Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T08:22:39.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Greater affective network maturity is associated with better clinical outcomes in women with early sexual trauma

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2023

Olena Kleshchova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada Reno, Reno, NV, USA
Jack Grinband
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
Mariann R. Weierich*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada Reno, Reno, NV, USA
*
Corresponding author: Mariann R. Weierich; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Early-life adversity accelerates the maturation of affect-related circuitry, which might be a short-term adaptation with long-term tradeoffs. Sexual trauma is associated with a particularly strong impact on pubertal development and mental health outcomes. Our objective was to test the relations between trauma type, affective network maturity, and mental health outcomes in young women with trauma history. Trauma-exposed women aged 18–29 completed a clinical interview (n = 35) and an fMRI scan (n = 28). We used a public data set to train a machine learning algorithm to predict age from resting-state affective network connectivity and calculated network maturity as the difference between predicted and true age. We also performed principal component analysis on mental health outcomes and retained two components: clinical and state psychological outcomes. Compared to nonsexual trauma (n = 17), sexual trauma (n = 11) was associated with greater affective network maturity. In addition, for sexual trauma only, greater affective network maturity was associated with better clinical but not state psychological outcomes. These results suggest that sexual trauma during development might uniquely alter the maturational trajectory of affect-related circuitry, with distinct mental health consequences in emerging adulthood. Whereas delayed affective network maturation is associated with adverse clinical outcomes, accelerated affective network maturation might confer resilience in survivors.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Traumatic stress during sensitive periods of development is a risk factor for mental health problems in adulthood (e.g., Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Hoppen & Chalder, Reference Hoppen and Chalder2018). Compared to other types of adversity, early-life sexual trauma is associated with higher risk and symptom severity across a range of diagnostic domains, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, anxiety, and substance use (e.g., Dworkin et al., Reference Dworkin, Menon, Bystrynski and Allen2017; Molnar et al., Reference Molnar, Buka and Kessler2001; O’Brien et al., Reference O’Brien, White, Wu and Killian-Farrell2016). Disrupted brain development is a potential mechanism that might account for the relation between early-life trauma and adverse mental health outcomes (e.g., Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Bick & Nelson, Reference Bick and Nelson2016). Further, trauma type, and in particular sexual trauma, might moderate this relation by exerting circuit-specific effects on neurodevelopment (e.g., Cohodes et al., Reference Cohodes, Kitt, Baskin-Sommers and Gee2020).

Growing evidence suggests that early-life adversity accelerates development and sexual maturation, particularly in girls, as indexed by earlier pubertal onset (Gur et al., Reference Gur, Moore, Rosen, Barzilay, Roalf, Calkins, Ruparel, Scott, Almasy, Satterthwaite, Shinohara and Gur2019), younger age at menarche (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Platt, Keyes, Sumner, Allen and McLaughlin2019), and accelerated cellular aging (e.g., Sumner et al., Reference Sumner, Colich, Uddin, Armstrong and McLaughlin2019). The developmental effects of early-life adversity depend on the type of adversity, such that threat-related adversity (e.g., sexual abuse, violence) is associated with accelerated pubertal development and cellular aging, whereas deprivation-related adversity (e.g., neglect) is not (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020). Compared to other trauma types, early-life sexual trauma appears to have an especially strong association with abnormally advanced pubertal development (e.g., Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Leve, Van Ryzin and Natsuaki2014, Reference Mendle, Ryan and McKone2016; Noll et al., Reference Noll, Trickett, Long, Negriff, Susman, Shalev, Li and Putnam2017). Accelerated maturation in turn predicts a range of mental health problems in adolescence, such as internalizing symptoms (Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Leve, Van Ryzin and Natsuaki2014), and mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders (e.g., Platt et al., Reference Platt, Colich, McLaughlin, Gary and Keyes2017). Moreover, accelerated maturation, as indexed by earlier menarche and advanced cellular aging, mediates the relation between early-life adversity and later psychiatric symptoms (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Platt, Keyes, Sumner, Allen and McLaughlin2019; Sumner et al., Reference Sumner, Colich, Uddin, Armstrong and McLaughlin2019).

In addition to advancing pubertal development, early-life trauma might alter the developmental trajectory of brain maturation. For example, adolescent girls who have been exposed to childhood trauma (Gur et al., Reference Gur, Moore, Rosen, Barzilay, Roalf, Calkins, Ruparel, Scott, Almasy, Satterthwaite, Shinohara and Gur2019) or physical neglect (Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021) show greater structural maturity of the brain as a whole compared to non-trauma-exposed peers. Furthermore, the relation between early-life adversity and greater structural brain maturity is particularly strong among adolescents exposed to threat-related adversity, such as sexual abuse, home violence, or emotional abuse (Drobinin et al., Reference Drobinin, Van Gestel, Helmick, Schmidt, Bowen and Uher2022). However, emerging longitudinal evidence points to complex and nonlinear effects of early-life adversity on neurodevelopment, such that initially accelerated brain maturation may be followed by delayed brain maturation later (Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Cropley, Zalesky, Vijayakumar, Allen and Whittle2021).

Although early-life adversity has been associated with accelerated global maturation of the brain as a whole, there is also growing evidence of circuit- and network-specific effects (e.g., Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020; Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021). Frontolimbic circuitry centered on the amygdala, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) might be particularly susceptible to the neurodevelopmental effects of early-life adversity, given its role in affective processing, threat detection, and stress response regulation (VanTieghem & Tottenham, Reference VanTieghem, Tottenham, Vermetten, Baker and Risbrough2017). For example, early maternal separation and low maternal care accelerate the maturation of the hippocampus (e.g., Bath et al., Reference Bath, Manzano-Nieves and Goodwill2016), amygdala (Ono et al., Reference Ono, Kikusui, Sasaki, Ichikawa, Mori and Murakami-Murofushi2008), and frontolimbic circuits (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Sullivan, Howell and Tottenham2014; Manzano Nieves et al., Reference Manzano Nieves, Bravo, Baskoylu and Bath2020) in non-human animals. Similarly, maternally deprived children have been shown to display a more mature pattern of amygdala-mPFC coupling during emotion processing (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys, Telzer, Hare, Bookheimer and Tottenham2013) and at rest (Herzberg et al., Reference Herzberg, McKenzie, Hodel, Hunt, Mueller, Gunnar and Thomas2021) compared to peer controls, although the reliability of this association and the use of task-based amygdala-mPFC coupling as an index of brain maturity has been called into question (Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, VanTieghem, Gabard‐Durnam, Gee, Flannery, Caldera, Goff, Telzer, Humphreys, Fareri, Shapiro, Algharazi, Bolger, Aly and Tottenham2022; Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020) Childhood adversity also has been associated with accelerated structural maturation of the frontolimbic circuitry in adolescence, as indexed by lesser gray matter volumes, thinner cortex, greater white matter integrity, and greater longitudinal reductions in gray matter volume spanning prefrontal, insular, medial temporal, and subcortical limbic regions (e.g., Busso et al., Reference Busso, McLaughlin, Brueck, Peverill, Gold and Sheridan2017; Gur et al., Reference Gur, Moore, Rosen, Barzilay, Roalf, Calkins, Ruparel, Scott, Almasy, Satterthwaite, Shinohara and Gur2019; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Viding, Wallace, Schaer, De Brito, Robustelli and McCrory2013; Tyborowska et al., Reference Tyborowska, Volman, Niermann, Pouwels, Smeekens, Cillessen, Toni and Roelofs2018). In addition, early-life adversity has been associated with accelerated functional maturation of frontolimbic circuitry in young adolescents (Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, Sisk and Gee2021; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Collins and Luciana2022). Furthermore, accelerated frontolimbic maturation has been specifically linked to threat-related adversity (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020). Despite much evidence in support of accelerated maturation, some studies have shown delayed maturation of affective circuitry in trauma-exposed youths (e.g., Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Yücel, Lubman, Pantelis and Allen2013). This apparent discrepancy in the literature might point to nonlinear effects of early-life adversity on the neurodevelopmental trajectory. For example, a recent longitudinal study showed that neighborhood disadvantage is associated with accelerated brain maturation in early adolescence followed by delayed maturation in late adolescence in at-risk youths (e.g., Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Cropley, Zalesky, Vijayakumar, Allen and Whittle2021).

