Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:44:06.528Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cortisol awakening response and subsequent depression: prospective longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rebecca Carnegie*
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Psychiatry, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, UK
Ricardo Araya
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Psychiatry, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, UK
Yoav Ben-Shlomo
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, UK
Vivette Glover
Affiliation:
Institute of Reproductive and Developmental Biology, Imperial College London, UK
Thomas G. O'Connor
Affiliation:
Wynne Center for Family Research, Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, USA
Kieran J. O'Donnell
Affiliation:
Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Canada
Rebecca Pearson
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Psychiatry, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, UK
Glyn Lewis
Affiliation:
Mental Health Sciences Unit, University College London, UK
*
Rebecca Carnegie, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Some studies have found an association between elevated cortisol and subsequent depression, but findings are inconsistent. The cortisol awakening response may be a more stable measure of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal function and potentially of stress reactivity.

Aims

To investigate whether salivary cortisol, particularly the cortisol awakening response, is associated with subsequent depression in a large population cohort.

Method

Young people (aged 15 years, n = 841) from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) collected salivary cortisol at four time points for 3 school days. Logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios for developing depression meeting ICD-10 criteria at 18 years.

Results

We found no evidence for an association between salivary cortisol and subsequent depression. Odds ratios for the cortisol awakening response were 1.24 per standard deviation (95% CI 0.93–1.66, P = 0.14) before and 1.12 (95% CI 0.73–1.72, P = 0.61) after adjustment for confounding factors. There was no evidence that the other cortisol measures, including cortisol at each time point, diurnal drop and area under the curve, were associated with subsequent depression.

Conclusions

Our findings do not support the hypothesis that elevated salivary cortisol increases the short-term risk of subsequent depressive illness. The results suggest that if an association does exist, it is small and unlikely to be of clinical significance.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2014 

Research into the neurobiology of depressive illness has frequently implicated the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which has been found to be abnormal in some people with both current Reference Bhagwagar, Hafizi and Cowen1,Reference Vreeburg, Hoogendijk, van, Derijk, Verhagen and van2 and recently remitted Reference Vreeburg, Hoogendijk, van, Derijk, Verhagen and van2,Reference Bhagwagar, Hafizi and Cowen3 depression. Elevated cortisol levels have also been found in cross-sectional studies of individuals at high risk for depression, Reference Dougherty, Klein, Olino, Dyson and Rose4-Reference van, Vreeburg, Van, Spinhoven, Zitman and Penninx9 suggesting that HPA abnormalities may play a role in the aetiology of depressive illness. Given the high prevalence of depressive symptoms in hypercortisolaemic states, both pathological as in Cushing’s syndrome, Reference Pereira, Tiemensma and Romijn10 as well as iatrogenic such as in long-term exogenous steroid use, Reference Patten11 it seems plausible that abnormal HPA function could increase the risk of subsequent depression.

The increasing availability of salivary cortisol assays has facilitated studies of HPA function on a larger scale. This has enabled a handful of studies to analyse baseline cortisol levels in individuals followed longitudinally to assess the onset of depression. Reference Halligan, Herbert, Goodyer and Murray12-Reference Herbert, Ban, Brown, Harris, Ogilvie and Uher18 Abnormalities in baseline cortisol levels among individuals developing subsequent depression have strengthened support for the hypothesis that the HPA axis has an aetiological role in depression, and that elevated cortisol levels could increase the risk of developing depressive illness. Reference Ellenbogen, Hodgins, Linnen and Ostiguy13,Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith17 There are a number of hypotheses for the mechanisms underlying this association, Reference Pariante and Lightman19 and it is difficult to specify precisely how HPA axis abnormalities might increase risk of depression, or exactly which cortisol measures might confer this risk. The lack of a clear hypothesis has led to a mixture of cortisol measures and methods for analysis being used. The consistent replication of results is a particular problem. Reference Knorr, Vinberg, Kessing and Wetterslev20,Reference Burke, Davis, Otte and Mohr21 Methodological differences, including variation in salivary collection protocols, sample populations, outcome measures and varying adjustment for confounding factors Reference Lopez-Duran, Kovacs and George22 may account for some of the inconsistency. It may also be a consequence of the inherent difficulties in reliably measuring an endocrinological system with marked diurnal as well as pulsatile variations. Reference Pariante and Lightman19 Some have also suggested that the relationship between cortisol and depression may be U-shaped rather than linear, further complicating analysis. Reference Herbert, Ban, Brown, Harris, Ogilvie and Uher18 Finally, it is possible that modest sample sizes combined with a wide range of potential ways to analyse cortisol may have increased type 1 errors. Using a more comprehensive approach to cortisol assessment and a larger sample size may enable a more thorough and inclusive analysis of HPA function, including the consideration of non-linear relationships. A cortisol awakening response (CAR) is increasingly regarded as a useful measure in cortisol research. The CAR is a normal physiological response to waking, and potentially captures information about stress reactivity in a minimally invasive way. Reference Pariante and Lightman19,Reference Adam and Kumari23 There is also evidence that it is a relatively stable measure, with evidence of heritability. Reference Wust, Federenko, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum24 The CAR may therefore be a particularly useful measure for prospective research in depression.

