Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T07:08:37.341Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Distinguishing characteristics of subjects with good and poor early outcome in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Eve C. Johnstone*
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
Richard Cosway
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
Stephen M. Lawrie
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
*
Eve Johnstone, University Department of Psychiatry, Kennedy Tower, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Morningside Park, Edinburgh EH10 5HF, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

High-risk’ studies of schizophrenia have the potential to clarify the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Here, results of extreme outcome groups in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study are presented.

Aims

To compare groups of good and poor outcome from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study and clarify the nature of the change from the state of vulnerability to that of developing psychosis.

Method

The recruitment procedure is described. Good and poor outcome are defined. These groups are compared in terms of genetic liability and of baseline and change in neuropsychology and neuroanatomy.

Results

Demographic characteristics and genetic liability do not differ between the groups. The good outcome group perform better at baseline in some neuropsychological tests, but there is little neuroanatomical difference. The poor outcome group show consistently impaired memory function and a tendency to reduction in temporal lobe size.

Conclusions

In genetically predisposed subjects, the change from vulnerability to developing psychosis may be marked by a reduced size and impaired function of the temporal lobe.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2002 

The biological basis of schizophrenia is poorly understood although genetic factors are known to be important. Individuals who develop schizophrenia may have abnormalities of language (Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994), behaviour (Reference Done, Crow and JohnstoneDone et al, 1994) and motor development (Reference Walker, Savoie and DavisWalker et al, 1994) in childhood, but whether these features represent a vulnerability to schizophrenia or are precursors of the disorder is unclear. ‘High-risk’ studies of individuals at enhanced risk of developing schizophrenia could potentially clarify this but have mainly concerned individuals identified in infancy as the children of mothers with schizophrenia and thus extend for decades (Reference AsarnowAsarnow, 1988; Reference Cornblatt and ObuchowskiCornblatt & Obuchowski, 1997). The Edinburgh High-Risk Study (Reference Byrne, Hodges and GrantByrne et al, 1999; Reference Hodges, Byrne and GrantHodges et al, 1999; Lawrie et al, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Kestelman1999, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Abukmeil2001a , Reference Lawrie, Byrne and Miller b ; Reference Cosway, Byrne and ClaffertyCosway et al, 2000; Reference Johnstone, Abukmeil and ByrneJohnstone et al, 2000; Miller et al, Reference Miller, Lawrie and Hodges2001, Reference Miller, Byrne and Hodges2002) differs from others as the subjects have been recruited as young adults who will pass through the period of maximum risk of developing schizophrenia during the planned 10 years of the study. The investigation concerns young people aged between 16 and 25 years at ascertainment (when they were considered well) who have at least two close blood relatives with schizophrenia. A total of 229 such young people were identified and 162 of them have so far provided data. They were compared with 34 age-and gender-matched well young people, with no family history from the same communities (Reference Hodges, Byrne and GrantHodges et al 1999; Reference Johnstone, Abukmeil and ByrneJohnstone et al, 2000), and 36 age-matched subjects with first episodes of schizophrenia. The numbers in the control and first-episode groups were chosen to reflect the number of high-risk subjects eventually predicted to develop schizophrenia (approximately 30 individuals). The study has now been in progress for more than 6 years and some results have been presented (Reference Byrne, Hodges and GrantByrne et al, 1999; Lawrie et al, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Kestelman1999, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Abukmeil2001a ,Reference Lawrie, Byrne and Miller b ; Miller et al, Reference Miller, Lawrie and Hodges2001, Reference Miller, Byrne and Hodges2002). This report compares those individuals from within the high-risk sample who so far have achieved the best and the worst outcomes.

METHOD

Case ascertainment

The methods of the study have been described in detail in earlier papers. Essentially, subjects were assessed, at ascertainment and every 18 months until they develop schizophrenia or reach the age of 30 years, in terms of the following variables: (a) psychopathology as determined by the Present State Examination (PSE; Reference Wing, Cooper and SartoriusWing et al, 1974); (b) structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Lawrie et al, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Kestelman1999, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Abukmeil2001a ); and (c) an extensive programme of neuropsychological tests (Reference Byrne, Hodges and GrantByrne et al, 1999). In addition, assessments of social function, personality and behaviour and life events were made (Reference Hodges, Byrne and GrantHodges et al, 1999; Miller et al, Reference Miller, Lawrie and Hodges2001, Reference Miller, Byrne and Hodges2002).