Differences in the neurodevelopmental effects of early-life trauma (i.e., acceleration or delay) could also underlie different functional outcomes and thus reflect vulnerability or resilience. Earlier frontolimbic maturation has been proposed to have adaptative value in harsh early-life environments by promoting earlier emergence of affect-related functions and stress-regulatory behaviors (Callaghan & Tottenham, Reference Callaghan and Tottenham2016). For example, accelerated limbic maturation is associated with earlier development of stress reactivity and fear learning in non-human animals (e.g., Bath et al., Reference Bath, Manzano-Nieves and Goodwill2016; Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Sullivan, Howell and Tottenham2014). Similarly, a more mature pattern of amygdala-mPFC coupling during emotion processing has been associated with less separation anxiety in maternally deprived children (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys, Telzer, Hare, Bookheimer and Tottenham2013), although recent research has not replicated this association in more normative samples (e.g., Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, VanTieghem, Gabard‐Durnam, Gee, Flannery, Caldera, Goff, Telzer, Humphreys, Fareri, Shapiro, Algharazi, Bolger, Aly and Tottenham2022). Additionally, greater functional maturity of resting corticolimbic connectivity in adolescents has been shown to mediate the relation between early-life adversity and less internalizing symptoms two years later (Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, Sisk and Gee2021). Consistent with the hypothesized adaptive value of accelerated maturation, delayed frontolimbic maturation has been associated with psychiatric symptoms (e.g., Albaugh et al., Reference Albaugh, Ducharme, Karama, Watts, Lewis, Orr, Nguyen, Mckinstry, Botteron, Evans and Hudziak2017; Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Albaugh, Hudziak, Botteron, Nguyen, Truong, Evans, Karama, Ball, Byars, Schapiro, Bommer, Carr, German, Dunn, Rivkin, Waber, Mulkern, Vajapeyam and OʼNeill2014; Vanes et al., Reference Vanes, Moutoussis, Ziegler, Goodyer, Fonagy, Jones, Bullmore and Dolan2020), particularly in trauma-exposed youths (e.g., Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Yücel, Lubman, Pantelis and Allen2013). Furthermore, slower development of frontal and limbic structures has been shown to mediate the relation between early-life adversity and poor outcomes in adolescents (e.g., Barch et al., Reference Barch, Donohue, Elsayed, Gilbert, Harms, Hennefield, Herzberg, Kandala, Karcher, Jackson, Luking, Rappaport, Sanders, Taylor, Tillman, Vogel, Whalen and Luby2022; Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Hariri and Williamson2015; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Vijayakumar, Dennison, Schwartz, Simmons, Sheeber, Allen and Rosenfeld2016).

Accelerated pubertal development and frontolimbic maturation might constitute a short-term adaptation with long-term mental health consequences (e.g., Belsky and Shalev, Reference Belsky and Shalev2016; Callaghan & Tottenham, Reference Callaghan and Tottenham2016). For example, earlier pubertal onset has been associated with psychiatric symptoms in adolescence (e.g., Colich et al., Reference Colich, Platt, Keyes, Sumner, Allen and McLaughlin2019; Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Leve, Van Ryzin and Natsuaki2014; Platt et al., Reference Platt, Colich, McLaughlin, Gary and Keyes2017). However, the precise relationship between altered development and short-term vs. long-term outcomes remains unclear given prior reports of both positive and negative mental health outcomes of accelerated maturation. One possibility is that negative outcomes of early-life trauma might be mediated by accelerated sexual maturation and biological aging (e.g., Colich et al., Reference Colich, Platt, Keyes, Sumner, Allen and McLaughlin2019; Sumner et al., Reference Sumner, Colich, Uddin, Armstrong and McLaughlin2019), whereas positive outcomes might be mediated by selectively accelerated maturation of neural circuitry involved in affective processing and stress regulation (e.g., Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, Sisk and Gee2021). An alternative explanation supported by emerging evidence suggests that the mental health tradeoffs of accelerated brain maturation in early adolescence might result from a subsequent deceleration of brain maturation in late adolescence (e.g., Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Cropley, Zalesky, Vijayakumar, Allen and Whittle2021). For example, a prospective study of youths with a family history of mood disorders showed that the risk of developing a mood disorder was associated with delayed brain maturation during late adolescence and emerging adulthood (de Nooij et al., Reference de Nooij, Harris, Hawkins, Shen, Clarke, Chan, Ziermans, McIntosh and Whalley2020).

Affect-related frontolimbic circuits follow protracted maturational trajectories well into adulthood, as indexed by age-related changes in synaptic density (Petanjek et al., Reference Petanjek, Judaš, Šimić, Rašin, Uylings, Rakic and Kostović2011), subcortical gray matter volume (Narvacan et al., Reference Narvacan, Treit, Camicioli, Martin and Beaulieu2017), white matter structure (Lebel et al., Reference Lebel, Gee, Camicioli, Wieler, Martin and Beaulieu2012), intracortical myelination (Grydeland et al., Reference Grydeland, Walhovd, Tamnes, Westlye and Fjell2013), and functional connectivity (Gabard-Durnam et al., Reference Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Gee, Humphreys, Telzer, Hare and Tottenham2014). Due to this prolonged immaturity and a high concentration of receptors for various stress mediators (e.g., Joëls & Baram, Reference Joëls and Baram2009), frontolimbic circuitry might be particularly sensitive to traumatic stress not only in childhood and adolescence but also in early adulthood. However, as research on posttrauma maturation of affect-related circuitry beyond childhood and adolescence is still lacking, the relation between altered frontolimbic development and later mental health outcomes remains unclear.

The importance of clarifying the relation between trauma-related alterations in frontolimbic maturity and mental health outcomes in emerging adults is threefold. First, studies that focus on trauma-exposed children and adolescents miss the delayed effects of early-life trauma on mental health, including adult-onset psychopathology (e.g., Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010). Conversely, assessment of mental health outcomes in young adulthood permits stronger conclusions about vulnerability vs. resilience to adverse outcomes of early-life trauma. Second, because frontolimbic circuits remain malleable into early adulthood (e.g., Petanjek et al., Reference Petanjek, Judaš, Šimić, Rašin, Uylings, Rakic and Kostović2011), temporary deviations from normative maturity in childhood or adolescence that eventually normalize are unlikely to account for adverse mental health outcomes in adulthood. Finally, emerging research suggests that the neurodevelopmental alterations of early-life trauma and their functional significance are age-dependent (Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Cropley, Zalesky, Vijayakumar, Allen and Whittle2021), such that potentially beneficial early acceleration might be followed by potentially detrimental late deceleration of maturation. Thus, it is important to characterize the trauma-related effects on brain maturation in emerging adulthood separately from earlier stages of development.

Trauma exposure during sensitive periods of frontolimbic development might interact with the maturational trajectories of affect-related circuits to produce distinct mental health outcomes later in life (e.g., Gee & Casey, Reference Gee and Casey2015). Of the various types of childhood adversity, sexual trauma has been robustly associated with stronger impact on pubertal maturation (e.g., Noll et al., Reference Noll, Trickett, Long, Negriff, Susman, Shalev, Li and Putnam2017) and poorer mental health outcomes (e.g., Dworkin et al., Reference Dworkin, Menon, Bystrynski and Allen2017), however the effect of sexual trauma on the maturation of affect-related circuitry remains unclear. Early-life sexual trauma is thought to be distinct from other trauma types in that it represents a particularly damaging combination of traumatic sexualization, betrayal, powerlessness, and stigmatization (Finkelhor & Browne, Reference Finkelhor and Browne1985), which together exert profound effects on survivors’ affective function and emotion regulation abilities. Emotion dysregulation in turn mediates the relation between sexual trauma and psychopathology (e.g., Raudales et al., Reference Raudales, Short and Schmidt2019). Given the particular vulnerability associated with early-life sexual trauma, the impact of trauma on the maturation of affect-related circuitry might be especially pronounced in survivors of sexual trauma compared to other types of adversity and might account for vulnerability or resilience to adverse mental health outcomes.