A handful of studies have looked at CAR in cross-sectional studies of individuals considered to be at high risk of depression. These studies have shown an elevated CAR in individuals with a family history of depression, Reference Vreeburg, Hartman, Hoogendijk, van, Zitman and Ormel6,Reference Mannie, Harmer and Cowen8 and in those with high hopelessness reactivity. Reference van, Vreeburg, Van, Spinhoven, Zitman and Penninx9 Conversely, a depressed CAR has been associated with other psychological vulnerabilities to depression such as rumination. Reference Kuehner, Holzhauer and Huffziger25 To our knowledge, only one study has used a prospective longitudinal design to assess whether CAR at baseline is associated with subsequent onset of depressive illness. Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith17 In that study of 230 healthy participants, there was some evidence that participants who went on to develop depression at 1 year had a higher CAR at baseline than those not developing depression (odds ratio (OR) = 2.96 per standard deviation, 95% CI 1.06-8.26, P = 0.04). Unfortunately, despite oversampling of individuals with symptoms of neuroticism to increase the incidence of depression, only 18 participants developed depression in the cohort. Continuing follow-up of these participants found decreasing evidence for an association between CAR and subsequent depression over time. Reference Vrshek-Schallhorn, Doane, Mineka, Zinbarg, Craske and Adam26 It is possible that the abnormalities may be early biological changes of an emerging or subclinical depressive process, rather than pre-existing markers observable in healthy individuals. Further studies using longer follow-up periods would help to clarify the association between CAR and subsequent depression. In our study we investigated whether CAR was associated with subsequent depression in a longitudinal study of a population cohort of British adolescents. We also examined other salivary cortisol measures used in previous HPA research. To our knowledge, this is the largest longitudinal study using a comprehensive cortisol protocol to date. Using an established population birth cohort has the advantage of being able to account for confounding factors without recall bias, as well as maximising on the proven collaboration of participants, which have both been suggested as limitations in previous cortisol research. Reference Pariante and Lightman19,Reference Halpern, Whitsel, Wagner and Harris27

Method

Participants

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing prospective longitudinal study of children born in the former county of Avon, UK. Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser and Henderson28,Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding and Davey29 Pregnant women with expected delivery dates between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 were recruited, with catch-up recruitment at age 7. The complete ALSPAC sample consists of 14 701 children alive at 1 year. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the local research ethics committees.

Participants were recruited from attendees of a research clinic at 15 years, which assessed 5501 of the ALSPAC participants (online Fig. DS1). Around 3020 participants were invited at random to participate in the cortisol data collection, 1175 declined. Of the 1845 participants agreeing to enter the study, 1035 returned their saliva sampling packs. However, 75 participants provided empty cortisol samples and 119 provided insufficient data for analysis of CAR. Of the remaining 841 participants, 173 participants had no outcome data for depression at 18, either because of non-attendance at the follow-up research clinic (n = 122) or non-completion of the Revised Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS-R) Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn30 diagnostic tool at the follow-up clinic (n = 51).

This left 668 participants (22.1% of invited) with both exposure and outcome measure at 18, on which the primary analysis was undertaken. For 208 participants there were one or more covariates missing from the ALSPAC data-set, leaving 460 complete cases. Sociodemographic characteristics of active participants were compared with the invited participants, using chi-squared tests to evaluate any selective attrition. Non-participants were defined as those randomly invited to participate at the baseline clinic but either declining to participate or failing to provide sufficient data.

Cortisol sampling

As described in detail elsewhere, Reference O'Donnell, Glover, Jenkins, Browne, Ben-Shlomo and Golding31 each participant agreeing to take part in the study was given a cortisol sampling pack during their clinic visit. Each pack contained detailed sampling instructions, 12 Salivette collection devices (Sarstedt, Germany), a sample collection diary sheet and a pre-paid envelope in which to return the samples to the laboratory. Participants were instructed to provide four samples per day over 3 consecutive school days, and to record the time of the sample. The samples were collected immediately on waking, at 30 min after waking, after school and before bed. To avoid contamination, participants were instructed not to eat or brush their teeth for at least 30 min prior to collection. On completion, samples were returned to the laboratory and stored at –20°C. Salivary cortisol was determined using a commercially available enzyme immune assay. Inter-assay and intra-assay variation were 7.9 and 8.9 respectively.

Cortisol samples were excluded if they were greater than 82 nmols/l (the upper limit of the cortisol assay) or greater than 4 standard deviations above the mean. Individual samples were also excluded if the timing deviated from the specified protocol. For the CAR, samples were excluded if taking the initial sample after 10.00 h or taking the second sample less than 20 or more than 60 min after the first. For the mean levels at each time point, the second sample was excluded if taken after 11.00 h, the ‘after school’ samples were excluded if before 15.00 h or after 18.00 h, and the final ‘before bed sample’ was excluded if before 19.00 h. Although previous literature suggested an absent CAR in a proportion of the population, a secondary analysis excluded individuals with an absent or negative CAR, in case this was the result of measurement error or non-adherence.