Definition of outcome categories

As previously described (Reference Johnstone, Abukmeil and ByrneJohnstone et al, 2000), to simplify consideration of the psychopathology as determined by PSE, a simplified classification was drawn up on the basis of the PSE profiles whereby a score of 4=Catego S+ together with a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia; 3=fully rated psychotic symptom(s) 55-92 and/or fully rated behavioural item(s) 128, 129, 135, 136, 137; 2=3, but features partially rated or features 49-54 partially or fully rated and/or 108, 109, 118, 125, 126 fully and 133 partially or fully rated; 1=none of the above, but any other items fully rated; 0=none of the above. For the purposes of this study, those with the best outcome were those who have never achieved any fully rated score on any psychopathological item at PSE on any occasion of assessment (i.e. they always scored 0 on the study score), and who, in addition, had a record of sustained employment (or successful study towards employment) at a level higher or at least as high in terms of the Registrar General's ratings (Her Majesty's Stationery Office, 1991) of social class as their parents. Furthermore, at interview they were noted to have no abnormalities of social presentation and gave an account of unimpaired social performance. Within the context of the high-risk study, these individuals are referred to as the ‘perfects’. Those with the worst outcomes have developed schizophrenia, i.e., they achieved a score of 4 on the study score at the last time of assessment and in addition all fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia according to ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1993).

Comparisons

The ‘perfects’ and the individuals with newly developed schizophrenia were compared in terms of basic demographics, degree of genetic liability, baseline neuropsychology and neuroanatomy, and in those where there were at least two assessments before development of illness, change in neuropsychology and change in neuroanatomy. It will be appreciated that whereas most of the ‘perfects’ provided at least two assessments the numbers of individuals with newly developed schizophrenia were reduced by the fact that some of them became unwell before the second assessment could be carried out.

RESULTS

There are 24 ‘perfects’, i.e. 13 males and 11 females of mean age 21.2 years at ascertainment (range 16-24). Thirteen high-risk subjects have developed schizophrenia (8 males and 5 females) who at ascertainment were of mean age 20.3 years (range 16-23). This difference in age is not significant.

Genetic liability

Genetic liability was assessed categorically in terms of the numbers of relatives of first and second degree known to be affected but this does not, of course, take account of the entire numbers of relatives that the subjects had, and a continuous measure of genetic liability was devised by Professor Pak Sham at the Institute of Psychiatry. It has been described by Lawrie et al (Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Abukmeil2001a ) and takes account of the total number of relatives ill and well of each subject and their degree of relationship to the high-risk individual. On this scale, a higher score indicates a greater degree of genetic liability. The mean score of the ‘perfects’ was 0.25 (range -0.02 to +0.70) and that of those with new schizophrenia 0.16 (-0.01 to +0.40) but this difference is not significant. In the ‘perfects’, 18 had a genetic liability from the maternal side and 6 from the paternal. As far as those with new schizophrenia are concerned, six are known to have maternal genetic liability and five paternal. In the remaining two cases, it is possible that the inheritance is from both sides, but we do not have complete data on both maternal and paternal branches of these families.

Baseline measures

An extensive programme of neuropsychological tests was carried out at baseline on all entrants to the study and these are compared between the ‘perfects’ and those with new schizophrenia. Many of these tests showed no differences between these two groups (Table 1). Differences that were present were always in the direction that those who were destined to develop schizophrenia performed less well (Table 1). Baseline scans were available on 23 of the perfects and 10 of those destined to develop schizophrenia. Reasons for non-availability include pregnancy as well as reluctance to be scanned. The results are shown in Table 2. The significant difference in whole brain relates to the fact that there are more males in the newly developed schizophrenia group and where correction is made for gender and height, this difference disappears.