To investigate this question, we used a machine-learning-based approach to measuring functional maturity of affect-related brain circuits. Our first objective was to test differences in the functional maturity of affective networks in young women who had been exposed to either sexual or nonsexual trauma prior to study enrollment (i.e., during childhood, adolescence, or emerging adulthood), which we refer to as early-life trauma. Given the absence of a universally accepted definition of “early life” and the growing evidence of protracted brain maturation well into young adulthood (e.g., Grydeland et al., Reference Grydeland, Walhovd, Tamnes, Westlye and Fjell2013; Lebel et al., Reference Lebel, Gee, Camicioli, Wieler, Martin and Beaulieu2012), here we define early-life trauma as traumatic stress during the first three decades of life when the brain continues to undergo structural and functional maturation. Our second objective was to quantify the relation between affective network maturity and trauma-related mental health outcomes in emerging adulthood. We used a public neuroimaging data set of healthy no-trauma controls ages 6–45 to train a machine learning algorithm to predict age based on resting-state connectivity of affect-related networks (orbito-affective and cingulo-opercular). We then used the trained algorithm to predict age from functional affective network connectivity in a separate sample of trauma-exposed women ages 18–29 with a history of either sexual or nonsexual trauma.

We hypothesized that, compared to nonsexual trauma, sexual trauma would be associated with greater functional affective network maturity, as indexed by greater difference between predicted age and true age. Such a result would be consistent with accelerated frontolimbic maturation following early-life sexual trauma. Given the well-documented differences in the maturational trajectories of different brain circuits (e.g., Grydeland et al., Reference Grydeland, Walhovd, Tamnes, Westlye and Fjell2013; Lebel et al., Reference Lebel, Gee, Camicioli, Wieler, Martin and Beaulieu2012), the calculated discrepancy between predicted and true age should not be interpreted as an index of global brain maturity, but rather as an index of network-specific maturity (i.e., the functional maturity of affect-related circuitry). We also hypothesized that affective network maturity would be associated with mental health outcomes, which we derived by performing dimensionality reduction on eight measures of trauma-related outcomes. A negative association between affective network maturity and adverse outcomes would suggest that greater affective network maturity has adaptive value even in emerging adulthood. Conversely, a positive association would be consistent with mental health tradeoffs of accelerated affective network maturation. Finally, we hypothesized that trauma type would moderate the relation between affective network maturity and mental health outcomes, such that the association between affective network maturity and outcomes would be more pronounced in participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma.

Materials and methods

Participants

We recruited 36 trauma-exposed women ages 18–29 (M = 21.2, SD = 2.7) from an urban university in the northeastern United States based on their responses to the Life Events Checklist, a self-report screen for potential trauma exposure (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Litz, Hsu and Lombardo2004). We verified trauma exposure during a structured clinical interview. Participants were excluded if they had a history of head trauma, had a history of a developmental or neurological disorder, or had MRI contraindications.

Procedure

Participants completed two study sessions: a lab visit, which included a clinical interview and questionnaires, and an MRI scan. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board and conducted in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Clinical interview

Following informed consent, participants completed a clinical interview that included the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS-5; Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Bovin, Lee, Sloan, Schnurr, Kaloupek, Keane and Marx2018) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders (SCID; First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams2002a). We used the CAPS to assess trauma exposure and PTSD symptoms and the SCID to assess lifetime history of major psychiatric disorders. Trauma exposure was defined according to Criterion A of the CAPS.

Questionnaires

After the interview, participants completed “state” psychological measures; although the timeframe for each measure varied slightly, we conceptualized the responses to each to reflect the participant’s general state during participation, and we distinguished between these phenomena and clinical outcomes. The measures were the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS, Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983), the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II, Beck et al., Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-State Version (STAI-S, Spielberger et al., Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983), and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS, Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). The PSS is a 10-item measure of the degree to which life events over the past month are perceived as stressful, uncontrollable, and unpredictable. The BDI-II is a 21-item measure of depressed mood over the past two weeks. The STAI-S is a 20-item measure of state anxiety. The PANAS is a 20-item measure of state negative and positive affect.

MRI scan

The MRI session included 13 minutes of structural scans followed by a 7-min resting scan during which the participants were instructed to fixate on a white cross on a black background and let their minds wander. Imaging data were acquired on a Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3T MRI scanner using a 64-channel gradient head coil. We acquired T1-weighted (T1w) images using the MPRAGE sequence (TR/TE/flip angle = 2.4 s/2.28 ms/8°; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; voxel size = 0.8 mm3) and resting-state functional data using multiband image acquisition and a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging T2*-weighted sequence (TR/TE/flip angle = 995 ms/34 ms/52°; FOV = 208 × 208 mm; voxel size = 2 mm3; MB = 6; 425 volumes).

Training data set for machine learning

To build a normative model of affective network maturation, we used the enhanced Nathan Kline Institute-Rockland Sample (NKI-RS; Nooner et al., Reference Nooner, Colcombe, Tobe, Mennes, Benedict, Moreno, Panek, Brown, Zavitz, Li, Sikka, Gutman, Bangaru, Schlachter, Kamiel, Anwar, Hinz, Kaplan, Rachlin and Milham2012) as the training data set for machine learning. Our goal was to build a sex-specific normative model of functional maturation of the affective networks from middle childhood through middle adulthood in the absence of prior trauma exposure or clinical history. To this end, we selected the normative sample using the following inclusion criteria: female, age 6–45, right-handed, no neurological, developmental, or psychiatric disorders, and no prior history of trauma. These stringent selection criteria were used to minimize known sources of variability in functional connectivity that are unrelated to age, such as sex, handedness, psychiatric history, and trauma exposure. Our choice of a wider age range for the training data set compared to the test data set was motivated by both conceptual and practical considerations. First, because the degree of the hypothesized maturation acceleration or delay was unclear, we chose relatively wide “safety margins” (±12–16 years) to avoid underestimating any deviations from the normative trajectory. In addition, our wide age range was meant to increase the model’s prediction accuracy for the age group of interest (age 18–29) by exposing the machine learning algorithm to relative extremes of the brain age spectrum relative to the age group of interest (i.e., middle childhood and middle adulthood). Finally, by including a wider age range, we were able to not only increase the sample size but also increase age-related variability while decreasing age-unrelated variability through stringent exclusion criteria.

Clinical history. In the NKI-RS sample, trauma history was assessed using the University of California at Los Angeles Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index (Pynoos et al., Reference Pynoos, Rodriguez, Steinberg, Stuber and Frederick1998). Lifetime neurological and psychiatric history was assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, R., Gibbon and Williams2002b) and the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Green, Adler, Barkley, Chatterji, Faraone, Finkelman, Greenhill, Gruber, Jewell, Russo, Sampson and Van Brunt2010) for participants above age 17 and the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for participants under age 18 (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997).

Neuroimaging data. NKI-RS imaging data were acquired on a Siemens MAGNETOM Trio Tim 3T MRI scanner using a 32-channel gradient head coil. We used the selected participants’ T1w MPRAGE images (TR/TE/flip angle = 1.9 s/2.52 ms/9°; FOV = 250 × 250 mm; voxel size = 1 mm3) and the 9.5-min resting-state scans which were acquired using multiband image acquisition and a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging T2*-weighted sequence (TR/TE/flip angle = 1.4 s/30 ms/65°; FOV = 224 × 224 mm; voxel size = 2 mm3; MB = 4; 404 volumes). During the resting scan, NKI-RS participants were instructed to fixate on a white cross on a black background.

Data preparation

Neuroimaging data preprocessing

Usable MRI data were available for 85 healthy no-trauma controls in the training data set (NKI-RS sample) and 29 trauma-exposed participants in the test data set. The training and test data sets had comparable neuroimaging parameters and therefore were preprocessed similarly (see Supplementary Information, SI). Briefly, anatomical and functional scans were preprocessed using fMRIPrep (Esteban et al., Reference Esteban, Markiewicz, Blair, Moodie, Isik, Erramuzpe, Kent, Goncalves, DuPre, Snyder, Oya, Ghosh, Wright, Durnez, Poldrack and Gorgolewski2019) and converted to the Connectivity Informatics Technology Initiative format using the fmriprep_ciftify BIDS-app (Dickie et al., Reference Dickie, Anticevic, Smith, Coalson, Manogaran, Calarco, Viviano, Glasser, Van Essen and Voineskos2019). All subsequent denoising operations and analyses were performed on the preprocessed Connectivity Informatics Technology Initiative data.

