Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:39:17.435Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Graphical Representations of a Television Series: A Study with Deaf and Hearing Adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Cristina Cambra*
Affiliation:
Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona (Spain)
Aurora Leal
Affiliation:
Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona (Spain)
Núria Silvestre
Affiliation:
Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Cristina Cambra. Edificio B. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona. 08193 Bellaterra. Barcelona. (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The understanding of a television story can be very different depending on the age of the viewer, their background knowledge, the content of the programme and the way in which they combine the information gathered from linguistic, audio and visual elements. This study explores the different ways of interpreting an audiovisual document considering that, due to a hearing impaired, visual, audio and linguistic information could be perceived very differently to the way it is by hearing people. The study involved the participation of 20 deaf and 20 hearing adolescents, aged 12 to 19 years who, after watching a fragment of a television series, were asked to draw a picture of what had happened in the story. The results show that the graphical representation of the film is similar for both groups in terms of the number of scenes, but there is greater profusion, in the deaf group, of details about the context and characters, and there are differences in their interpretations of some of the sequences in the story.

La comprensión de una historia de la televisión puede ser muy diversa según la edad del espectador, sus conocimientos previos, el contenido del programa, y la manera como combina los elementos informativos lingüísticos, sonoros y la imagen visual. Este estudio explora las distintas maneras de interpretar un documento audiovisual considerando que, debido a un déficit auditivo, las informaciones visuales, sonoras y lingüísticas pueden ser percibidas de forma distinta a como lo hacemos los oyentes. En el estudio han participado 20 adolescentes sordos y 20 oyentes, de 12 a 19 años, a los que, tras visionar un fragmento de una seria televisiva, se les solicita que hagan un dibujo de lo que sucede en la historia. Los resultados indican que la representación gráfica del film es similar en ambos grupos en cuanto al número de escenas, pero difieren en la mayor profusión, en el grupo de adolescentes sordos, de detalles contextuales y de personajes, así como en la interpretación de alguna de las secuencias de la historia.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackermann Valladao, E. (1987). Que deviennent les idées a propos d'un phénomène, une fois retraduites a travers différents médias? Le rôle du dessin, du récit et du geste dans la construction d'une connaissance.[What happens to ideas about a phenomenon, once translated back through different media? The role of drawing, narrative and gesture in the construction of knowledge]. Archives de Psychologie, 55, 195218.Google Scholar
Bombi, A. S., & Pinto, G. (1993). Los colores de la amistad [The colours of friendship]. Madrid: Visor.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Riviere, A., Fukushima, M., French, D., Hadwin, J., Cross, P. et al. , (1996). Reading the mind in the face: a cross-cultural and development study. Visual Cognition, 3(1), 3959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W. G., & Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf individuals see better?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 512518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cambra, C., Silvestre, N., & Leal, A. (2008). Función de la subtitulación y la interpretación de la imagen en la comprensión de los mensajes televisivos: la comprensión de una serie por parte de los adolescentes sordos. [The captioning function and image interpretation in understanding television content: Deaf adolescents' understanding of a television series]. Cultura y Educación, 20(1), 8193.Google Scholar
Calvert., S. L. (1999). The form of thought. In Siegel, I. E., ed. Development of mental representation: Theories and applications. London: LEAGoogle Scholar
Collins, W. A. (1983). Interpretation and inference in children's television viewing. In Bryant, J., & Anderson, D. R., eds. Children's understanding of TV: Research on attention and comprehension. San Diego, CA: Academia Press.Google Scholar
Denis, M. (1979). Las imágenes mentales. Madrid: S. XXIGoogle Scholar
Dyck, M., & Denver, E. (2003). Can the emotion recognition ability of deaf children be enhanced? A pilot study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(3), 348356.Google Scholar
Dyck, M., Farrugia, C., Shochet, I., & Holmes-Brown, M. (2004). Emotion recognition ability in deaf or blind children: do sounds, sights, or words make the difference?. Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 789800.Google Scholar
Dye, M. W. G., Baril, D. E., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Which aspects of visual attention are changed by deafness? The case of the attentional network test. Neuropsychologia, 45, 18011811.Google Scholar
Dye, M. W. G., Hauser, P. C., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Visual attention in deaf children and adults. In Marschark, M. & Hauser, P. C. eds. Deaf cognition: Foundations and outcomes (pp. 250263) New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Russell, P.A., Scott, C., & Hunter, N. (1998). The matching of facial expressions by deaf and hearing children and their production and comprehension of emotion labels. Motivation and Emotion, 22(4), 293313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keetay, V. (1995). The effect of visual distractions on speech perception in children. The Volta Review, 98(2), 4354.Google Scholar
Kusche, C. A., Garfield, T. S., & Greenberg, M. T. (1983). The understanding of emotional and social attributions in deaf adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 12(2), 153160.Google Scholar
Leal, A. (2006). Simbolización gráfica: medio de expresión e instrumento del pensamiento. [Graphic symbolization: a means of expression and instrument of thought]. Infancia y Aprendizaje 29(2), 203218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martí, E. (2003). Representar el mundo externamente. [Representing the outside world]. Madrid: Antonio Machado.Google Scholar
Pasquier, D. (1999). La culture des sentiments. [Culture of feelings]. París: Maison des Sciences de l'Homme.Google Scholar
Quittner, A. L., Smith, L. B., Osberger, M. J., Mitchell, T. V., & Katz, D. B. (1994). The impact of audition on the development of visual attention. Psychological Science, 5(6), 347353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvestre, N., & Cambra, C. (2009). Graphic representation and language: Drawings in deaf students aged three to five. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24(1), 315.Google Scholar
Torres, E., Conde, E., & Ruiz, C. (2002). Desarrollo humano en la sociedad audiovisual. [Human development in the audiovisual society]. Madrid: Alianza Psicología.Google Scholar