin2010; Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Ebert, Acarturk, Andersson and Cristea2016).

Finally, trials were excluded that were not ‘bona-fide’ psychotherapy comparisons (Wampold et al., Reference Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, Benson and Ahn1997). The bona-fide criteria were interpreted liberally; we only eliminated comparisons to treatment-as-usual conditions and comparisons to ‘non-active’ control conditions, as identified by the trials' authors. The large effect trials were graded on their overall methodological quality by two independent raters (two of the authors) using the Psychotherapy Quality Rating Scale (PQRS) (Kocsis et al., Reference Kocsis, Gerber, Milrod, Roose, Barber, Thase, Perkins and Leon2010). This scale was developed for RCTs of psychotherapy and has demonstrated good internal consistency in validation studies (α = 0.87). The scale includes 24 items that range from 0 to 2, examining six trial quality domains: definition and delivery of treatment, patient description, outcome assessment, appropriateness of data analysis, treatment assignment and overall quality. The final item ranges from 1 to 7, where a score of 1 indicates an exceptionally poor trial and 7 indicates an exceptionally good trial. Thus, the PQRS ranges from 1 to 55, with higher scores indicating a higher quality trial.

Replication searches

The first step in locating replications was based on the premise that a replication would cite the trial it was replicating. Accordingly, we used the Web of Science to search all citations of each large effect trial in our database. The number of citations for each trial is shown in Table 3. Presumably, a replication would use the word replication in the title or abstract, which we coded. However, partly because only one citation to any of the trials used the term replication in this way, we did not limit the identification of replications to this standard. Rather, each study was coded by two independent raters (two authors) where we applied the criteria in Table 1 to determine if each citation was a replication of a previous trial. If either rater indicated that a trial was a possible replication, we included that trial in our results.

After completing this process, we were concerned that we may have missed replication attempts of trials in our database. Thus, in a post-hoc search, we used two key words (Psychotherapy, Replication) to search the Web of Science and PsycINFO databases for any replication attempt of a psychotherapy clinical trial as identified by its title. Results were filtered only by ‘clinical trial methodology’ from the list of methodologies. An advantage of this secondary search was that it allowed us to assess the extent of replication efforts in the psychotherapy literature writ large.

Results

Meta-analyses

Search results for the meta-analyses returned 648 ‘hits’ from PsycINFO. In total, 638 meta-analyses were excluded for the lack of direct comparisons, the inclusion of non-RCT research designs or the lack of patient samples with a defined disorder (see Fig. 1). Ultimately, ten meta-analyses met all inclusion criteria and composed the meta-analysis database (Table 2).

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the meta-analysis selection process.

Table 2. Meta-analyses of direct treatment comparisons

CBT, cognitive behavioural therapy; PDT, psychodynamic therapy; NDST, non-directive supportive therapy; IPT, interpersonal therapy; EDT, experiential dynamic therapy.

Note: all effect sizes are reported for targeted symptom measures.

Of the ten meta-analyses, four examined populations suffering from depression, five focused on both depression and anxiety-related disorders and one examined anxiety. Aggregate effect size estimates ranged from 0.02 to 0.46 in these meta-analyses, and five of the ten reported finding evidence for treatment superiority (see Table 2). An important caveat relates to Tolin's (Reference Tolin2014) meta-analysis which found cognitive-behaviour therapy (CBT) superior to other psychotherapies for anxiety disorders as this conclusion was corrected in a subsequent corrigendum due to a calculation error contained in the earlier analysis (n.b., we used the corrected effect sizes in this study) (Tolin, Reference Tolin2015).

Clinical trials

Clinical trials drawn from these meta-analyses spanned the years 1972–2016, totalling 137 unique trials with 157 treatment comparisons – involving CBT, dynamic therapies, behavioural activation, cognitive therapy (CT), interpersonal therapy and emotion-focused therapies. Effect sizes from all trials ranged from d = 0.0 to d = 1.56 on targeted outcomes, with a mean average effect of d = 0.41 (s.d. = 0.32). Approximately 57% (k = 88) of the comparisons yielded small-to-medium effects, 34% (k = 56) yielded medium-to-large effects, and 9% (k = 14) of the comparisons exhibited large effects greater than or equal to the d = 0.8 cut-off used in this study. These large effect sizes came from 12 distinct clinical trials (see Table 3). It is important to note that five of the large effect trails appeared in more than one of our meta-analyses.