Denoising of resting-state functional data

To minimize the influence of head motion on estimates of functional connectivity and brain maturity, we excluded high-motion participants and performed nuisance regression and high-pass filtering (see SI). After exclusion of 6 high-motion participants from the training data set (see SI for exclusion criteria), the final sample included 79 participants (age 6–45, M = 23.2, SD = 11.8). No participants were excluded from the test data set.

Functional connectivity estimation

All functional connectivity estimation operations were performed on the cleaned and filtered functional data using Connectome Workbench and custom Matlab code (see SI). Briefly, we used the Cole-Anticevic Brain-wide Network Partition (Ji et al., Reference Ji, Spronk, Kulkarni, Repovš, Anticevic and Cole2019) to parcellate the brain into 718 regions of interest (ROIs) and excluded ROIs with group median temporal signal-to-noise ratio of <100. We calculated correlation coefficients between mean time series from each pair of the remaining 567 ROIs and performed Fisher’s r-to-Z transformation. To minimize the effects of residual motion on functional connectivity, we excluded edges that were correlated with head motion at uncorrected p < .05 (39%) from all subsequent analyses. The features that were used to train the final model are presented in SI.

Data analysis

Affective network maturity estimation

All machine learning operations were performed in Matlab R2019b using the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox and the Regression Learner App. We used support vector regression (SVR; Smola and Schölkopf, Reference Smola and Schölkopf2004; Vapnik, Reference Vapnik1995) to train the normative model of affective network maturation using the selected NKI-RS participants as the training data set (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Nardos, Cohen, Fair, Power, Church, Nelson, Wig, Vogel, Lessov-Schlaggar, Barnes, Dubis, Feczko, Coalson, Pruett, Barch, Petersen and Schlaggar2010; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Greene, Gratton, Dosenbach, Petersen and Schlaggar2019) and the trauma-exposed participants as the test data set (see SI for details). Briefly, the affective network maturation model was trained on 500 features sampled from the cingulo-opercular and orbito-affective networks (Ji et al., Reference Ji, Spronk, Kulkarni, Repovš, Anticevic and Cole2019) based on the strength of their correlations with age. We performed leave-one-out cross-validation on the training sample to determine the accuracy of the trained SVR model and used permutation testing to determine whether the model’s performance was statistically different from chance. We then used the trained SVR model to predict the ages of trauma-exposed participants and calculated affective network maturity as the difference between predicted age and true chronological age.

The trained SVR model showed high accuracy (R 2 = .86, r = .93, RMSE = 4.6, MAE = 3.8) and was statistically significant at p < .001 in the training data set (Figure 1). The MAE for the test group as a whole was 2.5. The performance of our model is consistent with reported model performances in previous life span studies that used a range of machine learning algorithms to predict age based on various neuroimaging modalities and feature sets (Franke et al., Reference Franke, Bublak, Hoyer, Billiet, Gaser, Witte and Schwab2020). Although most prior studies used training data sets ranging from n = 122 (R 2 = .57; age range: 7–31; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Greene, Gratton, Dosenbach, Petersen and Schlaggar2019) to n = 2354 (R 2 = .87; age range: 19–82; Liem et al., Reference Liem, Varoquaux, Kynast, Beyer, Kharabian Masouleh, Huntenburg, Lampe, Rahim, Abraham, Craddock, Riedel-Heller, Luck, Loeffler, Schroeter, Witte, Villringer and Margulies2017), successful “brain age” prediction has been achieved with as few as 74 participants (R 2 = .46; age range 8–18: Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021), 63 participants (R 2 = .61; age range 18–45: Tian et al., Reference Tian, Ma and Wang2016), or even 44 participants (R 2 = .48; age range: 23–74; Neeb et al., Reference Neeb, Zilles and Shah2006) in the training data set. Despite our relatively small training data set, our trained model showed better performance compared to previous studies that used resting-state fMRI to predict age in more heterogenous mixed-gender samples and achieved R 2 in the range of .55–.61 (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Nardos, Cohen, Fair, Power, Church, Nelson, Wig, Vogel, Lessov-Schlaggar, Barnes, Dubis, Feczko, Coalson, Pruett, Barch, Petersen and Schlaggar2010; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Greene, Gratton, Dosenbach, Petersen and Schlaggar2019; Tian et al., Reference Tian, Ma and Wang2016).

Figure 1. Performance of the machine learning algorithm trained to predict age based on affective network connectivity (MAE = 3.8, RMSE = 4.6, R 2 = .86, r = .93, p < .001). Linear support vector regression was trained on 500 features sampled from the cingulo-opercular and orbito-affective networks (“affective networks”) based on the strength of their correlations with age. Affective network maturity was calculated as the difference between predicted age and true age (i.e., prediction errors).

We did not perform bias correction associated with regression to the mean, because prediction errors were not correlated with chronological age, r = −.33, p = .087, and because bias correction is inaccurate when the age ranges of the training and test samples are different, such as in our case (Liang et al., Reference Liang, Zhang and Niu2019). To test the degree to which regression to the mean may have influenced our results, we repeated our analyses of affective network maturity with chronological age as a covariate. Chronological age did not improve model fit and was not a statistically significant predictor.

Principal component analysis on mental health outcomes

To generate summary indices of mental health outcomes in trauma-exposed women, we performed dimensionality reduction on eight measures of psychological functioning and past and current clinical history. To estimate the amount of shared variance among different measures of mental health outcomes, we calculated Pearson’s correlations between perceived stress (PSS), depressed mood (BDI), state anxiety (STAI), negative affect (PANAS), PTSD symptom number and severity (CAPS), and number of current and past psychiatric diagnoses (SCID). The predominance of strong positive correlations among measures of mental health outcomes (Figure 2A) reflected a substantial amount of shared variance and warranted dimensionality reduction. We performed principal component analysis on the eight measures of mental health outcomes and retained the first two principal components with eigenvalues above 1, which together explained 72% of total variance (Figure 2B). After varimax rotation, self-reported “state” measures of perceived stress, depressed mood, anxiety, and negative affect loaded most strongly on the first rotated component (PC1), whereas number of current and past psychiatric diagnoses loaded most strongly on the second rotated component (PC2). Number and severity of PTSD symptoms loaded moderately on both components (Figure 2C). For each participant, we calculated individual PC1 scores (“state psychological outcomes”) and PC2 scores (“clinical outcomes”), which we used as two composite measures of mental health outcomes.

Figure 2. Principal component analysis on indices of mental health outcomes. Panel A: The high number of strong positive correlations among indices of mental health outcomes warranted dimensionality reduction. Panel B: The first two principal components together captured 72% of total variance. Panel C: The pattern of loadings of the original measures of mental health outcomes on the first two components after varimax rotation suggests that PC1 captures state psychological outcomes, whereas PC2 captures clinical outcomes.

Associations between trauma type, affective network maturity, and mental health outcomes

To determine the effect of trauma type on mental health outcomes, we tested group differences in PC1 scores (“state psychological outcomes”) and PC2 scores (“clinical outcomes”) between participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma. To determine the effect of trauma type on affective network development, we tested group differences in affective network maturity (age prediction errors) between participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma. To clarify the functional significance of affective network maturity, we tested associations between affective network maturity (age prediction errors) and mental health outcomes (PC1 and PC2 scores). Finally, we tested trauma type as a moderator of the relations between mental health outcomes and affective network maturity. To minimize the influence of any outliers, we used a robust general linear model with the bisquare weight function to test all associations and group differences. All statistical analyses were performed using Matlab R2019b and the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox.