Table 1 Comparison of baseline neuropsychological test results between the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

‘Perfects’, mean (s.d.) New schizophrenia, mean (s.d.) P
NART IQ 98.8 (9.6) 97.6 (11.1) NS
WAIS—R 103.1 (14.1) 94.9 (9.8) 0.1
Absolute difference PIQ—VIQ 12.2 (7.6) 18.1 (6.3) 0.03
Arithmetic 10.5 (2.5) 8.5 (2.2) 0.03
Object assembly 10.3 (2.4) 8.5 (2.7) 0.06
Verbal fluency FAS 39.8 (14.2) 40.8 (14.1) NS
Verbal fluency animal 16.6 (5.7) 16.1 (4.4) NS
Stroop
Trial 3 23.1 (6.2) 23.2 (5.7) NS
Trial 3-1 12.8 (5.8) 13.8 (5.3) NS
Trial 3-2 10.3 (5.6) 10.8 (4.4) NS
2Hayling time for A 18.4 (1.7) 21.9 (1.9) NS
Type A errors 0 (0-6)1 3 (3-10)1 NS
Type B errors 2 (0-14)1 3 (0-9)1 NS
Total errors 2 (1-14)1 5 (2-12)1 NS
RBMT standardised 22 (21-24)1 20 (19-22)1 0.02
RBMT story 10.2 (2.9) 7.6 (4.1) 0.04
RAVLT total recall 52.2 (10.3) 45.1 (8.6) 0.06
RAVLT delayed recall 10.7 (2.9) 8.6 (2.8) 0.06
SCOLP spot the word 47 (4.3) 42.5 (4.7) 0.01
Token test 163 (162-163)1 162 (160-163)1 0.04

Table 2 Whole-brain volumes (cm3) and regional proportions (%), at baseline on the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

‘Perfects’ (12 males, 11 females) mean (s.d.) New schizophrenia (7 males, 3 females) mean (s.d.) P
Whole brain 1336.5 (120.4) 1432.4 (93.7) 0.033
Amygdala—hippocampus left 0.34 (0.039) 0.34 (0.042) 0.83
Amygdala—hippocampus right 0.36 (0.048) 0.36 (0.038) 0.75
Thalamic nucleus left 0.46 (0.043) 0.44 (0.054) (P < 0.05 in females)
Thalamic nucleus right 0.46 (0.047) 0.43 (0.049) (P < 0.05 in females)
Third ventricle 0.033 (0.018) 0.030 (0.025) 0.75

Differences between first and second assessments

We then examined the relationship between the first and second neuropsychological assessment and compared this between the ‘perfects’ and those of the newly developed schizophrenia group on whom we had two assessments (eight cases). The significant findings are shown in Table 3. There is consistently poorer performance in memory tests in those who will develop schizophrenia and an improvement in function in the Stroop tests in those patients but not in the ‘perfects’. All other tests were non-significant. Similarly, we compared the difference between the first and second scan in the ‘perfects’ and those with newly developed schizophrenia for whom two scans were available before they became ill. Most comparisons showed no tendency to significance. In particular, the amygdala—hippocampus, which has shown clear-cut findings such that this is smallest in the control schizophrenia group, next in the generality of the high-risk cases and largest in the normal controls (Lawrie et al, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Kestelman1999, Reference Lawrie, Whalley and Abukmeil2001a ), showed no tendency to a difference between the ‘perfects’ and those with new schizophrenia. By contrast, there was an apparent difference in the change in temporal lobe size between scans 1 and 2 (see Table 4). This does not achieve significance because of the small numbers and high variance but is of interest.