Table 3. Clinical trials with the largest effect sizes

CBT, cognitive behaviour therapy; CT, cognitive therapy; DYN, dynamic therapy; REM, reminiscence psychotherapy; EFT, emotion-focused therapy; TRN, transdiagnostic therapy; SUP, supportive therapy; BAT, behavioural activation therapy; RLX, relaxation therapy; GFT, goal-focused therapy; CT, cognitive therapy; AR, applied relaxation, quality ratings were averaged between two independent raters; GAD, generalised anxiety disorder; PD, panic disorder.

a Manual not available.

*Trial unavailable in English. Quality = item 25 from the PQRS.

Large effect trials

Treatment comparisons from the large effect trials included CBT, interpersonal therapy (IPT), psychodynamic psychotherapies (DYN), transdiagnostic therapies (TRN), manualised supportive psychotherapies (SUP) and behavioural activation (BAT). The average number of patients per treatment condition was 21.8 and the average number of psychotherapy sessions delivered for each treatment condition was 13.6. There were no systematic differences between superior and inferior treatments in terms of sample size or treatment dose in these trials. Effect sizes at post-treatment on targeted outcomes (e.g. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) for depression) ranged from d = 0.8 to d = 1.56, with a mean effect of d = 1.1 (s.d. = 0.25).

The index of rater agreement for trial quality ratings was good (ICC = 0.83) between the two raters (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009), and the final PQRS scores were the average of the two (Table 3). The ratings on the PQRS ranged from 16 to 43 out of the possible score of 55. No large effect trials were rated as exceptionally good but two were rated as very good; over half were rated as exceptionally poor, very poor or moderately poor. Treatment comparisons in the lowest quality trials found CBT superior to behavioural activation (d = 0.89) for depressed adults (Taylor and Marshall, Reference Taylor and Marshall1977); CT superior to behaviour therapy and supportive therapy (d = 1.13; d = 0.90, respectively) for depressed college students (Shaw, Reference Shaw1977); and goal-focused group psychotherapy superior to group reminiscence therapy (d = 1.14) for late-life depressed adults (Klausner et al., Reference Klausner, Clarkin, Spielman, Pupo, Abrams and Alexopoulos1998). The highest quality trials found dynamic therapy superior to applied relaxation (d = 0.89) for adults with panic disorder (Milrod et al., Reference Milrod, Leon, Busch, Rudden, Schwalberg, Clarkin, Aronson, Singer, Turchin, Klass, Graf, Teres and Shear2007); and CBT superior to dynamic therapy (d = 1.44) (Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994). Trial characteristics that attenuated quality ratings included poor definition or execution of treatment delivery, and unclear description of patients and diagnostic classification.

Replication search results

The large effect trials were cited 1035 times in the Web of Science (see Table 3), with an average number of 85 citations per trial. The least cited trial was (Taylor and Marshall, Reference Taylor and Marshall1977) not cited at all in the Web of Science and the most (Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994) was cited 268 times. Of all the recorded citations, 126 (12%) were clinical trials.

Applying the criteria from Table 1 to each citing clinical trial, there were no trials that could be deemed a direct replication attempt in terms of their methodology. That is, no trials methodologically duplicated an original trial by comparing the same treatments (dose, format, manual), on the same population (diagnosis, patient characteristics), with therapists of the same training and experience, using an approximately similar measure of clinical outcome. In fact, only 15 of all citing trials screened included at least one general treatment from the original trial in an active comparison (Table 3). The absence of identical treatment comparisons made these trials only eligible as conceptual replication attempts (see online Supplementary Table S4).