Results

Descriptive data

Participant information is presented in Table 1. Thirteen participants (37%) endorsed sexual violence as their index trauma, 11 of whom had usable fMRI data. Twenty-two participants endorsed nonsexual trauma as their index trauma, 17 of whom had usable fMRI data. One participant did not provide the details of her trauma and was excluded from the analysis. Age at the earliest trauma ranged from 3 to 27 years, and time since the most recent trauma ranged from less than a year to 15 years. Participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma did not differ in age, Wilcoxon rank-sum test: Z = 0.81, p = .418, r = .14; number of traumatic events, Z = 0.15, p = .883, r = .02; age at the earliest trauma, Z = 1.47, p = .141, r = .25; or time since the most recent trauma, Z = 0.86, p = .389, r = .15.

Table 1. Participants

Note. PSS = Perceived Stress Scale; STAI-S = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-State Version; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; NACM = negative alterations in cognitions and mood.

a Any major psychiatric diagnosis other than PTSD, as assessed using the SCID.

p < 0.1; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.

Participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma did not differ in perceived stress, Z = 0.86, p = .392, r = .14; depressed mood, Z = 0.79, p = .432, r = .13; negative state affect, Z = 0.55, p = .583, r = .09; or positive state affect, Z = −1.06, p = .288, r = −.18. However, participants with sexual vs. nonsexual trauma showed a nonsignificant trend (medium effect size) towards greater state anxiety, Z = 1.90, p = .058, r = .32. Almost half (46%) of the participants with sexual trauma met criteria for a provisional PTSD diagnosis compared to almost one-third (27%) of the participants with nonsexual trauma. Although there was no difference in probability of a provisional PTSD diagnosis by trauma type, χ2 = 1.29, p = .256, participants with sexual trauma endorsed more, Z = 2.87, p = .004, r = .48, and more severe, Z = 2.00, p = .045, r = .34, PTSD symptoms compared to participants with nonsexual trauma. There were no differences by trauma type in number of current psychiatric diagnoses, Z = 0.74, p = .462, r = .12, or past psychiatric diagnoses, Z = 0.14, p = .888, r = .02. We repeated the above analyses in participants with imaging data and obtained a similar pattern of results, except the probability of a provisional PTSD diagnosis was now higher in the sexual trauma group, χ2 = 4.04, p = .044.

Participants with sexual trauma showed a nonsignificant trend towards poorer state psychological outcomes indexed by PC1 scores, compared to participants with nonsexual trauma, F(1,33) = 3.49, p = .071, R 2 = .10 (Figure 3). There was no group difference by trauma type in clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores, F(1,33) = 0.84, p = .365, R 2 = .03.

Figure 3. Trauma type and mental health outcomes. Compared to participants with a history of nonsexual trauma, participants with a history of sexual trauma showed a nonsignificant trend towards poorer state psychological outcomes indexed by PC1 scores (left panel) but no difference in clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores (right panel). Bars represent group means; error bars represent standard errors.

Trauma type, affective network maturity, and mental health outcomes

Participants with sexual trauma showed greater affective network maturity indexed by age prediction errors (M = 2.5, SD = 2.9, range: −3.6 to 5.9), compared to participants with nonsexual trauma (M = 0.6, SD = 2.7, range: −3.8 to 7.1), F(1,26) = 5.33, p = .029, R 2 = .17 (Figure 4A). Affective network maturity was not associated with either state psychological outcomes indexed by PC1 scores, F(1,26) = 0.41, p = .529, R 2 = .02, or with clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores, F(1,26) = 0.63, p = .433, R 2 = .03. However, trauma type moderated the relation between clinical outcomes (independent variable) and affective network maturity (dependent variable), F(1,24) = 4.44, p = .046, ηp 2 = .16. Poorer clinical outcomes were associated with less affective network maturity in participants with sexual trauma but not in participants with nonsexual trauma (Figure 4B). Trauma type did not moderate the relation between psychological outcomes and affective network maturity, F(1,24) = 0.21, p = .648, ηp 2 = .01.

Figure 4. Trauma type, affective network maturity, and clinical outcomes. Panel A: Compared to participants with a history of nonsexual trauma, participants with a history of sexual trauma showed greater affective network maturity indexed by age prediction errors. Panel B: Trauma type moderated the relation between affective network maturity and clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores. Less affective network maturity was associated with poorer clinical outcomes in participants with sexual trauma but not in participants with nonsexual trauma. Bars represent group means; error bars represent standard errors. The lines of best fit were generated using robust regression.

Discussion

In this proof-of-concept study, we used a machine learning approach to test atypical functional maturation of affect-related brain circuitry as a candidate mechanism that might account for the relation between exposure to early sexual trauma and adverse mental health outcomes in adulthood. Our first objective was to compare the functional maturity of affective networks in young women who were exposed to either sexual or nonsexual trauma during formative years of brain development (i.e., early-life sexual trauma). Consistent with our hypothesis, young women with early sexual trauma showed greater affective network maturity compared to women with nonsexual trauma. This result aligns with the previously documented relation between early-life adversity and accelerated maturation of affect-related frontolimbic circuitry (e.g., Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020; Gur et al., Reference Gur, Moore, Rosen, Barzilay, Roalf, Calkins, Ruparel, Scott, Almasy, Satterthwaite, Shinohara and Gur2019; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Collins and Luciana2022; Tyborowska et al., Reference Tyborowska, Volman, Niermann, Pouwels, Smeekens, Cillessen, Toni and Roelofs2018). However, our data also suggest that the effect of early traumatic stress on neurodevelopment depends on the type of trauma. In line with previous evidence that sexual trauma is uniquely capable of advancing pubertal development (e.g., Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Ryan and McKone2016; Noll et al., Reference Noll, Trickett, Long, Negriff, Susman, Shalev, Li and Putnam2017), our results suggest that sexual trauma also has a greater impact on frontolimbic maturation compared to other trauma types. These data also extend prior research in children and adolescents and show that the neurodevelopmental effects of traumatic stress are evident even in emerging adulthood, which is consistent with the protracted maturational trajectory of affect-related circuitry (e.g., Lebel et al., Reference Lebel, Gee, Camicioli, Wieler, Martin and Beaulieu2012; Narvacan et al., Reference Narvacan, Treit, Camicioli, Martin and Beaulieu2017). Together with prior literature, our results suggest that the developmental trajectory of affect-related circuitry remains sensitive to traumatic stress into emerging adulthood, and the effects of sexual trauma on affective network maturation can manifest beyond childhood and adolescence.

Our second objective was to test the functional significance of affective network maturity in relation to mental health outcomes in emerging adulthood. Counter to our hypothesis, affective network maturity was not associated with either state psychological or clinical outcomes in trauma-exposed women as a group. However, trauma type moderated the relation between affective network maturity and clinical outcomes. In women with sexual trauma, lesser affective network maturity was associated with poorer clinical outcomes, whereas there was no such relation in women with nonsexual trauma. In contrast, state psychological outcomes were not associated with affective network maturity in the trauma-exposed sample as a whole or in either trauma group separately. These results suggest that different types of trauma-related outcomes (i.e., state psychological vs. clinical) are mediated by different mechanisms and that subclinical levels of psychological distress are unrelated to the maturation of affect-related circuitry in early-trauma survivors.

The relation between greater affective network maturity and better clinical outcomes in sexual trauma survivors is consistent with the stress acceleration theory, which posits that accelerated maturation of affect-related circuitry is adaptive under conditions of early adversity (e.g., Callaghan & Tottenham, Reference Callaghan and Tottenham2016). Our results suggest that greater maturity of affect-related circuitry might reflect resilience or constitute a protective factor against adverse clinical outcomes in survivors of early-life sexual trauma. This result is consistent with prior evidence of greater frontolimbic maturity being protective against internalizing symptoms in adolescents with early-life adversity (e.g., Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, Sisk and Gee2021). In addition, given the robust literature documenting poorer clinical outcomes overall in sexual trauma survivors (e.g., Dworkin et al., Reference Dworkin, Menon, Bystrynski and Allen2017; O’Brien et al., Reference O’Brien, White, Wu and Killian-Farrell2016), the absence of a difference in clinical outcomes by trauma type might reflect a high prevalence of resilience in our sexual trauma group. This absence might be accounted for by greater affective network maturity in our sample of sexual trauma survivors.