Table 3 Differences (mean (s.d.)) between first and second neuropsychological assessments in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

‘Perfects’ (n=22) New schizophrenia (n=8) P
Assessment 1 Assessment 2 Assessment 1 Assessment 2
RBMT
Standardised score1 22.5 (21-24) 22 (20-24) 21 (18.5-22.8) 20 (18.5-22.8) 0.02
Story (immediate recall) 10.4 (2.9) 9.2 (3.8) 7.9 (4.4) 6.5 (2.5) 0.04
Story (delayed recall) 8.7 (3.1) 8.2 (3.4) 6.7 (3.1) 6.1 (2.6) 0.04
RAVLT delayed recall 10.7 (2.9) 10.3 (2.9) 9 (2.7) 7.5 (2.6) 0.04
Stroop trial 3-1 12.4 (5.7) 11.3 (4.5) 15.3 (5) 11.1 (2.9) 0.04

Table 4 Differences between first and second scans in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group in terms of volume changes in left and right amygdala—hippocampus and temporal lobes

‘Perfects’ (n=22) New schizophrenia P
(mean, s.d.) mm3 (n=8) (mean, s.d.) mm3
Amygdala—hippocampus left -164.8 (458.9) -164.5 (590.2) 0.99
Amygdala—hippocampus right 152.1 (514.2) 10.5 (515.8) 0.51
Temporal lobe left -1089.9 (3826.8) -1854.0 (3708.9) 0.63
Temporal lobe right -139.2 (3579.2) -2245.2 (3234.5) 0.16

DISCUSSION

This paper presents preliminary findings concerning a comparison between two extreme subgroups of a much larger study. The conclusions that can be drawn are, therefore, tentative. None the less, it is clear that in terms of baseline demographic characteristics the two groups are similar and there is no evidence of greater genetic liability in those who will develop schizophrenia. There are neuropsychological differences at baseline between the two groups, such that the good outcome group perform better in terms of memory and some, but not all, measures of IQ. This is redolent of our previous study of treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia (Reference Lawrie, Ingle and SantoshLawrie et al, 1995). Frontal (Hayling test) and cingulate (Stroop test) tasks did not significantly differ between the two groups. The relatively low National Adult Reading Test (NART; Reference NelsonNelson, 1982) scores are likely to be because of the subjects' youth. At baseline there were essentially no neuroanatomical differences between the two groups and this contrasts with the baseline differences we have established between the high-risk subjects and both normal and schizophrenia controls (Reference Lawrie, Whalley and KestelmanLawrie et al, 1999). This may well be because of the small size of the groups in the current comparison, in that numbers larger than this are generally required to demonstrate differences between patients with schizophrenia and normal controls (Reference Lawrie and AbukmeilLawrie & Abukmeil, 1998). Where we have had the opportunity to assess the subjects twice before illness develops in comparison to the ‘perfects’, those who will develop schizophrenia show consistently poor memory function (Table 3). They also show a significant improvement in performance on the Stroop test, but this is not easy to interpret as it results from an initially non-significantly poorer performance.

The interest of the impaired memory function that we see before the manifestation of psychosis in those destined to develop schizophrenia is enhanced by the tendency of these subjects to show a reduction in temporal lobe size over the same period because, of course, memory function is most localisable to the temporal lobe. This finding reflects our earlier result (Reference Cosway, Byrne and ClaffertyCosway et al, 2000) of a pre-psychotic decline in memory. We have already shown that the neuropsychological impairments in subjects at enhanced risk of schizophrenia are widespread and affect many more individuals than are likely to develop the condition (Reference Byrne, Hodges and GrantByrne et al, 1999). We suggest that the findings may indicate that the feature that marks the change from vulnerability to developing psychosis is a reduction in size and impairment of function of the temporal lobe. Cognitive change seems to be a precursor and not a consequence of psychosis in people who have schizophrenia.

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Among genetically predisposed subjects, those who will go on to develop schizophrenia do not have greater genetic liability than those who will remain well.

  2. Some individuals with high genetic liability to schizophrenia are asymptomatic, with high levels of occupational and social function.

  3. Memory function may distinguish between those genetically predisposed individuals who will go on to develop schizophrenia and those who will not.

LIMITATIONS

  1. This is an interim analysis of selected subgroups and thus much information from the sample as a whole is not included.

  2. The membership of the groups is not yet fixed — more subjects will develop schizophrenia and some of the ‘perfects’ may deteriorate.