From the 15 trials that could possibly be considered conceptual replications, treatment format and dose varied from the influential trial, which is acceptable in a conceptual replication whose purpose is generalizability. However, only six of these possible replications (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Goldin and Charbonneau-Powis1983; Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996; Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Shapiro, Hardy and Rees1999; Mohr et al., Reference Mohr, Boudewyn, Goodkin, Bostrom and Epstein2001; Arntz, Reference Arntz2003; Gallagher-Thompson et al., Reference Gallagher-Thompson, Coon, Solano, Ambler, Rabinowitz and Thompson2003) used the same treatment manual as the original trial. The number of potential replication attempts was further reduced, because only four of these trials included an approximately similar diagnostic population of patients (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Goldin and Charbonneau-Powis1983; Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996; Mohr et al., Reference Mohr, Boudewyn, Goodkin, Bostrom and Epstein2001; Arntz, Reference Arntz2003). From the remaining four trials, only one of them was explicitly identified as a replication attempt by its authors (Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996). Characteristics of the replication attempts can be seen in online Supplementary Table S4.

Are any of these trials ‘successful replication attempts? Conclusions from two of the four quasi-valid conceptual replication attempts loosely corroborated the original trial. Mohr et al. (Reference Mohr, Boudewyn, Goodkin, Bostrom and Epstein2001) compared 16 weeks of CBT to dynamic therapy and found a significant difference at post-treatment on the BDI in favour of CBT for depressed patients with multiple sclerosis. But this effect was notably smaller (d = 0.55 v. d = 1.27) than the post-treatment difference found in the original trial. Barkham et al. (Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996) intentionally replicated Shapiro et al.'s (Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994) trial and found general equivalence between CBT and dynamic therapy at post-treatment; however, significant differences favouring CBT observed on the BDI (d = 1.44)Footnote 1 from the original trial were not replicated. The remaining two replication attempts reached divergent conclusions than the original trial they cited. For example, in direct contrast to Durham et al. (Reference Durham, Murphy, Allan, Richard, Treliving and Fenton1994), Arntz (Reference Arntz2003) found applied relaxation and CT to be equivalent after 12 sessions of treatment on composite measures of a generalized anxiety disorder (d = 0.14). Finally, Wilson et al. (Reference Wilson, Goldin and Charbonneau-Powis1983) found small differences (d = 0.25) between CT and behavioural treatment following eight sessions of counselling, whereas Shaw (Reference Shaw1977) found CT superior to behaviour therapy (d = 1.13). As a whole, these conceptual replications produced weaker or contradictory results compared to the original trial they cited.

Results from the general replication search resulted in 38 hits from our search terms Replication and Psychotherapy. Examining each hit revealed no direct or conceptual replication attempts of a psychotherapy comparison for adult depression or anxiety. More specifically, six studies were excluded because they examined a child population, ten did not include direct psychotherapy comparisons, and four were not clinical trials at all. The remaining 18 studies were either narrative reviews, comparisons to treatment-as-usual conditions or waitlists. One study was identified as a replication attempt, but not of a comparative psychotherapy result (Chambless et al., Reference Chambless, Allred, Chen, McCarthy, Milrod and Barber2017).

Discussion

We examined replication attempts of trials that exhibited large effects in published meta-analyses. The essential finding is that replications are scarce: There were no direct replication attempts, and only a few possible conceptual replication attempts, many of which failed to corroborate the conclusions of the original trial. Several large effect trials had poor quality ratings. However, even for the two trials rated as ‘very good’ quality (Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994; Milrod et al., Reference Milrod, Leon, Busch, Rudden, Schwalberg, Clarkin, Aronson, Singer, Turchin, Klass, Graf, Teres and Shear2007), there were no conceptual replication attempts for one of these trials (Milrod et al., Reference Milrod, Leon, Busch, Rudden, Schwalberg, Clarkin, Aronson, Singer, Turchin, Klass, Graf, Teres and Shear2007). For the other ‘very good’ trial (Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994), there were two possible conceptual replication attempts. Unfortunately, one attempt used a sub-clinical population, and the other did not corroborate the original result, particularly the large effect favouring CBT vis-à-vis dynamic therapy (Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996; Barkham et al., Reference Barkham, Shapiro, Hardy and Rees1999).

