Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:36:57.518Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Review of Campbell and McGrath and Kelly

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2016

Robert E. Rosenwein*
Affiliation:
Lehigh University, Price Hall, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania 18015
Get access

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © Association for Politics and the Life Sciences 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chappell, E. D. (1940). “Measuring Human Relations: An Introduction to the Study of the Interaction of Individuals.” Genetic Psychology Monographs 22: 3147.Google Scholar
Chappell, E. D. (1953). “The Standard Experimental (Stress) Interview as Used in Interaction Chronograph Investigations.” Human Organization 12: 2332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chappell, E. D., and Coon, C. S. (1942). Principles of Anthropology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S. (1979). “Neonatal Entrainment and Enculturation.” In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S. (1980). “The Relation of Interactional Synchrony to Cognitive and Emotional Processes.” In Key, M. R. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication. New York: Mouton Publishers.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S., and Sander, L. W. (1974). “Neonate Movement is Synchronized with Adult Speech: Interactional Participation and Language Acquisition.” Science 183: 99101.Google Scholar
Gatewood, J. B., and Rosenwein, R. E. (1981). “Interactional Synchrony—Genuine or Spurious?Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 6: 1229.Google Scholar