Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T13:32:17.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Benedikt Till
Affiliation:
Medical University of Vienna, Center for Public Health, Institute of Social Medicine, Suicide Research Unit, Vienna, Austria. Email: [email protected]
Thomas Niederkrotenthaler
Affiliation:
Medical University of Vienna, Austria
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015 

Regarding the questionnaires discussed by Drs Jha & Kumar, we want to clarify some statements regarding some of the measures of our study. As they correctly pointed out, the Erlanger Depression Scale Reference Lehrl and Gallwitz1 consists of 9 items, but only 8 of these items are used to calculate the score for depression, and the first item of the scale is a ‘warm-up’ item used for introduction to the scale. Further, the 48-item scale by Linehan and colleagues Reference Linehan, Goodstein, Nielsen and Chiles2 is commonly referred to as the Reasons for Living Inventory, Reference Brown3-Reference Range5 even though earlier versions of this scale may exist.

We agree that factors other than the outcome of the suicidal crisis portrayed in the films (e.g. camera positioning, audio quality, lighting, special effects) might have determined the impact of the movies on the audiences, and this was discussed in our paper. Further studies are necessary to determine the effect of movies that do not differ with regard to characteristics other than the crisis outcome.

Regarding the screening process, ethical considerations and the safety of participants are of course a main priority. Therefore, we excluded individuals with high depression or suicidality scores from participation in the study and offered psychological counselling to them and to all participants after the screening, to help them cope with any distress they may have experienced due to exposure to the films or answering questions on suicidality. The screening process was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Vienna and the Vienna General Hospital (study protocol 942/2011, date 24/11/2011). Of note, there is no evidence of general harmful effects of answering questions on suicidality among depressed patients Reference Smith, Poindexter and Cuckrowicz6 or the general population. Obtaining a detailed clinical history or examining the mental state with screening instruments such as the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, Reference Kessler and Ustün7 as suggested by Jah & Kumar, would have further increased the participants' time spent on completing questionnaires, which may have resulted in negative consequences on participation. It is also important to note that suicidal ideation scores among study participants with baseline suicidality above the median who watched the suicide film were still considerably lower after the film screening than suicidal ideation scores of individuals with a history of suicidal ideation or parasuicide in previous studies (e.g. Linehan et al Reference Linehan, Goodstein, Nielsen and Chiles2 ). We also checked for incoherent responses during the screening process in order to identify potentially unreliable responses. There were no contradictory or inconsistent responses in the questionnaires, and there were no indicators of psychotic illnesses among the participants during briefing and debriefing of the study, which were both conducted by a psychologist (B.T.).

References

1 Lehrl, S, Gallwitz, A. Erlanger Depression Scale (EDS) [in German]. Vless, 1983.Google Scholar
2 Linehan, MM, Goodstein, JL, Nielsen, SL, Chiles, JA. Reasons for staying alive when you are thinking of killing yourself: the Reasons for Living Inventory. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983; 51: 276–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Brown, GK. A Review of Suicide Assessment Measures for Intervention Research with Adults and Older Adults. National Institute of Mental Health, 2004.Google Scholar
4 Osman, A, Kopper, BA, Barrios, FX, Osman, JR, Besett, T, Linehan, MM. The Brief Reasons for Living Inventory for Adolescents (BRFL-A). J Abnorm Child Psychol 1996; 24: 433–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Range, LM. The family of instruments that assess suicide risk. J Psychopathol Behav Assessment 2005; 27: 133–40.Google Scholar
6 Smith, P, Poindexter, E, Cuckrowicz, K. The effect of participating in suicide research: does participating in a research protocol on suicide and psychiatric symptoms increase suicide ideation and attempts? Suicide Life Threat Behav 2010; 40: 535–43.Google Scholar
7 Kessler, RC, Ustün, TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). Int J Meth Psychiatr Res 2004; 13: 93121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.