The non-replication of trials producing large effects is problematic for meta-analytic conclusions. Several such trials appeared in multiple meta-analyses from our database. Meta-analyses with more than two large effect trials reported the strongest evidence for treatment superiority. For example, Tolin (Reference Tolin2010) found CBT superior to other treatments at post-treatment on measures of depression or anxiety (d = 0.22) and this analysis included four trials with large effects. Similarly, Cuijpers et al. (Reference Cuijpers, Ebert, Acarturk, Andersson and Cristea2016) reported evidence for the superiority of CBT (d = 0.29) on measures of depression at post-treatment and included four trials with large effects. By contrast, the meta-analyses in our database with only one or no large effect trials reported more modest findings, even for the same class of treatments (e.g. CBT) (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, van Straten and Smit2006, Reference Cuijpers, van Straten, Schuurmans, van Oppen, Hollon and Andersson2010b).

Before we discuss broader implications of these findings, we examine the most valid replication attempt from our results. The original trial known as the Second Sheffield Psychotherapy Project (SPP2), conducted by Shapiro et al. (Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994), compared cognitive-behavioural psychotherapy to psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy with two different durations (eight or 16 sessions) for the treatment of depression. In this study, the patients were treated in a research clinic at the University Sheffield.

In 1996, Barkham et al. (Reference Barkham, Rees, Shapiro, Stiles, Agnew, Halstead, Culverwell and Harrington1996) sought to conceptually replicate the results of SPP2 in an applied setting as opposed to the special arrangements that existed in a speciality research clinic in a university setting, intentionally varying one aspect of the experimental arrangement. Thus, the intention and procedure align with the idea of a conceptual replication (Schmidt, Reference Schmidt2009). The title of the Barkham trial clearly denoted their intention: ‘Outcomes of time-limited psychotherapy in applied settings: Replicating the Second Sheffield Psychotherapy Project’ (emphasis added). Moreover, Barkham et al. explicitly compared the results of the influential study and the replication, as summarised in online Supplementary Table S5. As the only trial that closely hewed to the idea of a conceptual replication in our investigation, it could serve as a model for researchers.

Recent reviews have expressed the need to increase the focus on replication in clinical social sciences (Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Brandes, King and Markon2019). We add to these efforts by highlighting how non-replicated findings influence superiority conclusions in psychotherapy. The trials with large effects in our review tended to have small samples, thus it cannot be ruled out that their effects were due to chance. At the same time, our unrestricted search suggests that replication attempts are rare regardless of trial effect or sample size. A recent investigation (Sakaluk et al., Reference Sakaluk, Williams, Kilshaw and Rhyner2019) supports this conclusion, reporting ‘weak’ evidence for replicability across empirically supported psychotherapies. Unfortunately, a limitation of this study was its classification system, which grouped heterogeneous treatments and treatment comparisons.

Moving forward, replication should be a collective priority in psychotherapy research. A challenge is trial quality. High-quality trials appear to be the exception, not the rule in psychotherapy research (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, van Straten, Bohlmeijer, Hollon and Andersson2010a). The case could be made that methodologically flawed trials should not be replicated. From this perspective, the dearth of replication for poor trials is expected, even desirable. However, if a low-quality trial is unworthy of a replication attempt, should it be admissible as scientific evidence in the first place? Clearly, low-quality trials should not serve as scientific evidence in a meta-analysis, and at the same time be exempt from replication standards. For example, Shaw (Reference Shaw1977) and Shear et al. (Reference Shear, Houck, Greeno and Masters2001) were two of the low-quality trials in our review, yet they appeared in multiple meta-analyses from our database (see Table 3). Unrepresentative therapists (one therapy, Shaw) and an unavailable treatment manual [Shear, personal communication, 7 March 2015] make these trials impossible to replicate.

Aside from trail quality, other challenges exist. Failure to replicate findings is often attributed to the lack of adherence to the treatment protocol (Laska et al., Reference Laska, Gurman and Wampold2014). For example, when advocates of a particular treatment are confronted with a trial that fails to support the treatment, they often claim that the treatment was not given with adequate adherence to the manual (Laska et al., Reference Laska, Gurman and Wampold2014; Wampold and Imel, Reference Wampold and Imel2015), an argument that attributes importance to the precise nature of treatments. According to this view – and one that we adopted for this investigation – the particular manual used to guide treatment delivery in a given trial must also be used in its replication. Some incremental steps towards addressing these challenges include (1) the development of authoritative replication criteria for clinical trials, and (2) stipulating quality benchmarks to aid research decisions to replicate trials.