  3. The number of subjects with two assessments before illness supervenes is small.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported and conducted under the auspices of the ethics committees relevant to the districts in which the subjects lived. We are most grateful to Suheib Abukmeil, Majella Byrne, Bobby Clafferty, Elizabeth Grant, Anne Hodges and Jane Morris who recruited the subjects and conducted some of the assessments, and to Norma Brearley who carefully prepared the manuscript. We greatly appreciate the helpfulness of the subjects and their extended families, general practitioners and psychiatrists throughout Scotland.

Footnotes

Presented in part at the European First Episode Schizophrenia Network Meeting, Whistler BC, Canada, 27 April 2001.

Declaration of interest

This study was financed by the Medical Research Council, which supports S.M.L. and R.C.

References

Asarnow, J. R. (1988) Children at risk for schizophrenia: converging lines of evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 14, 613631.Google Scholar
Byrne, M., Hodges, A., Grant, E., et al (1999) Neuropsychological assessment of young people at high genetic risk for developing schizophrenia compared with controls: preliminary findings of the Edinburgh High Risk Study (EHRS). Psychological Medicine, 29, 11611173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, B. & Obuchowski, M. (1997) Update of high-risk research: 1987–1997. International Review of Psychiatry, 9, 437447.Google Scholar
Cosway, R., Byrne, M., Clafferty, R., et al (2000) Neuropsychological change in young people at high risk for schizophrenia: Results from the first two neuropsychological assessments of the Edinburgh High Risk Study. Psychological Medicine, 30, 11111121.Google Scholar
Done, D. J., Crow, T. J., Johnstone, E. C., et al (1994) Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. BMJ, 309, 699703.Google Scholar
Her Majesty's Stationery Office (1991) Classification of Occupations of the Registrar General. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Hodges, A., Byrne, M., Grant, E., et al (1999) People at risk of schizophrenia. Sample characteristics of the first 100 cases in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 547553.Google Scholar
Johnstone, E. C., Abukmeil, S. S., Byrne, M., et al (2000) Edinburgh High Risk Study – findings after four years. Demographic, attainment and psychopathological issues. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 13981402.Google Scholar
Lawrie, S. M. & Abukmeil, S. S. (1998) Brain abnormality in schizophrenia. A systematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 110120.Google Scholar
Lawrie, S. M., Ingle, G. T., Santosh, C. G., et al (1995) Magnetic resonance imaging and single photon emission tomography in treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 202210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrie, S. M., Whalley, H., Kestelman, J. N., et al (1999) Magnetic resonance imaging of brain in people at high risk of developing schizophrenia. Lancet, 353, 3033.Google Scholar
Lawrie, S. M., Whalley, H., Abukmeil, S. S., et al (2001a) Brain structure, genetic liability and psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of developing schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 811823.Google Scholar
Lawrie, S. M., Byrne, M., Miller, P., et al (2001b) Neurodevelopmental indices and the development of psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 524530.Google Scholar
Miller, P. M., Lawrie, S. M., Hodges, A., et al (2001) Genetic liability, illicit drug use, life stress and psychotic symptoms: preliminary findings from the Edinburgh study of people at high risk for schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36, 338342.Google Scholar
Miller, P. M., Byrne, M., Hodges, A., et al (2002) Childhood behaviour, psychotic symptoms and psychosis onset in young people at high risk of schizophrenia: early findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Psychological Medicine 32, 173179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H. E. (1982) The National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Walker, E. F., Savoie, T. & Davis, D. (1994) Neuromotor precursors of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 441451.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974) The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. An Instruction Manual for the PSE and Catego Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1993) ICD–10: The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Comparison of baseline neuropsychological test results between the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

Figure 1

Table 2 Whole-brain volumes (cm3) and regional proportions (%), at baseline on the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

Figure 2

Table 3 Differences (mean (s.d.)) between first and second neuropsychological assessments in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group

Figure 3

Table 4 Differences between first and second scans in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group in terms of volume changes in left and right amygdala—hippocampus and temporal lobes

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.