The primary cortisol measure of interest was the CAR, derived using the difference between the two morning cortisol samples. Additionally, the mean cortisol level at each time point, the estimated daily secretion as given by the area under the curve and the diurnal drop in cortisol, and estimated total morning cortisol were analysed. The diurnal drop in cortisol was taken as the difference between mean morning and bedtime cortisol levels. Total morning cortisol was calculated using the sum of the two morning samples. Each cortisol variable was calculated as a mean of the available samples over the study period.

Confounding factors

Potential confounders were decided a priori based on the available literature. Confounders available from assessment at 15 years included current depressive symptoms (using bands of the Development and Well-Being Assessment, DAWBA) Reference Goodman, Heiervang, Collishaw and Goodman32 body fat percentage, oral contraceptive use, smoking and alcohol use. Historical assessments also provided potentially confounding background characteristics such as gender, social class, birth weight, gestation, prior depressive symptoms (using continuous scores of the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) Reference Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder and Silver33 assessed at 10, 13 and 14 years), maternal education, maternal history of depression and exposure to postnatal depression (assessed using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, EPDS). Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky34

Following the development of a full set of covariates, certain variables were dropped from further consideration as they were neither associated with the cortisol measures nor had an impact on the effect size. This also helped to minimise numbers lost through incomplete data. This subset included anxiety symptoms at 15 years, neuroticism, cannabis use, friendships and social support, early significant life events, hours of sleep and income. Additionally, to avoid overadjustment and preserve sample size, groups of similar potential confounders were reduced to a single representative variable, with the others dropped from the final model. This subset included baseline fitness level and exercise, which was replaced by adiposity alone and antenatal depressive and anxiety symptoms, which was replaced by postnatal depressive symptoms alone.

Finally, because of the composition of the sample, other variables with negligible prevalence were dropped as covariates. First, as none of the individuals in the sample population were pre-pubertal (taken as a Tanner stage <2), pubertal stage was dropped as a confounder. Similarly, as only three participants reported steroid use prior to the study period, none of whom developed depression on follow-up, this was also dropped from the final model.

Outcome measures

The main outcome measure was ICD-10 diagnosis of depression, 35 which was assessed using the CIS-R Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn30 at a research clinic attended by participants at a mean age of 17.7 years. Responses to the questions about 14 symptom groups, as well as their onset and duration, were given via computer self-assessment at a clinic visit. The five symptom groups used to assess depression were sleep problems, poor concentration, fatigue and depression (which were scored on a scale of 0-4) and depressive thoughts (scored from 0 to 5). Responses to questions were aggregated by the specified algorithm to make a binary variable of ICD-10 diagnosis of depression.

A secondary analysis was undertaken using a binary cut off of ‘depressive symptoms’, to account for subclinical symptoms of depression. A ‘depressive symptoms’ score was derived from the sum of the five depressive symptom groups in the CIS-R. This score ranged from 0 to 21. A score of ⩾8 depressive symptoms was chosen, aiming for a prevalence of ∼15%. Secondary analysis also looked at depressive symptoms after 1 year using a cut-off of ⩾11 on the SMFQ, Reference Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder and Silver33 assessed by postal questionnaire at a mean age of 16.7 years.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using Stata 12 on Windows. Logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios between each cortisol variable and the subsequent development of depression. As CAR measured on a single day has been suggested to be related to current circumstances rather than a stable interpersonal state marker, Reference Hellhammer, Fries, Schweisthal, Schlotz, Stone and Hagemann36 the number of days used to calculate a mean CAR was included as a covariate. The waking cortisol level was also included as a covariate as this has been shown to affect the size of the CAR. Reference O'Connor, Ben-Shlomo, Heron, Golding, Adams and Glover37

Table 1 Differences in characteristics of participants with exposure and outcome data compared with those invited to participate from baseline assessment clinic

Non-participants
(n = 2363)
Participants with exposure
and outcome (n = 668)
χ2 t-test P
Gender, n (%)
    Male 1159 (49.1) 301 (45.1) 3.3 0.07
    Female 1204 (51.0) 367 (54.9)
Maternal education, n (%)
    <O-level 426 (19.4) 99 (15.5) 7.2 0.03
    O-level 747 (34.0) 247 (38.7)
    A-level or higher 1022 (46.6) 293 (45.8)
    Unknown 168 29
Social class,Footnote a n (%)
    I 75 (3.6) 30 (4.8) 4.7 0.32
    II 563 (26.7) 177 (28.4)
    III 1133 (53.8) 327 (52.5)
    IV 281 (13.3) 70 (11.2)
    V 54 (2.6) 19 (3.1)
    Unknown 257 45
Ethnicity of child, n (%)
    White 2075 (96.1) 605 (96.2) 0.0 0.93
    Black and minority ethnic 84 (3.9) 24 (3.8)
    Unknown 204 39
Depression at 15 years, n (%)
    Yes 222 (9.5) 42 (6.3) 6.6 0.01
    No 2118 (90.5) 625 (93.7)
    Unknown 23 1
Previous depression, n (%)
    Yes 345 (14.8) 82 (12.4) 2.5 0.12
    No 1980 (85.2) 579 (87.6)
    Unknown 38 7
Depression at 18 years, n (%)
    Yes 108 (7.3) 46 (6.9) 0.1 0.74
    No 1376 (92.7) 622 (93.1)
    Unknown 879
Body fat, %: mean (s.d.) 22.2 (10.3) 23.2 (10.3) −2.2 0.03

a. I, professional occupations; II, managerial and technical; III, skilled occupations (includes manual and non-manual); IV, partly-skilled occupations; V, unskilled occupations.