The results of our investigation should be interpreted with caution. The replication criteria developed in Table 1 are the most logically consistent with the concepts of direct or conceptual replication in science writ large, but these criteria are not universally accepted for psychotherapy trials. In addition, our cut-off threshold (d = 0.80) for large effect trials is arbitrary, but by the same token, any cut-off value would be similarly arbitrary. Nevertheless, the conclusions that there are few replications of psychotherapy comparative studies seem robust (i.e. is not dependent on the cut-off chosen).

In conclusion, attempts to identify the most effective treatment for a particular disorder have not been successfully reproduced among trials showing the staunchest evidence. It seems wise to further investigate the replicability of psychotherapy trials. Especially if we are to have confidence in treatment guidelines premised on the assumption of treatment superiority.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796020000402

Data

Clinical trial and meta-analytic databases for this review can be found at https://data.mendeley.com/drafts/.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

None.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

1 Effect size (d = 1.44) for Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Barkham, Rees, Hardy, Reynolds and Startup1994 was obtained directly from Tolin (Reference Tolin2010) For 16 sessions, moderate depression, BDI at post-treatment. Statistics reported in Shapiro et al., (1994) does not allow verification of this effect.

References

Arntz, A (2003) Cognitive therapy versus applied relaxation as treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy 41, 633646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayen, I and Hautzinger, M (2004) Kognitive Verhaltenstherapie bei Depressionen im Klimakterium: Eine kontrollierte, randomisierte Interventionsstudie. [Cognitive behavior therapy for depression in menopausal women. A controlled, randomized treatment study.]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie: Forschung und Praxis. 33, 290299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baardseth, TP, Goldberg, SB, Pace, BT, Wislocki, AP, Frost, ND, Siddiqui, JR and Wampold, BE (2013) Cognitive-behavioral therapy versus other therapies: Redux. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 395405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, SA and Imel, ZE (2013) Therapist effects: finding and methods. In Lambert, MJ (ed.), Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. New York: Wiley, pp. 258297.Google Scholar
Barkham, M, Rees, A, Shapiro, DA, Stiles, WB, Agnew, RM, Halstead, J, Culverwell, A and Harrington, VM (1996) Outcomes of time-limited psychotherapy in applied settings: replicating the Second Sheffield Psychotherapy Project. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64, 10791085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkham, M, Shapiro, DA, Hardy, GE and Rees, A (1999) Psychotherapy in two-plus-one sessions: outcomes of a randomized controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal therapy for subsyndromal depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 67, 201211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L, Higgins, JPT and Rothstein, H (2009) Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambless, DL, Allred, KM, Chen, FF, McCarthy, KS, Milrod, B and Barber, JP (2017) Perceived criticism predicts outcome of psychotherapy for panic disorder: replication and extension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 85, 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, DM, Ehlers, A, Hackmann, A, McManus, F, Fennell, M, Grey, N, Waddington, L and Wild, J (2006) Cognitive therapy versus exposure and applied relaxation in social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 74, 568578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for The Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cottraux, J, Note, I, Albuisson, E, Yao, SN, Note, B, Mollard, E, Bonasse, F, Jalenques, I, Guérin, J and Coudert, AJ (2000) Cognitive behavior therapy versus supportive therapy in social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 69, 137146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, Driessen, E, Hollon, SD, van Oppen, P, Barth, J and Andersson, G (2012) The efficacy of non-directive supportive therapy for adult depression: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 32, 280291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, van Straten, A and Smit, F (2006) Psychological treatment of late-life depression: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21, 11391149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, van Straten, A, Bohlmeijer, E, Hollon, SD and Andersson, G (2010 a) The effects of psychotherapy for adult depression are overestimated: a meta-analysis of study quality and effect size. Psychological Medicine 40, 211223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, van Straten, A, Schuurmans, J, van Oppen, P, Hollon, SD and Andersson, G (2010 b) Psychotherapy for chronic major depression and dysthymia: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, Ebert, DD, Acarturk, C, Andersson, G and Cristea, IA (2016) Personalized psychotherapy for adult depression: a meta-analytic review. Behavior Therapy 47, 966980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, Karyotaki, E, Reijnders, M and Ebert, DD (2019 a) Was Eysenck right after all? A reassessment of the effects of psychotherapy for adult depression. