Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T19:07:39.916Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bitter taste sensitivity and frequency of bitter food intake in healthy Australian adults: a cross-sectional, mixed-methods study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2024

S. Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Dietetics & Food, Monash University, Notting Hill, Australia
L. Hassan
Affiliation:
Department of Dietetics, Nutrition and Sport, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Australia
A. Turner
Affiliation:
General Practice Clinical Unit, Faculty of Medicine, University of Queensland, Australia
L. Newman
Affiliation:
School of Science, STEM College, RMIT University, Bundoora, Australia
J. Danaher
Affiliation:
School of Science, STEM College, RMIT University, Bundoora, Australia
J. Biesiekierski
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Dietetics & Food, Monash University, Notting Hill, Australia Department of Dietetics, Nutrition and Sport, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Australia
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Bitter taste perception plays a dual role in human nutrition and evolutionary biology; being identifiable in nutrient-dense foods such as cruciferous vegetables and historically signalled toxic compounds. The TAS2R38 gene, part of the taste 2 receptor family, is central to individual differences in bitter taste perception(1). While genetic variations are influential, dietary habits and food preparation also impact taste perception. However, research investigating the interplay between these factors and genetic variations in influencing bitter taste sensitivity and food intake is limited. This study aimed to elucidate the relationship between bitter taste sensitivity and TAS2R38 haplotype variations in the context of bitter food consumption among Australian adults. A cross-sectional, mixed-methods study was conducted. Healthy adults who had maintained a stable diet for at least three months were eligible. Data collection was via an online survey (REDCap), capturing self-reported demographics, dietary patterns specific to bitter foods including metrics of bitter food avoidance, frequency, liking and perceived healthfulness, alongside a Dietary Quality Index (DQI) derived from a food frequency questionnaire(2). Bitter taste sensitivity was assessed using self-reported intensity perceptions of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) taste strips(3). Genotyping was conducted via TaqMan qPCR assays on DNA extracted from buccal swabs to ascertain TAS2R38 haplotypes. Data analysis utilised Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) and regression models, with all tests adjusted for confounding variables such as gender, age, and smoking status. A total of 222 participants (47.5 ± 17.7 years; 86% female; BMI 27.3 ± 7.1 kg/m2) completed the study. PROP sensitivity was strongly correlated with TAS2R38 haplotype, with supertasters predominantly having PAV/PAV, medium tasters with PAV/AVI, and non-tasters with AVI/AVI (p = 0.002). However, no relationship was observed between PROP sensitivity and either the frequency, liking, or avoidance of bitter foods (p>0.05). DQI was significantly related to bitter food consumption; individuals in the lowest DQI quintile consumed bitter foods more frequently than those in the third (p = 0.007) and top quintiles (p = 0.001). The perceived healthfulness of bitter foods was significantly higher in those with AVI/AVI haplotypes (non-tasters) compared to those with PAV/AVI (medium tasters) (p = 0.001). Counterintuitively, participants who reported greater enjoyment of bitter tastes consumed bitter foods less frequently (p<0.001). Our study confirms that TAS2R38 variants are predictive of PROP taste sensitivity, consistent with literature that identifies PAV/PAV individuals as supertasters. However, neither PROP sensitivity nor TAS2R38 haplotype influenced bitter food frequency or preference consumption patterns. Interestingly, those with lower Dietary Quality Index scores and less enjoyment of bitter taste consumed bitter foods more often. These observations highlight the need to investigate other factors influencing bitter food intake, such as additional sensory characteristics or psychological and behavioural aspects.

Type
Abstract
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

References

Beckett, EL, Martin, C, Yates, Z et al. (2014) Food & Function 5, 30403054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milte, CM, Thorpe, MG, Crawford, D et al. (2015) Exp Gerontol 64, 816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tepper, BJ (2008) Annu Rev Nutr 28, 367–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar