Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T17:16:43.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Drugs and Personality

III. The Effects of Stimulant and Depressant Drugs on Visual After-Effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

H. J. Eysenck
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London
H. Holland
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London
D. S. Trouton
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London

Extract

It is a well-known principle in psychology that the effects of sensory stimulation do not cease when the stimulus itself ceases. The so-called visual after-images are probably the best known of these after-effects, but almost equally well known are those induced by a rotating spiral. If a normal person fixates the centre of the rotating spiral for a period of time, and if then the motion of this spiral is suddenly stopped, an after-sensation of movement is well nigh universal. This after-sensation is opposite to the original motion in direction, just as the after-image produced by the fixation of a colour is usually the complementary colour to that which caused the original perception. There is little doubt that while peripheral factors play some part in these after-effects, central features also play a part, and it becomes tempting, therefore, to use phenomena of this type in attempts to verify or disprove the drug action hypotheses advanced by one of us in a previous paper. This hypothesis stated that stimulant drugs increased excitatory potential and decreased inhibitory potential, while depressant drugs decreased excitatory potential and increased inhibitory potential. The question now arises as to how this general postulate can be applied to the phenomena under discussion.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1957 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1 Eysenck, H. J., “A dynamic theory of anxiety and hysteria”, J. Ment. Sci., 1955, 101, 2851.Google Scholar
2 Idem , “Drugs and personality: I. Theory and methodology”, J. Ment. Sci., 1957, 103, 119131.Google Scholar
3 Idem , Casey, S., and Trouton, D., “Drugs and personality: II. The effects of stimulant and depressant drugs on continuous work”, J. Ment. Sci., 1957, 103, 645.Google Scholar
4 Eysenck, H. J., The dynamics of anxiety and hysteria, 1957. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
5 Gallese, A. J., “Spiral after-effect as a test of organic brain damage”, J. clin. Psychol., 1956, 12, 254258.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6 Klein, G. S., and Krech, D., “Cortical conductivity in the brain injured”, J. Personality, 1952, 21, 118148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Kohler, W., and Wallach, H., “Figurai after-effects”, Proc. Amer. Phil. Soc., 1944, 88, 265357.Google Scholar
8 Osgood, C. E., Method and theory in experimental psychology, 1953. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
9 Popov, Catherine, Contribution a l'étude du mecanisme d'èlaboration des connexions corticales, 1955. Paris: Parnasse.Google Scholar
10 Price, A. C., and Deabler, H. L., “Diagnosis of organicity by means of the spiral after-effect”, J. consult. Psychol, 1955, 19, 299302.Google Scholar
11 Wohlglmuth, A., “On the after-effect of seen movement”, Brit. J. Psychol., Monogr. Suppl., 1911, 1.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.