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 28, 2130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P, Reijnders, M and Huibers, MJH (2019 b) The role of common factors in psychotherapy outcomes. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 15, 207231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durham, RC, Murphy, T, Allan, T, Richard, K, Treliving, LR and Fenton, GW (1994) Cognitive therapy, analytic therapy and anxiety management training for generalised anxiety disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 165, 315323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, G (2012) The psychology of replication and replication in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7, 585594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher-Thompson, D, Coon, DW, Solano, N, Ambler, C, Rabinowitz, Y and Thompson, LW (2003) Change in indices of distress among Latino and Anglo female caregivers of elderly relatives with dementia: site-specific results from the REACH national collaborative study. The Gerontologist 43, 580591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher-Thompson, D and Steffen, AM (1994) Comparative effects of cognitive behavioral and brief psychodynamic psychotherapies for depressed family caregivers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62, 543549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heppner, PP, Kivlighan, DM and Wampold, BE (2008) Research Design In Counseling. Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Ioannidis, JP (2005) Contradicted and initially stronger effects in highly cited clinical research. Journal of the American Medical Association 294, 218228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, R, Treiber, R and Henrich, G (1986) Effectiveness of two psychological treatments for inpatients with severe and chronic depressions. Cognitive Therapy and Research 10, 645663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keefe, JR, McCarthy, KS, Dinger, U, Zilcha-Mano, S and Barber, JP (2014) A meta analytic review of psychodynamic therapies for anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review 34, 309323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Merikangas, KR and Walters, EE (2005 a) Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 593602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Demler, O, Frank, RG, Olfson, M, Pincus, HA, Walters, EE, Wang, P, Wells, KB and Zaslavsky, AM (2005 b) Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders, 1990 to 2003. New England Journal of Medicine 352, 25152523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klausner, EJ, Clarkin, JF, Spielman, L, Pupo, C, Abrams, R and Alexopoulos, GS (1998) Late-life depression and functional disability: the role of goal-focused group psychotherapy. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 13, 707716.3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kocsis, JH, Gerber, AJ, Milrod, B, Roose, SP, Barber, J, Thase, ME, Perkins, P and Leon, AC (2010) A new scale for assessing the quality of randomized clinical trials of psychotherapy. Comprehensive Psychiatry 51, 319324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, MJ ed. (2013) Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Laska, KM, Gurman, AS and Wampold, BE (2014) Expanding the lens of evidence-based practice in psychotherapy: a common factors perspective. Psychotherapy 51, 467481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latour, D and Cappeliez, P (1994) Pretherapy training for group cognitive therapy with depressed older adults. Canadian Journal on Aging 13, 221235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lilliengren, P, Johansson, R, Lindqvist, K, Mechler, J and Andersson, G (2016) Efficacy of experiential dynamic therapy for psychiatric conditions: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychotherapy 53, 90104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, BD (2009) Understanding why therapy allegiance is linked to clinical outcomes. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 16, 6972.Google Scholar
Milrod, B, Leon, AC, Busch, F, Rudden, M, Schwalberg, M, Clarkin, J, Aronson, A, Singer, M, Turchin, W, Klass, ET, Graf, E, Teres, JJ and Shear, MK (2007) A randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 265272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohr, DC, Boudewyn, AC, Goodkin, DE, Bostrom, A and Epstein, L (2001) Comparative outcomes for individual cognitive-behavior therapy, supportive-expressive group psychotherapy, and sertraline for the treatment of depression in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 69, 942949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munder, T, Gerger, H, Trelle, S and Barth, J (2011) Testing the allegiance bias hypothesis: a meta-analysis. Psychotherapy Research 21, 670684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munder, T, Brütsch, O, Leonhart, R, Gerger, H and Barth, J (2013) Researcher allegiance in psychotherapy outcome research: an overview of reviews. