We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Chappell, E. D. (1940). “Measuring Human Relations: An Introduction to the Study of the Interaction of Individuals.” Genetic Psychology Monographs22: 3–147.Google Scholar
Chappell, E. D. (1953). “The Standard Experimental (Stress) Interview as Used in Interaction Chronograph Investigations.” Human Organization12: 23–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chappell, E. D., and Coon, C. S. (1942). Principles of Anthropology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S. (1979). “Neonatal Entrainment and Enculturation.” In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S. (1980). “The Relation of Interactional Synchrony to Cognitive and Emotional Processes.” In Key, M. R. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication. New York: Mouton Publishers.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S., and Sander, L. W. (1974). “Neonate Movement is Synchronized with Adult Speech: Interactional Participation and Language Acquisition.” Science183: 99–101.Google Scholar
Gatewood, J. B., and Rosenwein, R. E. (1981). “Interactional Synchrony—Genuine or Spurious?” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior6: 12–29.Google Scholar