Whereas accelerated affective network maturation might be adaptive, failure to accelerate maturation of affect-related circuitry following early sexual trauma might reflect a lack of adaptation, which might in turn confer vulnerability to adverse clinical outcomes. This interpretation is broadly consistent with previous evidence that delayed frontolimbic maturation is associated with psychiatric symptoms in adolescence (e.g., Albaugh et al., Reference Albaugh, Ducharme, Karama, Watts, Lewis, Orr, Nguyen, Mckinstry, Botteron, Evans and Hudziak2017; Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Albaugh, Hudziak, Botteron, Nguyen, Truong, Evans, Karama, Ball, Byars, Schapiro, Bommer, Carr, German, Dunn, Rivkin, Waber, Mulkern, Vajapeyam and OʼNeill2014; Keding et al., Reference Keding, Heyn, Russell, Zhu, Cisler, McLaughlin and Herringa2021; Vanes et al., Reference Vanes, Moutoussis, Ziegler, Goodyer, Fonagy, Jones, Bullmore and Dolan2020) and that slower development of frontal and limbic structures mediates the relation between early-life adversity and negative outcomes (e.g., Barch et al., Reference Barch, Donohue, Elsayed, Gilbert, Harms, Hennefield, Herzberg, Kandala, Karcher, Jackson, Luking, Rappaport, Sanders, Taylor, Tillman, Vogel, Whalen and Luby2022; Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Hariri and Williamson2015; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Vijayakumar, Dennison, Schwartz, Simmons, Sheeber, Allen and Rosenfeld2016). Our results are consistent with prior literature and suggest that early-life trauma can alter frontolimbic development, however we also demonstrate that deviation from the normative maturational trajectory is not necessarily pathological and might reflect a compensatory developmental process that is necessary for successful adaptation following trauma.

Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that delayed maturation of the affect-related circuitry leads to higher risk of adverse clinical outcomes in adults with early sexual trauma. However, the limitations of this study, such as the small sample size and cross-sectional design, preclude us from making strong claims about causal relations between sexual trauma, brain maturation, and clinical outcomes. Our data suggest that exposure to early-life sexual trauma accelerates functional maturation of affect-related networks. However, due to the cross-sectional nature of our study and our focus on emerging adults, we cannot determine whether this deviation from the normative maturational trajectory occurs shortly following trauma or later in development due to sleeper effects (e.g., Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Cropley, Zalesky, Vijayakumar, Allen and Whittle2021). In addition, because we measured brain maturity only once in emerging adulthood, we cannot assume that the observed direction or functional significance of accelerated maturation is the same at earlier stages of development. For example, risk of depression has been linked to greater global brain maturity in younger adolescents (Drobinin et al., Reference Drobinin, Van Gestel, Helmick, Schmidt, Bowen and Uher2022) and delayed brain maturation in older adolescents and emerging adults (de Nooij et al., Reference de Nooij, Harris, Hawkins, Shen, Clarke, Chan, Ziermans, McIntosh and Whalley2020). Similarly, although we show that greater functional maturity of affect-related circuitry is associated with better clinical outcomes in young women with sexual trauma, we cannot determine the temporal order or causal relation between brain maturity and outcomes. For example, disrupted neurodevelopment could be an outcome rather than an underlying mechanism of trauma-related symptoms (e.g., Muetzel et al., Reference Muetzel, Blanken, van der Ende, El Marroun, Shaw, Sudre, van der Lugt, Jaddoe, Verhulst, Tiemeier and White2018). Finally, because cross-sectional and longitudinal research on neurodevelopment can lead to different conclusions about the effects of early adversity on brain maturation (e.g., Rakesh & Whittle, Reference Rakesh and Whittle2021), it is important to replicate our results in larger, longitudinal samples.

Another promising direction will be to investigate interactions among trauma characteristics, such as trauma type, chronicity, and timing, on brain development and mental health outcomes. For example, future research with larger samples would be necessary to determine the effects of trauma timing (e.g., age of onset, duration, recency) on the relationship between frontolimbic maturation and mental health outcomes. Due to our limited sample size, we were unable to probe such interactions or control for other trauma characteristics to isolate the unique contributions of trauma type. Despite this limitation, our results underscore the importance of considering trauma characteristics as potential moderators of the relations between trauma exposure and its sequelae. In addition, because our sample included high-functioning young women on the lower end of the psychopathology spectrum, our results might have been influenced by high levels of resilience in our sample. We suggest that in a clinical sample of sexual trauma survivors with more severe symptoms and higher diagnostic rates, we would observe less affective network maturity compared to nonsexual trauma survivors.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that exposure to sexual trauma during formative years of brain development is associated with greater functional maturity of affect-related brain circuitry in emerging adulthood. Accelerated functional maturation of affective circuitry might represent an adaptation that protects survivors of early sexual trauma from adverse clinical outcomes later in life. In contrast, failure to accelerate maturation of affect-related circuitry following early sexual trauma might confer vulnerability to mental health problems in adulthood. Given the prolonged developmental trajectory of affect-related circuitry, functional maturity of affect-related circuits might hold prognostic or diagnostic promise as a potential neural marker of resilience or vulnerability to negative clinical outcomes in emerging adulthood. For example, interventions that improve clinical outcomes in young survivors of early-life sexual trauma might normalize the maturational trajectory or promote compensatory developmental processes, such as accelerated maturation, in affect-related circuits. Establishing the clinical utility of affective network maturity as a potential resilience or vulnerability marker is a promising avenue for future work.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000676

Acknowledgements

This study was conducted at The Graduate Center, The City University of New York. Data reported in this article have been previously reported in Olena Kleshchova’s doctoral dissertation.

Funding statement

This research was supported by National Institute of Health grants awarded to M.R.W.: NIDA DA012136, NIMHD MD007599, NINDS NS080686, and NINDS NS119709. The sponsors were not involved in study design, data collection, analysis, or interpretation, writing of the report, or the decision to submit the article for publication.

Competing interests

None.