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 501511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munder, T, Fluckiger, C, Leichsenring, F, Abbass, AA, Hilsenroth, MJ, Luyten, P, Rabung, S, Steinert, C and Wampold, BE (2019) Is psychotherapy effective? A re-analysis of treatments for depression. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 28, 268274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newby, JM, McKinnon, A, Kuyken, W, Gilbody, S and Dalgleish, T (2015) Systematic review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and depressive disorders in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review 40, 91110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, PJ (2012) A randomized clinical trial of transdiagnostic cognitve-behavioral treatments for anxiety disorder by comparison to relaxation training. Behavior Therapy 43, 506517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prasad, V, Vandross, A, Toomey, C, Cheung, M, Rho, J, Quinn, S, Chacko, SJ, Borkar, D, Gall, V, Selvaraj, S, Ho, N and Cifu, A (2013) A decade of reversal: an analysis of 146 contradicted medical practices. Mayo Clinic Proceedings 88, 790798, 9p.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakaluk, JK, Williams, AJ, Kilshaw, RE and Rhyner, KT (2019) Evaluating the evidential value of empirically supported psychological treatments (ESTs): a meta-scientific review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 128, 500509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, S (2009) Shall we really do it again? The powerful concept of replication is neglected in the social sciences. Review of General Psychology 13, 90100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, DA, Barkham, M, Rees, A, Hardy, GE, Reynolds, S and Startup, M (1994) Effects of treatment duration and severity of depression on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62, 522534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, BF (1977) Comparison of cognitive therapy and behavior therapy in the treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 45, 543551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shear, MK, Houck, P, Greeno, C and Masters, S (2001) Emotion-focused psychotherapy for patients with panic disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 19931998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tackett, JL, Brandes, CM, King, KM and Markon, KE (2019) Psychology's replication crisis and clinical psychological science. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 15, 579604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajika, A, Ogawa, Y, Takeshima, N, Hayasaka, Y and Furukawa, TA (2015) Replication and contradiction of highly cited research papers in psychiatry: 10-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry 207, 357362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, FG and Marshall, WL (1977) Experimental analysis of a cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 1, 5972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolin, DF (2010) Is cognitive–behavioral therapy more effective than other therapies?: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 710720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, DF (2014) Beating a dead dodo bird: looking at signal vs. noise in cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 21, 351362.Google Scholar
Tolin, DF (2015) Corrigendum to ‘Beating a dead dodo bird: looking at signal vs. noise in cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders’. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 22, 315316.Google Scholar
Wampold, BE (2005) Are ESTs more effective than other treatments for particular disorders? Not a scintilla of evidence to support an affirmative answer and the suggestion that ESTs are more effective than other treatments should not be disseminated. In Norcross, JC, Beutler, LE and Levant, RF (eds), Evidence-based Practices in Mental Health: Debate and Dialogue on the Fundamental Questions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 299308.Google Scholar
Wampold, BE and Imel, ZE (2015) The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, BE, Mondin, GW, Moody, M, Stich, F, Benson, K and Ahn, H (1997) A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: empirically, ‘All must have prizes’. Psychological Bulletin 122, 203215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, BE, Flückiger, C, Del Re, AC, Yulish, NE, Frost, ND, Pace, BT, Goldberg, SB, Miller, SD, Baardseth, TP, Laska, KM and Hilsenroth, MJ (2017) In pursuit of truth: a critical examination of meta-analyses of cognitive behavior therapy. Psychotherapy Research 27, 1432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, PH, Goldin, JC and Charbonneau-Powis, M (1983) Comparative efficacy of behavioral and cognitive treatments of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 7, 111124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Direct replication criteria

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the meta-analysis selection process.

Figure 2

Table 2. Meta-analyses of direct treatment comparisons

Figure 3

Table 3. Clinical trials with the largest effect sizes

Supplementary material: File

Frost et al. supplementary material

Frost et al. supplementary material

Download Frost et al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.9 KB