The possibility of a non-linear relationship between cortisol and depression was investigated using a quadratic term. We postulated a priori that there may be an interaction between gender and CAR and tested this using an appropriate interaction term in the regression model. An interaction term in the regression model was also used to investigate whether the association between cortisol and subsequent depression differed for participants with previous depression, baseline depressive symptoms or those with a positive family history of depression.

Missing data

Multiple imputation using chained equations (MICE) was used to address missing data. Reference White, Royston and Wood38 The comprehensiveness of data collection in this cohort means that the missing values were assumed to be dependent on other observed data. The MICE procedure was used to address data missing on two levels: first, participants with one or more missing confounding factors had this data imputed; and second, a depression outcome at 18 years was imputed using previous measures of depression in earlier adolescence. The data were imputed using the ‘ice’ command of Stata, using 45 imputation models set at 20 cycles. The imputation model included all variables identified as potential confounders, including those discarded from the final analysis model. Additionally, previously collected values of the missing variables and background characteristics of the sample previously shown to affect missing data were included in the imputation model (Fig. DS1). Non-normally distributed variables (such as adiposity) were log-transformed to allow imputation. Highly skewed variables such as maternal measures of depression and previous depressive symptom scores were imputed using prediction matching. Categories with very small numbers (such as DAWBA bands 4 and 5, more than three siblings, household income bands, and frequency of exercise, cannabis and alcohol use) were collapsed. Monte Carlo errors were <10% of standard errors and the maximum fraction of missing information value was 0.43. Reference White, Royston and Wood38

Table 2 Cortisol values (nmols/l) in all participants

n Mean s.d. Range Interquartile range Mean sample time
Cortisol awakening response
    All participants 841 3.84 4.71 −14.60 to 26.11 1.04 to 6.40 0 h 35 min
    With depression 46 5.07 4.61 −12.34 to 26.11 1.28 to 7.93
    Without depression 622 3.87 5.52 −4.77 to 24.53 1.13 to 6.40
Waking cortisol
    All participants 887 7.62 3.72 0.03 to 26.74 5.10 to 9.22 07.03 h
    With depression 49 7.11 3.03 2.38 to 17.53 4.75 to 9.22
    Without depression 659 7.59 3.80 0.29 to 26.74 5.08 to 9.05
Cortisol sample 2
    All participants 891 11.30 5.20 0.44 to 33.75 7.81 to 14.25 07.41 h
    With depression 49 11.85 5.70 2.79 to 33.06 7.82 to 15.10
    Without depression 665 11.29 5.24 0.44 to 33.75 7.78 to 14.15
After school cortisol
    All participants 868 2.40 1.60 0.00 to 11.20 1.40 to 2.95 16.30 h
    With depression 45 2.16 1.33 0.39 to 7.61 1.48 to 2.39
    Without depression 646 2.39 1.53 0.00 to 11.20 1.37 to 2.96
Bedtime cortisol
    All participants 930 1.19 1.38 0.00 to 12.22 0.46 to 1.33 21.49 h
    With depression 49 0.92 0.81 0.00 to 4.35 0.39 to 1.16
    Without depression 687 1.15 1.27 0.00 to 11.09 0.45 to 1.30
Diurnal drop
    All participants 897 8.41 3.79 −2.30 to 28.46 5.95 to 10.50 16 h 5 min
    With depression 49 8.57 3.37 2.12 to 17.55 6.65 to 10.85
    Without depression 687 8.42 3.89 −2.30 to 28.46 5.94 to 10.49
Total daily cortisol (area under the curve, g)
    All participants 721 12 275 4581 116 to 37 811 9060 to 14 885
    With depression 39 11 388 4068 3712 to 26 646 8813 to 14 005
    Without depression 540 12 221 4504 116 to 32 485 8976 to 14 811

Table 3 Characteristics of participants by quartile of cortisol awakening response (CAR) (n = 841)Footnote a

CAR quartile, n (%)
Attribute n 1 (n = 210) 2 (n = 210) 3 (n = 210) 4 (n = 211) P
Baseline and prior depression
    With depression at baseline 839 8.1 3.8 4.8 11.0 0.22
    History of depression 832 11.1 12.9 12.4 13.0 0.60
Background characteristics
    Female 841 43.3 42.9 56.7 75.8 <0.01
    Social class 4 or 5 774 15.3 12.6 15.6 15.3 0.80
    Post O-level maternal education 798 42.0 49.5 41.7 48.7 0.43
    Maternal history of depression 791 7.7 7.0 3.6 4.0 0.05
    Exposed to postnatal depression 834 16.7 15.9 17.3 12.4 0.31
    Low birth weight 809 3.0 5.9 5.0 3.0 0.88
    Preterm birth (⩽32 weeks) 841 0.5 1.0 2.4 0.0 1.00
Lifestyle factors
    Highest body fat quartile 837 22.0 22.6 24.9 40.3 <0.01
    Taking oral contraceptive pill 811 4.4 3.9 4.5 5.5 0.58
    Current daily smoker 828 3.5 4.8 1.4 6.5 0.41
    Alcohol >2 days per week 704 2.9 2.2 4.1 7.3 0.03

a. Table includes all participants with a CAR, including those with missing outcome data.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The characteristics of participants actively taking part in the cortisol study are shown in Table 1. Active study participants were more likely to be female, have higher maternal education and a lower prevalence of depression at baseline than non-participants. Distribution of social class, ethnicity, previous depression scores and prevalence of depression at 18 (where this was known) differed little between the groups.