References

Albaugh, M. D., Ducharme, S., Karama, S., Watts, R., Lewis, J. D., Orr, C., Nguyen, T.-V., Mckinstry, R. C., Botteron, K. N., Evans, A. C., & Hudziak, J. J. (2017). Anxious/depressed symptoms are related to microstructural maturation of white matter in typically developing youths. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 751758. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000444 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(4), 183191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Donohue, M. R., Elsayed, N. M., Gilbert, K., Harms, M. P., Hennefield, L., Herzberg, M., Kandala, S., Karcher, N. R., Jackson, J. J., Luking, K. R., Rappaport, B. I., Sanders, A., Taylor, R., Tillman, R., Vogel, A. C., Whalen, D., & Luby, J. L. (2022). Early childhood socioeconomic status and cognitive and adaptive outcomes at the transition to adulthood: The mediating role of gray matter development across five scan waves. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(1), 3444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.07.002 Google ScholarPubMed
Bath, K. G., Manzano-Nieves, G., & Goodwill, H. (2016). Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Hormones and Behavior, 82, 6471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A., Steer, R., & Brown, G. (1996). Manual for Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II). Psychology Corporation. https://doi.org/10.1037/t00742-000 Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Shalev, I. (2016). Contextual adversity, telomere erosion, pubertal development, and health: Two models of accelerated aging, or one? Development and Psychopathology, 28(4), 13671383. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579416000900 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bick, J., & Nelson, C. A. (2016). Early adverse experiences and the developing brain. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 177196. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. A., VanTieghem, M., Gabard‐Durnam, L., Gee, D. G., Flannery, J., Caldera, C., Goff, B., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Fareri, D. S., Shapiro, M., Algharazi, S., Bolger, N., Aly, M., & Tottenham, N. (2022). Age-related change in task-evoked amygdala—prefrontal circuitry: A multiverse approach with an accelerated longitudinal cohort aged 4-22 years. Human Brain Mapping, 43(10), 32213244. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25847 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brieant, A. E., Sisk, L. M., & Gee, D. G. (2021). Associations among negative life events, changes in cortico-limbic connectivity, and psychopathology in the ABCD Study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 52, 101022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busso, D. S., McLaughlin, K. A., Brueck, S., Peverill, M., Gold, A. L., & Sheridan, M. A. (2017). Child abuse, neural structure, and adolescent psychopathology: A longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(4), 321328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, B. L., Sullivan, R. M., Howell, B., & Tottenham, N. (2014). The international society for developmental psychobiology Sackler symposium: Early adversity and the maturation of emotion circuits—A cross-species analysis. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(8), 16351650. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21260 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, B. L., & Tottenham, N. (2016). The stress acceleration hypothesis: Effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 7, 7681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohodes, E. M., Kitt, E. R., Baskin-Sommers, A., & Gee, D. G. (2020). Influences of early-life stress on frontolimbic circuitry: Harnessing a dimensional approach to elucidate the effects of heterogeneity in stress exposure. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(2), 153172. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21969 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., Platt, J. M., Keyes, K. M., Sumner, J. A., Allen, N. B., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Earlier age at menarche as a transdiagnostic mechanism linking childhood trauma with multiple forms of psychopathology in adolescent girls. Psychological Medicine, 50(7), 10901098. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291719000953 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., Rosen, M. L., Williams, E. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Biological aging in childhood and adolescence following experiences of threat and deprivation: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(9), 721764. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Nooij, L., Harris, M. A., Hawkins, E. L., Shen, X., Clarke, T. K., Chan, S. W., Ziermans, T. B., McIntosh, A. M., & Whalley, H. C. (2020). Longitudinal trajectories of brain age in young individuals at familial risk of mood disorder from the Scottish Bipolar Family Study [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. Wellcome Open Research, 4, 206. https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15617.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickie, E. W., Anticevic, A., Smith, D. E., Coalson, T. S., Manogaran, M., Calarco, N., Viviano, J. D., Glasser, M. F., Van Essen, D. C., & Voineskos, A. N. (2019). Ciftify: A framework for surface-based analysis of legacy MR acquisitions. NeuroImage, 197, 818826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dosenbach, N. U., Nardos, B., Cohen, A. L., Fair, D. A., Power, J. D., Church, J. A., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Vogel, A. C., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., Barnes, K. A., Dubis, J. W., Feczko, E., Coalson, R. S., Pruett, J. R. Jr., Barch, D. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science, 329(5997), 13581361. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1194144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drobinin, V., Van Gestel, H., Helmick, C. A., Schmidt, M. H., Bowen, C. V., & Uher, R. (2022). The developmental brain age is associated with adversity, depression, and functional outcomes among adolescents. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(4), 406414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.09.004 Google ScholarPubMed
Ducharme, S., Albaugh, M. D., Hudziak, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Nguyen, T. V., Truong, C., Evans, A. C., Karama, S., Ball, W. S., Byars, A. W., Schapiro, M., Bommer, W., Carr, A., German, A., Dunn, S., Rivkin, M. J., Waber, D., Mulkern, R., Vajapeyam, S., …OʼNeill, J. (2014). Anxious/depressed symptoms are linked to right ventromedial prefrontal cortical thickness maturation in healthy children and young adults. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 29412950.10.1093/cercor/bht151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dworkin, E. R., Menon, S. V., Bystrynski, J., & Allen, N. E. (2017). Sexual assault victimization and psychopathology: A review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 6581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteban, O., Markiewicz, C. J., Blair, R. W., Moodie, C. A., Isik, A. I., Erramuzpe, A., Kent, J. D., Goncalves, M., DuPre, E., Snyder, M., Oya, H., Ghosh, S. S., Wright, J., Durnez, J., Poldrack, R. A., & Gorgolewski, K. J. (2019). fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nature Methods, 16(1), 111116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse: A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55(4), 530541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1985.tb02703.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M., Spitzer, B., R., L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002b). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP, 1/2010 revision). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002a). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Franke, K., Bublak, P., Hoyer, D., Billiet, T., Gaser, C., Witte, O. W., & Schwab, M. (2020). In vivo biomarkers of structural and functional brain development and aging in humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 142164.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, E., Hare, T., & Tottenham, N. (2014). The development of human amygdala functional connectivity at rest from 4 to 23 years: A cross-sectional study. NeuroImage, 95, 193207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., & Casey, B. J. (2015). The impact of developmental timing for stress and recovery. Neurobiology of Stress, 1, 184194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2015.02.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, E. H., Hare, T. A., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Tottenham, N. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(39), 1563815643. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1307893110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, M. J., Litz, B. T., Hsu, J. L., & Lombardo, T. W. (2004). Psychometric properties of the life events checklist. Assessment, 11(4), 330341. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191104269954 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 113123. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grydeland, H., Walhovd, K. B., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., & Fjell, A. M. (2013). Intracortical myelin links with performance variability across the human lifespan: Results from T1-and T2-weighted MRI myelin mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 1861818630. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2811-13.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E., Moore, T. M., Rosen, A. F. G., Barzilay, R., Roalf, D. R., Calkins, M. E., Ruparel, K., Scott, J. C., Almasy, L., Satterthwaite, T. D., Shinohara, R. T., & Gur, R. C. (2019). Burden of environmental adversity associated with psychopathology, maturation, and brain behavior parameters in youths. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(9), 966975. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0943 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, J. L., Hariri, A. R., & Williamson, D. E. (2015). Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 78(9), 598605. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzberg, M. P., McKenzie, K. J., Hodel, A. S., Hunt, R. H., Mueller, B. A., Gunnar, M. R., & Thomas, K. M. (2021). Accelerated maturation in functional connectivity following early life stress: Circuit specific or broadly distributed? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 48, 100922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100922 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppen, T. H., & Chalder, T. (2018). Childhood adversity as a transdiagnostic risk factor for affective disorders in adulthood: A systematic review focusing on biopsychosocial moderating and mediating variables. Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 81151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, J. L., Spronk, M., Kulkarni, K., Repovš, G., Anticevic, A., & Cole, M. W. (2019). Mapping the human brain’s cortical-subcortical functional network organization. NeuroImage, 185, 3557. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joëls, M., & Baram, T. Z. (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 459466. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2632 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U. M. A., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keding, T. J., Heyn, S. A., Russell, J. D., Zhu, X., Cisler, J., McLaughlin, K. A., & Herringa, R. J. (2021). Differential patterns of delayed emotion circuit maturation in abused girls with and without internalizing psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 178(11), 10261036. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2021.20081192 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, P. A., Viding, E., Wallace, G. L., Schaer, M., De Brito, S. A., Robustelli, B., & McCrory, E. J. (2013). Cortical thickness, surface area, and gyrification abnormalities in children exposed to maltreatment: Neural markers of vulnerability? Biological Psychiatry, 74(11), 845852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Green, J. G., Adler, L. A., Barkley, R. A., Chatterji, S., Faraone, S. V., Finkelman, M., Greenhill, L. L., Gruber, M. J., Jewell, M., Russo, L. J., Sampson, N. A., & Van Brunt, D. L. (2010). Structure and diagnosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Analysis of expanded symptom criteria from the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(11), 11681178. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.146 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebel, C., Gee, M., Camicioli, R., Wieler, M., Martin, W., & Beaulieu, C. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan. NeuroImage, 60(1), 340352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.094 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, H., Zhang, F., & Niu, X. (2019). Investigating systematic bias in brain age estimation with application to post-traumatic stress disorders. Human Brain Mapping, 40(11), 31433152.10.1002/hbm.24588CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liem, F., Varoquaux, G., Kynast, J., Beyer, F., Kharabian Masouleh, S. K., Huntenburg, J. M., Lampe, L., Rahim, M., Abraham, A., Craddock, R. C., Riedel-Heller, S., Luck, T., Loeffler, M., Schroeter, M. L., Witte, A. V., Villringer, A., & Margulies, D. S. (2017). Predicting brain-age from multimodal imaging data captures cognitive impairment. Neuroimage, 148, 179188.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manzano Nieves, G., Bravo, M., Baskoylu, S., & Bath, K. G. (2020). Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. Elife, 9, e55263. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.55263 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendle, J., Leve, L. D., Van Ryzin, M., & Natsuaki, M. N. (2014). Linking childhood maltreatment with girlsʼ internalizing symptoms: Early puberty as a tipping point. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 24(4), 689702. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendle, J., Ryan, R. M., & McKone, K. M. (2016). Early childhood maltreatment and pubertal development: Replication in a population-based sample. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(3), 595602. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12201 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molnar, B. E., Buka, S. L., & Kessler, R. C. (2001). Child sexual abuse and subsequent psychopathology: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 91(5), 753760. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.91.5.753 Google ScholarPubMed
Muetzel, R. L., Blanken, L. M., van der Ende, J., El Marroun, H., Shaw, P., Sudre, G., van der Lugt, A., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Verhulst, F. C., Tiemeier, H., & White, T. (2018). Tracking brain development and dimensional psychiatric symptoms in children: A longitudinal population-based neuroimaging study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(1), 5462. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16070813 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvacan, K., Treit, S., Camicioli, R., Martin, W., & Beaulieu, C. (2017). Evolution of deep gray matter volume across the human lifespan. Human Brain Mapping, 38(8), 37713790. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23604 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neeb, H., Zilles, K., & Shah, N. J. (2006). Fully-automated detection of cerebral water content changes: Study of age-and gender-related H2O patterns with quantitative MRI. NeuroImage, 29(3), 910922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, A. N., Greene, D. J., Gratton, C., Dosenbach, N. U., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2019). Evaluating the prediction of brain maturity from functional connectivity after motion artifact denoising. Cerebral Cortex, 29(6), 24552469. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noll, J. G., Trickett, P. K., Long, J. D., Negriff, S., Susman, E. J., Shalev, I., Li, J. C., & Putnam, F. W. (2017). Childhood sexual abuse and early timing of puberty. Journal of Adolescent Health, 60(1), 6571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nooner, K. B., Colcombe, S. J., Tobe, R. H., Mennes, M., Benedict, M. M., Moreno, A. L., Panek, L. J., Brown, S., Zavitz, S. T., Li, Q., Sikka, S., Gutman, D., Bangaru, S., Schlachter, R. T., Kamiel, S. M., Anwar, A. R., Hinz, C. M., Kaplan, M. S., Rachlin, A. B., … Milham, M. P. (2012). The NKI-Rockland sample: A model for accelerating the pace of discovery science in psychiatry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 152. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00152 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, J. E., White, K., Wu, Q., & Killian-Farrell, C. (2016). Mental health and behavioral outcomes of sexual and nonsexual child maltreatment among child welfare-involved youth. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 25(5), 483503. https://doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2016.1167801 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ono, M., Kikusui, T., Sasaki, N., Ichikawa, M., Mori, Y., & Murakami-Murofushi, K. (2008). Early weaning induces anxiety and precocious myelination in the anterior part of the basolateral amygdala of male Balb/c mice. Neuroscience, 156(4), 11031110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petanjek, Z., Judaš, M., Šimić, G., Rašin, M. R., Uylings, H. B., Rakic, P., & Kostović, I. (2011). Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), 1328113286. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105108108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platt, J. M., Colich, N. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Gary, D., & Keyes, K. M. (2017). Transdiagnostic psychiatric disorder risk associated with early age of menarche: A latent modeling approach. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 79, 7079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.06.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pynoos, R., Rodriguez, N., Steinberg, A., Stuber, M., & Frederick, C. (1998). The University of California at Los Angeles Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index (UCLA-PTSD RI). UCLA Trauma Psychiatry Program.Google Scholar
Rakesh, D., Cropley, V., Zalesky, A., Vijayakumar, N., Allen, N. B., & Whittle, S. (2021). Neighborhood disadvantage and longitudinal brain-predicted-age trajectory during adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 51, 101002. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakesh, D., & Whittle, S. (2021). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain-A systematic review of neuroimaging findings in youth. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 130, 379407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudales, A. M., Short, N. A., & Schmidt, N. B. (2019). Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between trauma type and PTSD symptoms in a diverse trauma-exposed clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 2833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smola, A. J., & Schölkopf, B. (2004). A tutorial on support vector regression. Statistics and Computing, 14(3), 199222. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:STCO.0000035301.49549.88 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Sumner, J. A., Colich, N. L., Uddin, M., Armstrong, D., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Early experiences of threat, but not deprivation, are associated with accelerated biological aging in children and adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 85(3), 268278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thijssen, S., Collins, P. F., & Luciana, M. (2022). Does pubertal stage mediate the association between family environment and structure and function of the amygdala-mPFC circuit? A replication study of the longitudinal ABCD cohort. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 56, 101120.10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tian, L., Ma, L., & Wang, L. (2016). Alterations of functional connectivities from early to middle adulthood: Clues from multivariate pattern analysis of resting-state fMRI data. NeuroImage, 129, 389400.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyborowska, A., Volman, I., Niermann, H. C., Pouwels, J. L., Smeekens, S., Cillessen, A. H., Toni, I., & Roelofs, K. (2018). Early-life and pubertal stress differentially modulate grey matter development in human adolescents. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27439-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanes, L. D., Moutoussis, M., Ziegler, G., Goodyer, I. M., Fonagy, P., Jones, P. B., Bullmore, E. T., & Dolan, R. J. (2020). White matter tract myelin maturation and its association with general psychopathology in adolescence and early adulthood. Human Brain Mapping, 41(3), 827839. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24842 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanTieghem, M. R., & Tottenham, N. (2017). Neurobiological programming of early life stress: Functional development of amygdala-prefrontal circuitry and vulnerability for stress-related psychopathology. In Vermetten, E., Baker, D., & Risbrough, V. (Eds.), Behavioral neurobiology of PTSD (pp. 117136). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2016_42 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vapnik, V. N. (1995). The nature of statistical learning theory. Springer.10.1007/978-1-4757-2440-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 10631070.10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weathers, F. W., Bovin, M. J., Lee, D. J., Sloan, D. M., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Keane, T. M., & Marx, B. P. (2018). The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5): Development and initial psychometric evaluation in military veterans. Psychological Assessment, 30(3), 383395. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000486 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Dennison, M., Vijayakumar, N., Simmons, J. G., Yücel, M., Lubman, D. I., Pantelis, C., & Allen, N. B. (2013). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology affect brain development during adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(9), 940952. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Vijayakumar, N., Dennison, M., Schwartz, O., Simmons, J. G., Sheeber, L., Allen, N. B., & Rosenfeld, C. S. (2016). Observed measures of negative parenting predict brain development during adolescence. PloS One, 11(1), e0147774. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147774 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Performance of the machine learning algorithm trained to predict age based on affective network connectivity (MAE = 3.8, RMSE = 4.6, R2 = .86, r = .93, p < .001). Linear support vector regression was trained on 500 features sampled from the cingulo-opercular and orbito-affective networks (“affective networks”) based on the strength of their correlations with age. Affective network maturity was calculated as the difference between predicted age and true age (i.e., prediction errors).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Principal component analysis on indices of mental health outcomes. Panel A: The high number of strong positive correlations among indices of mental health outcomes warranted dimensionality reduction. Panel B: The first two principal components together captured 72% of total variance. Panel C: The pattern of loadings of the original measures of mental health outcomes on the first two components after varimax rotation suggests that PC1 captures state psychological outcomes, whereas PC2 captures clinical outcomes.

Figure 2

Table 1. Participants

Figure 3

Figure 3. Trauma type and mental health outcomes. Compared to participants with a history of nonsexual trauma, participants with a history of sexual trauma showed a nonsignificant trend towards poorer state psychological outcomes indexed by PC1 scores (left panel) but no difference in clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores (right panel). Bars represent group means; error bars represent standard errors.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Trauma type, affective network maturity, and clinical outcomes. Panel A: Compared to participants with a history of nonsexual trauma, participants with a history of sexual trauma showed greater affective network maturity indexed by age prediction errors. Panel B: Trauma type moderated the relation between affective network maturity and clinical outcomes indexed by PC2 scores. Less affective network maturity was associated with poorer clinical outcomes in participants with sexual trauma but not in participants with nonsexual trauma. Bars represent group means; error bars represent standard errors. The lines of best fit were generated using robust regression.

Supplementary material: PDF

Kleshchova et al. supplementary material

Kleshchova et al. supplementary material

Download Kleshchova et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 15.5 MB