Descriptive statistics for cortisol are given in Table 2. Around 75% of participants showed a rise in their morning cortisol, with the remainder showing no response or a negative response after waking. The four daily samples displayed the expected diurnal variation. The CAR was normally distributed with very slight right deviation (skewness 0.58).

Table 4 Odds ratios (ORs) for depression at 18 years for each standard deviation increase in cortisol awakening response (CAR), total morning cortisol, mean waking and bedtime cortisol, diurnal drop and area under the curveFootnote a

Cumulative adjustments
Unadjusted Complete cases Baseline and prior depressionFootnote b Background characteristicsFootnote c Lifestyle factorsFootnote d
OR (95% CI) P n OR (95% CI) P n OR (95% CI) P n OR (95% CI) P n OR (95% CI) P n
CAR 1.24 (0.93–1.66) 0.14 668 1.18 (0.82–1.70) 0.38 460 1.18 (0.87–1.61) 0.28 660 1.19 (0.84–1.68) 0.34 578 1.12 (0.73–1.72) 0.61 460
CAR (excluding flat or negative results) 1.25 (0.95–1.65) 0.12 562 1.21 (0.85–1.71) 0.29 387 1.17 (0.86–1.58) 0.32 555 1.21 (0.86–1.69) 0.27 484 1.31 (0.86–1.98) 0.21 387
Total early morning cortisol 1.49 (0.93–2.40) 0.10 668 1.40 (0.77–2.54) 0.27 460 1.38 (0.83–2.27) 0.21 660 1.43 (0.81–2.52) 0.22 578 1.30 (0.65–2.59) 0.47 460
Mean waking cortisol 0.87 (0.64–1.19) 0.39 708 0.85 (0.57–1.26) 0.41 485 0.86 (0.63–1.20) 0.38 700 0.81 (0.56–1.18) 0.28 608 0.86 (0.56–1.33) 0.50 485
Mean bedtime cortisol 0.77 (0.49–1.20) 0.25 708 0.74 (0.43–1.28) 0.29 488 0.76 (0.48–1.20) 0.24 701 0.64 (0.37–1.13) 0.12 609 0.57 (0.29–1.10) 0.09 488
Diurnal drop 1.05 (0.78–1.40) 0.76 710 0.99 (0.69–1.41) 0.94 487 1.01 (0.74–1.36) 0.97 702 0.97 (0.69–1.36) 0.84 609 0.95 (0.64–1.42) 0.82 487
Area under the curve, g 0.81 (0.57–1.17) 0.26 579 0.84 (0.56–1.26) 0.41 401 0.80 (0.56–1.15) 0.24 574 0.77 (0.53–1.14) 0.20 505 0.76 (0.49–1.17) 0.22 401
Imputed data-sets
    CARFootnote e 1.24 (0.93–1.66) 0.14 668 1.22 (0.91–1.64) 0.19 668 1.11 (0.80–1.53) 0.52 668 1.12 (0.80–1.56) 0.51 668
    CARFootnote f 1.20 (0.89–1.62) 0.24 841 1.17 (0.87–1.57) 0.30 841 1.07 (0.78–1.46) 0.68 841 1.09 (0.78–1.52) 0.61 841

a. All results are adjusted for mean cortisol at Time 1 and number of CAR samples, except for results that are unadjusted.

b. Includes additional adjustment for depressive symptoms at 15 years and prior depression.

c. Includes additional adjustment for gender, social class, birth weight, gestation, maternal education, maternal history of depression and exposure to postnatal depression.

d. Includes further adjustments for oral contraceptive use, body fat, alcohol and smoking.

e. Data-set with imputed confounders.

f. Data-set with imputed confounders and outcome.

Confounding factors

Table 3 shows the characteristics of participants in each quartile group of CAR. The biggest differences were seen for gender, body fat, alcohol and maternal history of depression. There was a higher proportion of baseline depression in the top quartile, however this quartile also had a higher proportion of women.

Association with depression

Of the participants, 46 (6.89%) were diagnosed with depression at 18 years, 33 of whom were female (71.74%), and 34 of whom (73.91%) were considered ‘new onset’ as they had no previous recorded episodes.

Results of logistic regression for the main cortisol parameters of interest are provided in Table 4. There was no statistical evidence for an association between any of the cortisol measures and depression at follow-up, either before or after adjustment for confounding factors. The unadjusted odds ratio for depression at 18 was 1.24 per standard deviation of CAR (95% CI 0.93-1.66, P = 0.14.) Adjusting for confounding factors and lifestyle factors decreased the effect size to 1.12 (95% CI 0.73-1.72, P = 0.61). There was still no evidence for an association using the first day sample only (which gave the highest value of CAR) (OR = 1.09, 95% CI 0.80-1.48, P = 0.58). A similar result was found when CIS-R ‘depressive symptoms’ was used as a binary outcome (OR = 1.07, 95% CI 0.78-1.48, P = 0.67). Although there was weak evidence for an association between CAR and depressive symptoms at 16 years (OR = 1.22, 95% CI 0.99-1.51, P = 0.06) this was markedly attenuated by confounding factors (online Table DS1). Adjusting for gender alone reduced the odds ratio to 1.11 (95% CI 0.89-1.37, P = 0.36).

There was no evidence that any of the other cortisol parameters including the area under the curve, diurnal drop or total morning cortisol secretion were associated with subsequent depression. There was no evidence that the association between CAR and depression varied by gender (P = 0.69), previous depression (P = 0.44) or maternal history of depression (P = 0.67). There was some evidence for a possible interaction between baseline depression score and CAR (P = 0.06). However, the apparent interaction disappeared after excluding the 42 people with depression at baseline (P = 0.87), suggesting a possible association between CAR and depression resolution or recurrence, but not with depression incidence.

Table 5 shows the characteristics of the cortisol awakening response divided into quartiles. The proportion with depression appears higher in the top and bottom quartiles, suggesting a U-shaped distribution. However, there was no statistical evidence for this possibility when investigated using a quadratic term (P = 0.38). The relationships were attenuated following adjustment for gender, and for adiposity and alcohol use, which were also most common in the top quartile. These factors also provided the biggest attenuation of the relationship between cortisol and depression in the regression analyses.

Missing data

Imputation of missing confounders increased the final adjusted model numbers to 668. Imputation of missing outcome data increased the sample size to 841, with depression prevalence of 7.61%. Results of logistic regression using both imputed data-sets are provided in Table 4. Despite gains in statistical power, imputation of the missing data did not alter our findings.

Table 5 Cortisol (nmols/l) and depressive symptoms score given by quartile of cortisol awakening response (CAR)

CAR
quartile
Cortisol
mean (s.d.)
Depressive symptoms
score, mean (s.d.)
CIS-R depression
(n = 668)
CIS-R depression
after adjustments (n = 462)
n % casesFootnote a OR (95% CI) P OR (95% CI) P
1 210 −1.48 (2.59) 2.97 (3.69) 6.88 1.00 1.00
2 210 2.17 (0.64) 3.10 (3.62) 5.95 0.77 (0.31-1.90) 0.578 0.47 (0.14-1.58) 0.224
3 210 4.75 (0.84) 3.22 (3.81) 5.33 0.68 (0.27-1.72) 0.578 0.57 (0.18-1.75) 0.324
4 211 9.88 (3.53) 3.31 (3.71) 9.36 1.27 (0.31-1.91) 0.564 0.85 (0.30-2.47) 0.772

CIS-R, Revised Clinical Interview Schedule.

a. Percentage of individuals with an ICD-10 diagnosis of depression based on the specified algorithm for the CIS-R.

Discussion

The findings from this large prospective cohort study do not support the hypothesis that the CAR, or any other basal salivary cortisol measures including cortisol at each time point, diurnal drop and area under the curve, increase the risk of developing a depressive illness. The small unadjusted association with depression at 16 years in this study may initially appear to corroborate previous suggestions of a time-limited association between the CAR and depression. Reference Vrshek-Schallhorn, Doane, Mineka, Zinbarg, Craske and Adam26 However, as adjusting for confounding factors nullifies this association, we find no evidence for a unique association between cortisol and depression at either time point.

Strengths and limitations

This study has several strengths including a large sample size, which is to our knowledge the largest prospective study using CAR to date. However, this study may still have lacked sufficient statistical power. Although the confidence intervals in this study do overlap those in previous research, the point estimate of the previous study (OR = 2.96) lies well outside our estimates here. Reference Adam, Doane, Zinbarg, Mineka, Craske and Griffith17 The upper limit of our confidence interval (OR = 1.7) suggests that if a true difference in CAR does exist, it is small.

Recruitment from the ALSPAC cohort has enabled this study to consider a wealth of potential confounders. The inability of previous studies to account for these without inducing recall bias has been suggested as a potential factor underlying some of the inconsistency among previous studies. Reference Pariante and Lightman19 In our study there was some evidence that adjusting for certain confounding factors, such as alcohol use, would attenuate an apparent relationship with subsequent depression.

It is important to consider both selection and loss to follow-up bias as only a subset of participants provided cortisol samples and there was some attrition over time. This raises the concern that participants who are susceptible to depression, may be less likely to participate in or to complete the study protocol. However, there was a similar prevalence of depression at follow-up in this sample to the rest of the ALSPAC cohort. Additionally, controlling for potentially confounding factors such as social class and baseline depression (which also related to whether data were missing) would have minimised this bias. Finally, we investigated any influence missing data might have made using multiple imputation, and this had no overall impact on our results.

A further strength of this study is the comprehensive protocol for cortisol assessment, which exceeded a ‘minimum protocol’ for salivary cortisol collection in several ways. Reference Adam and Kumari23 It is possible that a two-sample protocol for CAR calculation may lose some sensitivity compared with a four-sample protocol. Reference Adam and Kumari23,Reference Hellhammer, Fries, Schweisthal, Schlotz, Stone and Hagemann36 However, a two-sample protocol is well accepted in the literature, and any calculation of CAR in a prospective study has advantages over much of the literature to date. Nevertheless, measurement error is likely to have occurred because of inaccurate reporting of sample collection times, potentially reducing the strength of an association. Reference Halpern, Whitsel, Wagner and Harris27 This problem has led to some researchers starting to use automatic timing devices for salivary cortisol collection. However, there is no reason that poor adherence would occur more frequently than in previous studies, especially given the proven levels of cooperation of ALSPAC participants over the years. Measurement bias may also have been introduced by choosing point prevalences of depression at 16 and 18 years, although we do not think this would have introduced a major bias especially given the surge in depressive prevalence in this age group. Reference Moffitt, Harrington, Caspi, Kim-Cohen, Goldberg and Gregory39

Implications

The findings from this large prospective study do not support the hypothesis that salivary cortisol is associated with the subsequent onset of depression at a population level. Our results suggest that if a true association exists, it is smaller than estimated by previous studies, is markedly attenuated by confounding factors and is unlikely to have any clinical significance. There are a number of reasons why this result appears to contradict previous study findings, which have implications for future research.

It is possible that abnormal HPA function may have a minor influence on the complex aetiology of depression, making the overall population difference small, or that HPA abnormalities are only found in a small subset of depression, which is no doubt a heterogeneous disorder. Reference Antonijevic40 Future prospective studies will need adequate power, requiring large sample sizes to detect small differences, or to investigate distinct subgroups.

The biological complexities of the HPA axis may also account for research inconsistencies. Salivary cortisol alone may be too crude a measure for prospective prediction and a more comprehensive approach may be required. Reference Hellhammer, Wust and Kudielka41 Using laboratory-based tests of stress responsiveness, although impractical for studies of this size, may provide different parameters with which the association with depression becomes clearer.

At present a lack of clear hypothesis about which cortisol parameters are associated with future depressive illness may be causing spurious differences in cortisol being identified through multiple testing and selective reporting. Future research should publish both negative as well as positive results of all cortisol measures collected, to ensure a balanced view in literature and avoid publication bias. Furthermore, research to clarify any possible biological link between the HPA axis and depression, including mechanisms and potential parameters for measurement, will facilitate the improved design of any future epidemiological studies.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Footnotes

The UK Medical Research Council (Grant reference: 74882), the Wellcome Trust (Grant reference: 076467) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This research was supported by NIH grant R01 MH073842 to T.G.O'C., which funded the cortisol collection; Wellcome Trust grant 084268/2/07/Z to G.L., which funded the CIS-R; and the Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support grant 097825.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Bhagwagar, Z, Hafizi, S, Cowen, PJ. Increased salivary cortisol after waking in depression. Psychopharmacology 2005; 182: 54–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Vreeburg, SA, Hoogendijk, WJ, van, Pelt J, Derijk, RH, Verhagen, JC, van, Dyck R, et al. Major depressive disorder and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity: results from a large cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 617–26.Google Scholar
3 Bhagwagar, Z, Hafizi, S, Cowen, PJ. Increase in concentration of waking salivary cortisol in recovered patients with depression. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1890–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Dougherty, LR, Klein, DN, Olino, TM, Dyson, M, Rose, S. Increased waking salivary cortisol and depression risk in preschoolers: the role of maternal history of melancholic depression and early child temperament. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50: 1495–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Young, EA, Vazquez, D, Jiang, H, Pfeffer, CR. Saliva cortisol and response to dexamethasone in children of depressed parents. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60: 831–6.Google Scholar
6 Vreeburg, SA, Hartman, CA, Hoogendijk, WJG, van, Dyck R, Zitman, FG, Ormel, J, et al. Parental history of depression or anxiety and the cortisol awakening response. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 180–5.Google Scholar
7 Vinberg, M, Bennike, B, Kyvik, KO, Andersen, PK, Kessing, LV. Salivary cortisol in unaffected twins discordant for affective disorder. Psychiatry Res 2008; 161: 292301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Mannie, ZN, Harmer, CJ, Cowen, PJ. Increased waking salivary cortisol levels in young people at familial risk of depression. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164: 617–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 van, Santen A, Vreeburg, SA, Van, der Does AJ, Spinhoven, P, Zitman, FG, Penninx, BW. Psychological traits and the cortisol awakening response: results from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011; 36: 240–8.Google Scholar
10 Pereira, AM, Tiemensma, J, Romijn, JA. Neuropsychiatric disorders in Cushing's syndrome. Neuroendocrinology 2010; 92 (suppl 1): 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Patten, SB. Exogenous corticosteroids and major depression in the general population. J Psychosom Res 2000; 49: 447–9.Google Scholar
12 Halligan, SL, Herbert, J, Goodyer, I, Murray, L. Disturbances in morning cortisol secretion in association with maternal postnatal depression predict subsequent depressive symptomatology in adolescents. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62: 40–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Ellenbogen, MA, Hodgins, S, Linnen, AM, Ostiguy, CS. Elevated daytime cortisol levels: a biomarker of subsequent major affective disorder? J Affect Disord 2011; 132: 265–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14 Goodyer, IM, Tamplin, A, Herbert, J, Altham, PME. Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 499504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Harris, TO, Borsanyi, S, Messari, S, Stanford, K, Brown, GW, Cleary, SE, et al. Morning cortisol as a risk factor for subsequent major depressive disorder in adult women. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 505–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Goodyer, IM, Bacon, A, Ban, M, Croudace, T, Herbert, J. Serotonin transporter genotype, morning cortisol and subsequent depression in adolescents. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 3945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17 Adam, EK, Doane, LD, Zinbarg, RE, Mineka, S, Craske, MG, Griffith, JW. Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 921–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18 Herbert, J, Ban, M, Brown, GW, Harris, TO, Ogilvie, A, Uher, R, et al. Interaction between the BDNF gene Val/66/Met polymorphism and morning cortisol levels as a predictor of depression in adult women. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 201: 313–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Pariante, CM, Lightman, SL. The HPA axis in major depression: classical theories and new developments. Trends Neurosci 2008; 31: 464–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Knorr, U, Vinberg, M, Kessing, LV, Wetterslev, J. Salivary cortisol in depressed patients versus control persons: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 1275–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21 Burke, HM, Davis, MC, Otte, C, Mohr, DC. Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005; 30: 846–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Lopez-Duran, NL, Kovacs, M, George, CJ. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 1272–83.Google Scholar
23 Adam, EK, Kumari, M. Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 1423–36.Google Scholar
24 Wust, S, Federenko, I, Hellhammer, DH, Kirschbaum, C. Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000; 25: 707–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 Kuehner, C, Holzhauer, S, Huffziger, S. Decreased cortisol response to awakening is associated with cognitive vulnerability to depression in a nonclinical sample of young adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007; 32: 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26 Vrshek-Schallhorn, S, Doane, LD, Mineka, S, Zinbarg, RE, Craske, MG, Adam, EK. The cortisol awakening response predicts major depression: predictive stability over a 4-year follow-up and effect of depression history. Psychol Med 2013; 43: 483–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27 Halpern, CT, Whitsel, EA, Wagner, B, Harris, KM. Challenges of measuring diurnal cortisol concentrations in a large population-based field study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012; 37: 499508.Google Scholar
28 Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J, Lawlor, DA, Fraser, A, Henderson, J, et al. Cohort profile: the ‘Children of the 90s’–the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42: 111–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29 Fraser, A, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey, Smith G, et al. cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42: 97110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30 Lewis, G, Pelosi, AJ, Araya, R, Dunn, G. Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med 1992; 22: 465–86.Google Scholar
31 O'Donnell, KJ, Glover, V, Jenkins, J, Browne, D, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Golding, J, et al. Prenatal maternal mood is associated with altered diurnal cortisol in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38: 1630–8.Google Scholar
32 Goodman, A, Heiervang, E, Collishaw, S, Goodman, R. The ‘DAWBA bands’ as an ordered-categorical measure of child mental health: description and validation in British and Norwegian samples. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2011; 46: 521–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Messer, SC, Pickles, A, Winder, F, Silver, D. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Method Psych 1995; 5: 237–49.Google Scholar
34 Cox, JL, Holden, JM, Sagovsky, R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-Item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry 1987; 150: 782–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
36 Hellhammer, J, Fries, E, Schweisthal, OW, Schlotz, W, Stone, AA, Hagemann, D. Several daily measurements are necessary to reliably assess the cortisol rise after awakening: state- and trait components. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007; 32: 80–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 O'Connor, TG, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Heron, J, Golding, J, Adams, D, Glover, V. Prenatal anxiety predicts individual differences in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58: 211–7.Google Scholar
38 White, IR, Royston, P, Wood, AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med 2011; 30: 377–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39 Moffitt, TE, Harrington, H, Caspi, A, Kim-Cohen, J, Goldberg, D, Gregory, AM, et al. Depression and generalized anxiety disorder - cumulative and sequential comorbidity in a birth cohort followed prospectively to age 32 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64: 651–60.Google Scholar
40 Antonijevic, IA. Depressive disorders – is it time to endorse different pathophysiologies? Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006; 31: 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41 Hellhammer, DH, Wust, S, Kudielka, BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 163–71.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Differences in characteristics of participants with exposure and outcome data compared with those invited to participate from baseline assessment clinic

Figure 1

Table 2 Cortisol values (nmols/l) in all participants

Figure 2

Table 3 Characteristics of participants by quartile of cortisol awakening response (CAR) (n = 841)a

Figure 3

Table 4 Odds ratios (ORs) for depression at 18 years for each standard deviation increase in cortisol awakening response (CAR), total morning cortisol, mean waking and bedtime cortisol, diurnal drop and area under the curvea

Figure 4

Table 5 Cortisol (nmols/l) and depressive symptoms score given by quartile of cortisol awakening response (CAR)

Supplementary material: PDF

Carnegie et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Carnegie et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 407.8 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.